Do Grading Gray Stimuli Help to Encode Letter Position?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Word Data
3.2. Nonword Data
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix B
References
- Perea, M.; Lupker, S.J. Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter confusability effects in masked associative priming. Mem. Cogn. 2003, 31, 829–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Perea, M.; Lupker, S.J. Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions. J. Mem. Lang. 2004, 51, 231–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rimzhim, A.; Johri, A.; Kelty-Stephen, D.G.; Fowler, C.A. Transposition Effects in an Aksharic Writing System: The Case of Hindi. Lang. Speech 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McClelland, J.L.; Rumelhart, D.E. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. Psychol. Rev. 1981, 88, 375–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomez, P.; Ratcliff, R.; Perea, M. The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 115, 577–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Logan, G.D. The CODE theory of visual attention: An integration of space-based and object-based attention. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 603–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grainger, J.; van Heuven, W.J.B. Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In Mental Lexicon: Some Words to Talk about Words; Bonin, P., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers: Hauppauge, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Whitney, C. How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective litera ture review. Psychon. B Rev. 2001, 8, 221–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Grainger, J. Orthographic processing: A “mid-level” vision of reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2018, 71, 335–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perea, M.; García-Chamorro, C.; Martín-Suesta, M.; Gomez, P. Letter position coding across modalities: The case of Braille readers. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e45636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Reich, L.; Szwed, M.; Cohen, L.; Amedi, A. A ventral visual stream reading center independent of visual experience. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Massol, S.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Carreiras, M.; Grainger, J. Evidence for letter-specific position coding mechanisms. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e68460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernández-López, M.; Marcet, A.; Perea, M. Does orthographic processing emerge rapidly after learning a new script? Br. J. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marcet, A.; Perea, M.; Baciero, A.; Gomez, P. Can letter position encoding be modified by visual perceptual elements? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2019, 72, 1344–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Grainger, J.; O’Regan, J.K.; Jacobs, A.M.; Segui, J. Neighborhood frequency effects and letter visibility in visual word recognition. Percept. Psychophys. 1992, 51, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friedmann, N.; Rahamim, E. What can reduce letter migrations in letter position dyslexia? J. Res. Read. 2014, 37, 297–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wery, J.J.; Diliberto, J.A. The effect of a specialized dyslexia font, OpenDyslexic, on reading rate and accuracy. Ann. Dyslexia 2017, 67, 114–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Balota, D.A.; Yap, M.J.; Hutchison, K.A.; Cortese, M.J.; Kessler, B.; Loftis, B.; Neely, J.H.; Nelson, D.L.; Simpson, G.B.; Treiman, R. The English Lexicon Project. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 445–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Forster, K.I.; Forster, J.C. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behav. Res. Methods Instr. Comput. 2003, 35, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- JASP, version 0.13.1; JASP Team: Amsterdam, The Netherleands, 2020.
- Grainger, J.; Ziegler, J.C. A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tydgat, I.; Grainger, J. Serial position effects in the identification of letters, digits, and symbols. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. 2009, 35, 480–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Perea, M.; Baciero, A.; Marcet, A.; Fernández-López, M.; Gómez, P. Do Grading Gray Stimuli Help to Encode Letter Position? Vision 2021, 5, 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/vision5010012
Perea M, Baciero A, Marcet A, Fernández-López M, Gómez P. Do Grading Gray Stimuli Help to Encode Letter Position? Vision. 2021; 5(1):12. https://doi.org/10.3390/vision5010012
Chicago/Turabian StylePerea, Manuel, Ana Baciero, Ana Marcet, María Fernández-López, and Pablo Gómez. 2021. "Do Grading Gray Stimuli Help to Encode Letter Position?" Vision 5, no. 1: 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/vision5010012