Previous Article in Journal
Positional Differences in Youth Water Polo Players: Cognitive Functions, Specific Swimming Capacities and Anthropometric Characteristics
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Executive Function Response to Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping in Overweight Adolescents Aged 12–14: A Preliminary Study

Physical Education School, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2025, 10(2), 152; https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk10020152
Submission received: 24 March 2025 / Revised: 24 April 2025 / Accepted: 25 April 2025 / Published: 29 April 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Physical Exercise for Health Promotion)

Abstract

:
Background: Previous studies have shown that a high body mass index (BMI) is detrimental to executive function (EF) in children and elderly populations but may be improved by physical activity (PA). However, research on adolescents aged 12–14 is lacking. This study explores two parts: Part 1: cross-sectional correlation between BMI and EF; Part 2: the effect of an 8-week rope skipping intervention. Methods: Recruit 166 adolescents to participate in EF tasks. Screen and select 96 adolescents to be assigned to the normal weight control group (N-C, n = 23) and the normal weight exercise group (N-E, n = 23), the overweight control group (OV-C, n = 13), and the overweight exercise group (OV-E, n = 14), the obese control group (OB-C, n = 12), and the obese exercise group (OB-E, n = 11). Exercise program: moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping training. Control program: Self-directed reading. Data were recorded for BMI and EF tasks. Results: Part 1, EF and BMI exhibit a negative linear correlation. Part 2, the reaction time of EF tasks in N-E, OV-E and OB-E decreased about 9, 14, 13% (p < 0.05), respectively, while the average BMI in OV-E and OB-E decreased about 10% and 11% (p < 0.05), suggesting a potential correlation between the reduction in BMI and the improvement in EF, which means that the exercise intervention significantly improved EF performance. Conclusions: Among adolescents aged 12–14, overweight and obese individuals exhibit weaker EF compared with normal weight individuals. An 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping exercise program can improve EF in adolescents aged 12–14 with different BMIs, and the improvement is greater in overweight and obese individuals.

1. Introduction

Based on recent trends, it is projected that by 2035, over 750 million children aged between 5 and 19 years will be afflicted with overweight/obesity, as gauged by body mass index (BMI, kg/m2) [1]. In 2019, the overall prevalence of overweight/obesity among children and adolescents in China was 23.4%, with obesity at 9.6% [2]. Obesity is a crucial pathogenic factor for a multitude of chronic diseases, including cardiovascular disease, type II diabetes, coronary heart disease, chronic kidney disease, and site-specific cancers [3]. Obesity during childhood can endure into adulthood, predisposing individuals to increased risk of non-communicable diseases in adulthood [4]. Executive function (EF) consists of higher-order cognitive abilities that enable conscious control of thoughts, actions, and emotions for goal-directed behavior and adaptive functioning [5]. Increasing evidence suggests that obesity in adults or children may negatively affect brain health, specifically brain development [6] and EF [7], mainly reflected in the reduction in inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility [8]. It is hypothesized that such a reduction could be attributed to alterations in the brain’s microstructure, such as white matter hyperintensity (WMH), brain atrophy, and cerebral microbleed [9].
Regular physical activity (PA) has received significant attention in recent years due to its accessible, flexible, cost-effective, scalable, and well-documented health benefits [10], which encompass enhancements in brain tissue volume, preservation of white matter microstructural integrity, mitigation of WMH, prevention of cerebral small vessel disease, and improvement in cardiometabolic health [11,12,13]. Functionally, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) plays a part in distractor-resistant maintenance, while updating is thought to be supported by the striatum, and the neurotransmitter dopamine plays a crucial role in balancing these complementary processes [14]. Notably, exercise increases the release of dopamine [15], which, in turn, is intricately linked to the enhancement of the EF [16], as the previously discussed physiological factors are positively correlated with PA. This suggests that exercise may enhance and preserve the functioning of these mechanisms, thereby improving EF. Compared to the overweight/obese population, the relative weakness of these mechanisms provides a critical target for achieving more pronounced intervention effects.
Aerobic exercise targets cardiorespiratory fitness and mitochondrial biogenesis [17]. Interval training is characterized by brief bursts of intense effort interspersed by short periods (intervals) of recovery [18]. Both high-intensity cardiovascular exercise and chronic exercise interventions might be a promising way to promote multiple aspects of EF in children and adolescents [19,20]. Frequent participation in tennis play is related to better EF in children and adolescents [21]. One study found that coordination training significantly improved children’s attention than training focused on endurance, strength, and cardiovascular health [22]. Rope skipping is a cost-effective strategy that increases cardiovascular health and reduces cardiometabolic risk factors and BMI in young populations [23]. Liu et al. (2024) found that acute rope skipping can improve cognitive function in children [24]. Additionally, a comprehensive meta-analysis concluded that both high- and moderate-intensity exercise regimens effectively improve EF [20]. However, there is a lack of research on the use of moderate-to-high-intensity exercise for interval training with overweight/obese adolescents aged between 12 and 14 as participants to assess the effect on EF. The tripartite model of EF, which includes distinct components such as inhibitory, working memory, and cognitive flexibility, typically emerges during early adolescence, around the ages of 13 to 14 years [25].
This study conducted two evaluations. Part one: We aimed to identify the association between BMI and EF in adolescents. We hypothesized that there would be a negative correlation between higher BMI and poorer cognitive performance in adolescents. Part two: We aimed to identify and investigate the effect of an 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping training program and whether it improved EF in the overweight/obese adolescents’ group. It was hypothesized that EF would be improved by rope skipping exercise intervention in overweight/obese adolescents.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

All participants/parents provided informed consent prior to enrolment. The study was conducted at a single secondary school in Shenzhen, China.
The sample size was determined by power analysis using G-Power software (version 3.1) [26]. The first study utilized values of α = 0.05, power = 0.80, effect size = 0.25, with three groups (normal weight, overweight, and obese) and two measurements (BMI and EF) in a priori type of power analysis [27]. The minimum sample size required was n = 82. The second study utilized values of α = 0.05, power = 0.80, effect size = 0.25, with six groups (normal weight control and exercise groups, overweight control and exercise groups, obese control and exercise groups) and two measurements (BMI and EF) in a priori type of power analysis [28]. The minimum sample size required was n = 66. Each group requires at least 22 participants.
A total of 166 subjects aged between 12 and 14 years participated in the correlational study, out of which 96 agreed to continue their involvement in the exercise intervention program. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) Adolescents 12–14 years old whose BMI was within normal weight guidelines, or those who were overweight or obese as defined by age and gender [29]; (b) without cardiovascular, neurological, psychiatric disease, or serious injuries (such as external/internal traumatic injuries) in the last six months; (c) no hormones, weight loss, or psychotropic drugs had been consumed in the last six months. All participants signed informed consent forms. The baseline characteristics of the participants are summarized in Table 1.

2.2. Study Intervention

Exercise intervention: an 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping training (3 times per week). Each session included a general warm-up (dynamic exercises), a moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping training intervention, and a cool-down period (static stretching exercises) for a total of 50 min (Table 2). The rope skipping training consisted of fast jumping with feet together. The sedentary control groups maintained their resting heart rate during the training time without any prescribed PA. Control intervention: Sit quietly and read, ensuring that your heart rate does not exceed 100 beats per minute. Specific movement demonstrations are provided in Supplementary Video S1.
During the intervention, telemetric heart rate measurement was recorded using a fitness smart band (SHB-02, Shape, Shenzhen, China).

2.3. Study Outcomes

EF encompasses three domains: inhibitory control (cognitive inhibitory and behavioral inhibitory), working memory, and cognitive flexibility, which were assessed by the Flanker task, Stop-Signal Task (SST) [30], 2-Back task [31] and More-odd Shifting (MOS) task [32], respectively. Participants completed the tasks on E-prime 3.0 software. Body weight (kg) was measured with a smart body fat scale (HUAWEI, model: HAG-B19, Shenzhen, China). Height (m) was measured with a stadiometer (Shuang sheng, Danyang, China). BMI was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters.

2.3.1. Flanker Task

A modified version of the Eriksen flanker test was used to assess cognitive inhibitory [33]. The task consists of congruent and incongruent parts. The incongruent situation (for example: →→←→→/←←→←←); the congruent situation (for example: ←←←←←/→→→→→). For the congruent situation, the participant must press “F” on the keyboard; otherwise, they must press “J.” Participants were required to respond within 1500 milliseconds (ms); if not, it was recorded as no response. There were 96 tests in the formal experiment, and the congruent and incongruent stimuli were presented 48 times each (Figure 1A). The difference in times to complete the congruent and incongruent tasks and the average of both accuracies were used to reflect the cognitive inhibitory ability.

2.3.2. SST

A modified version of the SST was used to assess behavioral inhibitory [30]. Participants were required to press the corresponding “Q” key on the keyboard when a black circle arrow pointing to the left appeared. If a right-pointing arrow appeared, participants were required to press the “P” key. When a red circle arrow pointing to the left or right appeared on the screen, participants were not required to respond. The stimulus was presented for 500 ms—exceeding this range was recorded as no response. In total, there were 150 tests in the formal experiment. The reaction time and accuracy of the SST were used to reflect the behavioral inhibitory ability (Figure 1B).

2.3.3. 2-Back Task

A modified version of the 2-Back task was used to assess working memory [31]. In this task, a number appeared in the center of the screen. Participants were required to start from the third number and assess whether it was the same as the first, and then they were to move to the fourth number and assess whether it was the same as the second, and so on. Participants pressed the “F” key for consistency and the “J” key for inconsistency. The stimulus was presented for 3000 ms, and beyond this range was recorded as no response. There was a total of 48 tests in the formal experiment (Figure 1C). The reaction time and accuracy of the 2-Back task were used to assess working memory.

2.3.4. MOS Task

A modified version of the MOS task was used to assess cognitive flexibility [34]. The task consisted of unshifting (judging large/small or odd/even separately) and shifting parts (judging conversion of large/small and odd/even). Some numbers (1–9) appeared in the MOS task screen, but there was no 5. Large/small judgments (black numbers): for numbers ˂5, participants had to press “F”; for numbers ˃5, they were required to press “J.” Odd/even judgments (green numbers): for odd numbers, participants had to press “F”; for even numbers, participants had to press “J.” Conversion of large/small and odd/even judgments: if the presented number was black, participants had to make a large/small judgment; if the presented number was green, they had to make an odd/even judgment. The stimulus presentation had no time limit, and the subsequent test was performed after the participant answered. The formal experiments of the three parts consisted of 48 tests (Figure 1D). The accuracy and the differences in times to complete unshifting and shifting tasks were used to reflect cognitive flexibility.

2.4. Quality Monitoring

We utilized the intelligent sports classroom exercise load guidance system fitness trackers (Figure S1) to monitor the average heart rate during each session, ensuring the effective operation of the experimental data. Adherence to the exercise intervention was measured by the average heart rate during the moderate-to-high jump rope training sessions out of the 24 scheduled sessions [35]. Compliance with the intervention was assessed by calculating each participant’s average heart rate during the moderate-to-high jump rope training sessions and comparing it to the target heart rate. Data were deemed invalid if the participant’s average heart rate fell below the target heart rate. In the context of dietary intervention, health and nutrition intake workshops were conducted for the participants, and corresponding healthy recipes were provided to their parents.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Data are shown as mean ± SD. SPSS (version 27.3) and GraphPad Prism software (version 10.3) were used for data analysis. Part 1: The Shapiro–Wilk test was used to verify normal data distribution. Spearman’s correlation was used to determine correlations between BMI and EF. Correlations where p < 0.05 were selected to construct the linear graphs. One-way ANOVA (Analysis of Variance) was used to examine the differences in EF tasks among the normal weight group, the overweight group, and the obese group. Part 2: Two-way ANOVA with post hoc analysis. For BMI x Intervention, analyze the differences between groups after the intervention. The Bonferroni correction was used for pairwise comparisons. Differences were considered significant when p <0.05.

3. Results

Part one: Two hundred students are within the scope of recruitment. Twenty-three students with a low BMI and a thinner and leaner stature did not meet the criteria and were excluded. Eleven students refused to participate in the experiment due to personal time constraints. This resulted in one hundred and sixty-six participants being retained. Part two: Ninety-six participants were involved in the experiment. Seven students lost contact during the intervention; six students discontinued the intervention due to being busy. This resulted in eighty-three participants being retained (Figure 2).

3.1. Correlation Between BMI and EF in Adolescents

There was a significant linear positive correlation between BMI and the RT of the Flanker task (r = 0.6614, p < 0.0001, Figure 3A), SST (r = 0.5711, p < 0.0001, Figure 3C), 2-Back task (r = 0.5038, p < 0.0001, Figure 3E), MOS task (r = 0.5283, p < 0.0001, Figure 3G), the accuracy of the Flanker task (r = 0.1714, p = 0.0272, Figure 3B), 2-Back task (r = −0.3347, p < 0.0001, Figure 3F), and MOS task (r = −0.2680, p = 0.0005, Figure 3H). This result indicates that a higher BMI leads to a longer response time in EF tasks; therefore, participants with a higher BMI exhibited lower accuracy in the 2-Back and MOS tasks. Higher BMI was significantly associated with longer reaction times across all EF tasks (Flanker, SST, 2-Back, MOS; all r > 0.50, p < 0.0001) and reduced accuracy in working memory and cognitive flexibility tasks (2-Back/MOS; r = −0.27 to −0.33, p ≤ 0.0005), indicating higher BMI is linked to slower thinking speed and lower accuracy.

3.2. Descriptive Statistics and Comparative Analysis of Pre-Exercise Intervention

Since no significant within-group gender differences were observed (all p > 0.05), data were aggregated across genders within each experimental group for subsequent analyses. As shown in Table 3, there were significant differences in EF among different BMI groups (p < 0.0001). According to the reaction time data from the EF tasks, higher BMI levels were associated with relatively longer reaction times. Additionally, some tasks exhibit a relatively low accuracy rate.

3.3. Effects of Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping on BMI in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Adolescents

As shown in Figure 4A, based on BMI groupings, we categorized the intervention methods into N-C, N-E, OV-C, OV-E, OB-C, and OB-E. Significant differences were observed between OV-C and OV-E (p < 0.0001), OB-C and OB-E (p < 0.0001). The BMIs of the OV-E and the OB-E groups were nearly 9% and 11%, respectively, a reduction from pre-exercise intervention.

3.4. Effects of Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping on EF in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Adolescents

Before exercise intervention, there were no significant differences between N-C and N-E, OV-C and OV-E, OB-C, and OB-E in EF tasks. After 8 weeks of exercise, we will compare the performance of EF tasks following the post-exercise intervention.

3.4.1. Effects of Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping on Cognitive and Behavioral Inhibitory in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Adolescents

As shown in Table 4, ANOVA revealed significant main effects for BMI groups and intervention methods (exercise vs. control) on reaction times in the Flanker task (p < 0.0001, p < 0.0001) as shown in Figure 4B,C. A significant interaction was also observed between BMI groups and intervention methods (p = 0.019). Post hoc analyses with Bonferroni corrections demonstrated that the exercise group showed significantly greater improvement in cognitive inhibitory compared to the control group across normal weight, overweight, and obese groups (p = 0.004, p < 0.0001, p < 0.0001), was sequentially shortened close to 25, 35, 36% after the intervention. However, no significant main effects were observed in accuracy rates, indicating that, in the absence of significant differences in accuracy, the exercise group exhibited shorter reaction times compared to the control group. This suggests that exercise intervention has a beneficial impact on cognitive inhibitory control among adolescents of normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals.
As is shown in Table 4, Figure 4D,G, the ANOVA revealed significant main effects for BMI groups and intervention methods on reaction times in the SST (p < 0.0001, p < 0.0001), and a significant interaction between BMI groups and intervention methods (p = 0.049). Post hoc analyses with Bonferroni corrections demonstrated that the exercise group showed significantly greater improvement compared to the control group across the overweight and obese groups (p = 0.002, p = 0.001), and was sequentially shortened by about 7% and 8% after the intervention. Although no significance was observed in N-E, it still showed a 5% reduction compared to pre-intervention levels. Regarding accuracy, a significant main effect was observed for the intervention, but no significant interaction effects were found. This result shows that exercise has a more significant effect on improving behavioral inhibitory control in overweight and obese children compared to the normal-weight adolescents.

3.4.2. Effects of Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping on Working Memory in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Adolescents

As presented in Table 4, Figure 4E,H, the ANOVA results indicated significant effects on both BMI groups and intervention methods regarding reaction times in the 2-Back task (p < 0.0001, p = 0.003). However, no significant interaction was observed between BMI groups and intervention methods. When we conducted post hoc analyses with intervention methods as the main effect, significant differences were found only in the OV-E (p = 0.027), which was shortened by close to 9%, while the OB-E approached a significance of (p = 0.075) and was shortened by close to 10%. In terms of accuracy, differences were observed in the analysis with BMI groups as the main effect, but no interaction effects were present. Post hoc analyses also revealed no significant differences within each group. This finding indicates that exercise exerts a more pronounced impact on enhancing working memory abilities in overweight and obese children than in the normal-weight adolescents.

3.4.3. Effects of Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping on Cognitive Flexibility in Normal Weight and Overweight/Obese Adolescents

The results of the ANOVA show that the RT of the MOS has a significant main effect for both BMI groups and intervention methods (p < 0.0001, p < 0.0001), but no significant interaction effects between BMI groups and intervention methods (Table 4, Figure 4F,I). Post hoc analyses conducted separately for each main effect revealed that the exercise group showed significantly greater improvement in EF compared to the control group within the normal weight, overweight, and obese groups (p = 0.016, p < 0.0001, p = 0.012), was shortened nearly 19, 24, 21% after the intervention, respectively. In terms of accuracy, significance was only observed in the ANOVA with exercise as the main effect. Post hoc analyses revealed that the exercise group showed significantly greater improvement in EF compared to the control group within the normal weight, overweight, and obese groups (p = 0.003, p = 0.007, p = 0.011). Based on the results, we can conclude that exercise can significantly improve cognitive flexibility in adolescents of normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals. However, the extent of improvement is not significantly correlated with BMI levels.

4. Discussion

As it was hypothesized in this study, the first evaluation (Part 1) explored the association between BMI and EF, revealing that higher BMI is linked to lower EF. Additionally, the second evaluation (Part 2) investigated the effects of an 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping program on EF. The results showed that rope skipping effectively improved inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility in overweight and obese adolescents, with similar improvements observed in normal-weight children. Notably, the 8-week program also significantly enhanced working memory in adolescents aged 12–14, particularly in the overweight group.
Inhibitory control includes cognitive and behavioral aspects: cognitive inhibitory suppresses distractions to focus on tasks, while behavioral inhibitory restrains impulsive actions [5,36]. Working memory has limited capacity and involves interactions between cognitive processing, sensorimotor mechanisms, and long-term memory activation [37,38]. Cognitive flexibility relates to the ability to shift between tasks or mental sets [39].
In part one, we analyzed the correlation between EF performance—measured by reaction time and accuracy—and BMI in a sample of 166 participants. The results revealed no significant correlation between BMI and accuracy in the Flanker task (cognitive inhibitory control) or the SST (behavioral inhibitory control). However, reaction times were positively correlated with BMI, indicating that individuals with higher BMI exhibited slower response times. The lack of association between BMI and accuracy suggests that task performance, in terms of correctness, remained unaffected, while variations in reaction time directly reflected differences in inhibitory control. For the 2-Back (working memory) and MOS (cognitive flexibility) tasks, significant BMI-related correlations were observed. Specifically, higher BMI was associated with lower accuracy and longer reaction times, suggesting that individuals with higher BMI demonstrated weaker working memory and reduced cognitive flexibility. These findings are consistent with prior systematic reviews, wherein eight of nine cross-sectional studies reported significantly impaired cognitive performance in obese children/adolescents relative to normal-weight controls [40].It was also observed in older populations, where obesity has been linked to memory impairments in elderly individuals [41]. Mechanistic studies suggest that obesity is associated with structural and functional brain alterations, including cortical thinning, reduced brain volume, and abnormalities in key regions such as WMH, gray matter, the hippocampus, the hypothalamus, and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [42,43,44,45,46]—all of which play critical roles in EF development and regulation. Furthermore, obesity-induced systemic inflammation may contribute to neuroinflammation [47], potentially accelerating cognitive decline.
An eight-week high-intensity rope skipping study examined its effects on the Flanker task (cognitive inhibitory control) and SST (behavioral inhibitory control) in exercise and control groups. While no significant differences in accuracy were observed among overweight, obese, and normal-weight groups (p > 0.05), reaction times improved significantly following the intervention. In the Flanker task, the exercise group exhibited faster reaction times across all weight categories: overweight (p < 0.0001), obese (p < 0.0001), and normal-weight groups (p = 0.004). For the SST, significant improvements were observed exclusively in the overweight (p = 0.002) and obese groups (p = 0.001), with reduced task completion times. These findings suggest that high-intensity exercise enhances both cognitive and behavioral inhibitory control efficiency without affecting accuracy, particularly benefiting overweight and obese adolescents. Improvements in cognitive inhibitory control, as measured by the Flanker task, were consistent across all weight groups, underscoring the universal cognitive benefits of exercise. However, the overweight and obese groups, starting from lower baseline performance levels, demonstrated greater relative improvements (35% in overweight, 36% in obese, and 25% in normal-weight participants), highlighting the amplified benefits of exercise for these populations. In contrast, enhancements in behavioral inhibitory control were weight-specific, with significant improvements observed only in overweight and obese participants. Previous electroencephalography (EEG) studies have reported that exercise reduced current source density in the mid-frontal gyrus, indicating enhanced neural efficiency and supporting improved inhibitory control performance [48]. This neural mechanism provides a physiological basis for the observed cognitive benefits. Additionally, the significant BMI reductions in overweight and obese participants post-intervention may further contribute to the observed improvements in inhibitory control performance.
An eight-week high-intensity rope-skipping intervention assessed the effects of exercise on working memory using the 2-Back task in exercise and control groups. While no significant differences in task accuracy were observed among overweight, obese, and normal-weight participants (p > 0.05), reaction times improved significantly in the OV-E (p < 0.05) and showed a trend toward improvement in the OB-E (p = 0.075), indicating enhanced task efficiency. These findings suggest that exercise improves working memory performance by increasing processing speed without compromising accuracy. No significant improvement was observed in the N-E (p = 0.206), indicating that the cognitive benefits of exercise may be group-specific, primarily favoring overweight and obese individuals who started with lower baseline performance. This aligns with previous studies by de Bruijn et al., Meijer et al., and Wassenaar et al., which reported positive but non-significant effects of exercise on working memory in normal-weight adolescents [49,50,51]. The limited improvement in this group may be attributed to their higher baseline working memory capacity, resulting in smaller marginal gains, or to the possibility that working memory is less sensitive to short-term exercise interventions and may require longer training durations to elicit measurable effects.
An eight-week high-intensity rope-skipping intervention was conducted to evaluate its effects on cognitive flexibility using the MOS task. Compared to the control group, the exercise group demonstrated significant improvements in accuracy across all weight categories: overweight (p = 0.007), obese (p = 0.025), and normal weight (p = 0.003), indicating that exercise effectively enhances cognitive flexibility accuracy. Additionally, reaction times improved significantly in all groups (p = 0.001, p = 0.012, p = 0.016), reflecting faster task completion. The consistent improvements observed across all weight groups highlight the universal cognitive benefits of exercise. These findings align with previous studies, such as those by Ortega et al., which demonstrated that exercise enhances cognitive function and brain health in overweight and obese children by improving brain structure (e.g., increased gray matter volume) and neural connectivity [52]. Similarly, Costigan SA et al. found that HIIT positively impacted cognitive function and mental health in adolescents [53]. However, Wassenaar TM et al. reported that while a one-year high-intensity exercise intervention improved fitness and some cognitive functions in adolescents, cognitive flexibility did not significantly improve [51]. These differences may arise from varied cognitive tasks and exercise intervention features, such as mode, frequency, duration, intensity, and length.
This study provides compelling evidence supporting the cognitive benefits of moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping, with particularly pronounced effects observed in overweight and obese adolescents. The research makes significant contributions to exercise science, cognitive psychology, and pediatric health through its rigorous methodology, comprehensive assessment of EF, and practical intervention design.
However, several limitations should be noted. The relatively small sample size in the exercise intervention may affect the generalizability of the findings. Furthermore, the study did not account for potentially influential covariates, including dietary patterns, habits, sleep patterns, pubertal status, and baseline physical activity levels—may have influenced the observed results. For instance, variations in macronutrient intake or sleep duration could independently modulate cognitive performance [54,55]. Although participants were instructed to maintain their usual lifestyles during the trial, these factors were not systematically measured. The absence of long-term follow-up assessments represents another limitation, as it precludes evaluation of the intervention’s sustained effects. Additionally, the reliance on BMI rather than more precise measures like body fat percentage for classifying overweight/obesity status may impact the validity of the findings. Most importantly, the study did not investigate the underlying physiological and psychological mechanisms responsible for the observed cognitive improvements, which limits our understanding of how exercise influences executive function at a mechanistic level.
These limitations point to critical future research needs: larger samples, rigorous confounder control (via 24-h dietary recalls, actigraphy, and accelerometry), longer follow-ups, precise body composition analysis (e.g., DEXA), and mechanistic studies to unravel how rope skipping augments executive function in this population.

5. Conclusions

This study highlights that EF is weaker in overweight and obese adolescents compared to their normal-weight peers. However, an 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping intervention effectively improved EF in 12–14-year-old adolescents with overweight/obesity, with concurrent BMI reductions observed in the overweight/obese groups. Future research should focus on designing personalized exercise programs tailored to age and health status to optimize cognitive benefits. Furthermore, investigations should elucidate BMI’s potential mediating role in exercise-induced EF improvements. Beyond phenomenological studies, mechanistic research should explore underlying pathways, including neuroplasticity, gut microbiota, and inflammatory responses. Longitudinal follow-up studies are also imperative to evaluate the sustained cognitive effects of exercise interventions and identify determinants of long-term efficacy.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jfmk10020152/s1, Figure S1: The intelligent sports classroom exercise load guidance system fitness trackers; Video S1: Exercise protocol action demonstration video.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.Z. and Q.Y.; methodology, Q.Y.; software, Q.Y.; validation, X.W.; formal analysis, L.Z.; investigation, L.Z.; resources, L.Z.; data curation, L.Z. and Q.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, Q.Y.; writing—review and editing, L.Z.; supervision, L.Z.; project administration, L.Z.; funding acquisition, L.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [grant number 32200942]; Science, and the Technology and Innovation Commission of Shenzhen Municipality [grant number 20220811090705001].

Institutional Review Board Statement

All procedures were approved by the Shenzhen University General Hospital Ethics Committee (PN-202500001).

Informed Consent Statement

All participants underwent anthropometric analysis and assessment of their EF performance.

Data Availability Statement

The data and materials this study is based on are available from the corresponding author, L.Z.

Acknowledgments

We extend our sincere appreciation to all the participants in this experimental research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
EFExecutive function
BMIBody mass index
PAPhysical activity
N-CNormal weight control group
N-ENormal weight exercise group
OV-COverweight control group
OV-EOverweight exercise group
OB-CObese control group
OB-EObese exercise group
WMHWhite matter hyperintensity
PFCPrefrontal cortex
HRmaxMaximum heart rate
RSRope skipping
SSTStop-Signal Task
MOSMore-odd Shifting
RTReaction time
ACCAccuracy
msMillisecond
DLPFCLeft dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
MICTModerate-intensity interventions
HIITHigh-intensity interval training
EEGElectroencephalography

References

  1. Federation, W.O. World Obesity Atlas 2024. 2024. Available online: https://www.worldobesityday.org (accessed on 4 March 2024).
  2. Dong, Y.H.; Chen, L.; Liu, J.Y.; Ma, T.; Zhang, Y.; Chen, M.M.; Zhong, P.L.; Shi, D.; Hu, P.J.; Li, J.; et al. Epidemiology and prediction of overweight and obesity among children and adolescents aged 7–18 years in China from 1985 to 2019. Chin. J. Prev. Med. 2023, 57, 11–19. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Blüher, M. Obesity: Global epidemiology and pathogenesis. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2019, 15, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Volger, S.; Rigassio Radler, D.; Rothpletz-Puglia, P. Early childhood obesity prevention efforts through a life course health development perspective: A scoping review. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0209787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Sun, J.; Zeng, N.; Hui, Y.; Li, J.; Liu, W.; Zhao, X.; Zhao, P.; Chen, S.; Wu, S.; Wang, Z.; et al. Association of variability in body size with neuroimaging metrics of brain health: A population-based cohort study. Lancet Reg. Health West. Pac. 2024, 44, 101015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Blair, C.; Kuzawa, C.W.; Willoughby, M.T. The development of executive function in early childhood is inversely related to change in body mass index: Evidence for an energetic tradeoff? Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Fernandes, A.C.; Viegas, Â.A.; Lacerda, A.C.R.; Nobre, J.N.P.; Morais, R.L.S.; Figueiredo, P.H.S.; Costa, H.S.; Camargos, A.C.R.; Ferreira, F.O.; de Freitas, P.M.; et al. Association between executive functions and gross motor skills in overweight/obese and eutrophic preschoolers: Cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 2022, 22, 498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hamer, M.; Batty, G.D. Association of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio with brain structure: UK Biobank study. Neurology 2019, 92, e594–e600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lundh, H.; Arvidsson, D.; Greven, C.; Fridolfsson, J.; Börjesson, M.; Boman, C.; Lauruschkus, K.; Lundqvist, S.; Melin, K.; Bernhardsson, S. Physical activity and sedentary behaviour amongst children with obesity—Exploring cross-sectional associations between child and parent. J. Act. Sedentary Sleep Behav. 2025, 4, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chaddock, L.; Erickson, K.I.; Prakash, R.S.; Kim, J.S.; Voss, M.W.; Vanpatter, M.; Pontifex, M.B.; Raine, L.B.; Konkel, A.; Hillman, C.H.; et al. A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Res. 2010, 1358, 172–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Chaddock-Heyman, L.; Erickson, K.I.; Kienzler, C.; Drollette, E.S.; Raine, L.B.; Kao, S.C.; Bensken, J.; Weisshappel, R.; Castelli, D.M.; Hillman, C.H.; et al. Physical Activity Increases White Matter Microstructure in Children. Front. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Piercy, K.L.; Troiano, R.P.; Ballard, R.M.; Carlson, S.A.; Fulton, J.E.; Galuska, D.A.; George, S.M.; Olson, R.D. The Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. JAMA 2018, 320, 2020–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. D’Ardenne, K.; Eshel, N.; Luka, J.; Lenartowicz, A.; Nystrom, L.E.; Cohen, J.D. Role of prefrontal cortex and the midbrain dopamine system in working memory updating. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 19900–19909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Humińska-Lisowska, K. Dopamine in Sports: A Narrative Review on the Genetic and Epigenetic Factors Shaing Personality and Athletic Performance. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2024, 25, 11602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Söderqvist, S.; Bergman Nutley, S.; Peyrard-Janvid, M.; Matsson, H.; Humphreys, K.; Kere, J.; Klingberg, T. Dopamine, working memory, and training induced plasticity: Implications for developmental research. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 836–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Coffey, V.G.; Hawley, J.A. The molecular bases of training adaptation. Sports Med. 2007, 37, 737–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hawley, J.A.; Gibala, M.J. What’s new since Hippocrates? Preventing type 2 diabetes by physical exercise and diet. Diabetologia 2012, 55, 535–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Xue, Y.; Yang, Y.; Huang, T. Effects of chronic exercise interventions on executive function among children and adolescents: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2019, 53, 1397–1404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Moreau, D.; Chou, E. The Acute Effect of High-Intensity Exercise on Executive Function: A Meta-Analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 14, 734–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ishihara, T.; Sugasawa, S.; Matsuda, Y.; Mizuno, M. Relationship of tennis play to executive function in children and adolescents. Eur. J. Sport. Sci. 2017, 17, 1074–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gallotta, M.C.; Emerenziani, G.P.; Iazzoni, S.; Meucci, M.; Baldari, C.; Guidetti, L. Impacts of coordinative training on normal weight and overweight/obese children’s attentional performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wu, M.; Wang, X.T.; Xu, S.H.; Tang, Z.X.; Li, H.L.; Jing, J.J.; Tang, W.K.; Chen, X.Y.; Lan, R.L.; Zhu, Y.N. Effects of caloric restriction and rope-skipping exercise on cardiometabolic risk factors in overweight or obese college students. Chin. J. Prev. Med. 2022, 56, 1272–1278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Liu, Y.; Dong, X.; He, Q.; Jia, Y. Effects of acute rope skipping exercises of different exercise modes on cognitive function in 9-10-year-old children. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 29172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Theodoraki, T.E.; McGeown, S.P.; Rhodes, S.M.; MacPherson, S.E. Developmental changes in executive functions during adolescence: A study of inhibition, shifting, and working memory. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 38, 74–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wang, X.J.; Luo, W.F.; Wang, L.J.; Mao, C.J.; Wang, L.; Liu, C.F. Study on uric acid and the related factors associated with cognition in the patients with Parkinson’s disease. Chin. J. Prev. Med. 2009, 89, 1633–1635. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  28. Ziereis, S.; Jansen, P. Effects of physical activity on executive function and motor performance in children with ADHD. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 38, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cole, T.J.; Bellizzi, M.C.; Flegal, K.M.; Dietz, W.H. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: International survey. BMJ 2000, 320, 1240–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Pliszka, S.R.; Borcherding, S.H.; Spratley, K.; Leon, S.; Irick, S. Measuring inhibitory control in children. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 1997, 18, 254–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Owen, A.M.; McMillan, K.M.; Laird, A.R.; Bullmore, E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 46–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Salthouse, T.A.; Fristoe, N.; McGuthry, K.E.; Hambrick, D.Z. Relation of task switching to speed, age, and fluid intelligence. Psychol. Aging 1998, 13, 445–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Eriksen, B.A.; Eriksen, C.W. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hillman, C.H.; Kramer, A.F.; Belopolsky, A.V.; Smith, D.P. A cross-sectional examination of age and physical activity on performance and event-related brain potentials in a task switching paradigm. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2006, 59, 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Liang, X.; Qiu, H.; Wang, P.; Sit, C.H.P. The impacts of a combined exercise on executive function in children with ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2022, 32, 1297–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2004, 133, 101–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Baddeley, A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. D’Esposito, M.; Postle, B.R. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 115–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dajani, D.R.; Uddin, L.Q. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2015, 38, 571–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Smith, E.; Hay, P.; Campbell, L.; Trollor, J.N. A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: Implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obes. Rev. 2011, 12, 740–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Arnoriaga-Rodríguez, M.; Mayneris-Perxachs, J.; Burokas, A.; Contreras-Rodríguez, O.; Blasco, G.; Coll, C.; Biarnés, C.; Miranda-Olivos, R.; Latorre, J.; Moreno-Navarrete, J.M.; et al. Obesity Impairs Short-Term and Working Memory through Gut Microbial Metabolism of Aromatic Amino Acids. Cell Metab. 2020, 32, 548–560.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Yau, P.L.; Kang, E.H.; Javier, D.C.; Convit, A. Preliminary evidence of cognitive and brain abnormalities in uncomplicated adolescent obesity. Obesity 2014, 22, 1865–1871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Ribeiro, M.; Yordanova, Y.N.; Noblet, V.; Herbet, G.; Ricard, D. White matter tracts and executive functions: A review of causal and correlation evidence. Brain 2024, 147, 352–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Brooks, S.J.; Benedict, C.; Burgos, J.; Kempton, M.J.; Kullberg, J.; Nordenskjöld, R.; Kilander, L.; Nylander, R.; Larsson, E.M.; Johansson, L.; et al. Late-life obesity is associated with smaller global and regional gray matter volumes: A voxel-based morphometric study. Int. J. Obes. 2013, 37, 230–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Winkler, Z.; Kuti, D.; Polyák, Á.; Juhász, B.; Gulyás, K.; Lénárt, N.; Dénes, Á.; Ferenczi, S.; Kovács, K.J. Hypoglycemia-activated Hypothalamic Microglia Impairs Glucose Counterregulatory Responses. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 6224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gluck, M.E.; Viswanath, P.; Stinson, E.J. Obesity, Appetite, and the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2017, 6, 380–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Olsthoorn, L.; Vreeken, D.; Kiliaan, A.J. Gut Microbiome, Inflammation, and Cerebrovascular Function: Link Between Obesity and Cognition. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 761456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Solis-Urra, P.; Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Cadenas-Sanchez, C.; Hillman, C.H.; Kramer, A.F.; Catena, A.; Ortega, F.B. The effects of an exercise intervention on neuroelectric activity and executive function in children with overweight/obesity: The ActiveBrains randomized controlled trial. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2024, 34, e14486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Meijer, A.; Königs, M.; van der Fels, I.M.J.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Hartman, E.; Oosterlaan, J. The Effects of Aerobic Versus Cognitively Demanding Exercise Interventions on Executive Functioning in School-Aged Children: A Cluster-Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 43, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. de Bruijn, A.G.M.; van der Fels, I.M.J.; Renken, R.J.; Königs, M.; Meijer, A.; Oosterlaan, J.; Kostons, D.D.N.M.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Smith, J.; et al. Differential effects of long-term aerobic versus cognitively-engaging physical activity on children’s visuospatial working memory related brain activation: A cluster RCT. Brain Cogn. 2021, 155, 105812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wassenaar, T.M.; Wheatley, C.M.; Beale, N.; Nichols, T.v.; Salvan, P.; Meaney, A.; Atherton, K.; Diaz-Ordaz, K.; Dawes, H.; Johansen-Berg, H. The effect of a one-year vigorous physical activity intervention on fitness, cognitive performance and mental health in young adolescents: The Fit to Study cluster randomised controlled trial. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2021, 18, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ortega, F.B.; Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Cadenas-Sanchez, C.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Migueles, J.H.; Solis-Urra, P.; Verdejo-Román, J.; Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Molina-Garcia, P.; Ruiz, J.R.; et al. Effects of an Exercise Program on Brain Health Outcomes for Children With Overweight or Obesity: The ActiveBrains Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Netw. Open 2022, 5, e2227893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Costigan, S.A.; Eather, N.; Plotnikoff, R.C.; Hillman, C.H.; Lubans, D.R. High-Intensity Interval Training for Cognitive and Mental Health in Adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2016, 48, 1985–1993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bleiweiss-Sande, R.; Chui, K.; Wright, C.; Amin, S.; Anzman-Frasca, S.; Sacheck, J.M. Associations between Food Group Intake, Cognition, and Academic Achievement in Elementary Schoolchildren. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Lucassen, E.A.; Piaggi, P.; Dsurney, J.; de Jonge, L.; Zhao, X.C.; Mattingly, M.S.; Ramer, A.; Gershengorn, J.; Csako, G.; Cizza, G.; et al. Sleep extension improves neurocognitive functions in chronically sleep-deprived obese individuals. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e84832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the executive functional tasks. (A) Flow diagram of the Flanker task. (B) Flow diagram of the SST. (C) Flow diagram of the 2-Back task. (D) Flow diagram of the MOS task. Jfmk 10 00152 i001/Jfmk 10 00152 i002, fixation point; →→←→→/←←→←←, incongruent situation; ←←←←←/→→→→→, congruent situation; Jfmk 10 00152 i003, right-pointing arrow; Jfmk 10 00152 i004, left-pointing arrow; Jfmk 10 00152 i005, red circle arrow pointing to the left; Jfmk 10 00152 i006, red circle arrow pointing to the right; 4/5/1/3/9/3/2, numbers.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the executive functional tasks. (A) Flow diagram of the Flanker task. (B) Flow diagram of the SST. (C) Flow diagram of the 2-Back task. (D) Flow diagram of the MOS task. Jfmk 10 00152 i001/Jfmk 10 00152 i002, fixation point; →→←→→/←←→←←, incongruent situation; ←←←←←/→→→→→, congruent situation; Jfmk 10 00152 i003, right-pointing arrow; Jfmk 10 00152 i004, left-pointing arrow; Jfmk 10 00152 i005, red circle arrow pointing to the left; Jfmk 10 00152 i006, red circle arrow pointing to the right; 4/5/1/3/9/3/2, numbers.
Jfmk 10 00152 g001
Figure 2. Participant selection flowchart of the study.
Figure 2. Participant selection flowchart of the study.
Jfmk 10 00152 g002
Figure 3. Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and EF. Correlation findings are shown as Spearman’s correlation coefficient (p-value); strong correlations with |r| ≥ 0.8, moderate correlations with 0.5 ≤ |r| < 0.8, weak correlations with 0.3 ≤ |r| < 0.5, and no correlations with |r| < 0.3. p < 0.05 are shown as a significant correlation. (A) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and Flanker task reaction (ms). (B) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and Flanker task accuracy (%). (C) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and SST reaction (ms). (D) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and SST accuracy (%). (E) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and 2-Back reaction (ms). (F) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and 2-Back accuracy (%). (G) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and MOS reaction (ms). (H) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and MOS accuracy (%). RT, reaction time; ACC, accuracy; and BMI, body mass index.
Figure 3. Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and EF. Correlation findings are shown as Spearman’s correlation coefficient (p-value); strong correlations with |r| ≥ 0.8, moderate correlations with 0.5 ≤ |r| < 0.8, weak correlations with 0.3 ≤ |r| < 0.5, and no correlations with |r| < 0.3. p < 0.05 are shown as a significant correlation. (A) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and Flanker task reaction (ms). (B) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and Flanker task accuracy (%). (C) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and SST reaction (ms). (D) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and SST accuracy (%). (E) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and 2-Back reaction (ms). (F) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and 2-Back accuracy (%). (G) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and MOS reaction (ms). (H) Linear plot of the correlation between BMI and MOS accuracy (%). RT, reaction time; ACC, accuracy; and BMI, body mass index.
Jfmk 10 00152 g003
Figure 4. Comparison of EF after exercise intervention. Comparisons of the following parameters after exercise intervention: (A) BMI, (B) Flanker task reaction (ms), (C) Flanker task accuracy (%), (D) SST reaction (ms), (E) 2-Back reaction (ms), (F) MOS task reaction (ms), (G) SST accuracy (%), (H) 2-Back accuracy (%), and (I) MOS task accuracy (%). N-C, Normal weight control group; N-E, normal weight exercise group; OV-C, overweight control group; OV-E, overweight exercise group; OB-C, obese control group; OB-E, obese exercise group; RT, reaction time; ACC, accuracy; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, and **** p < 0. 0001.
Figure 4. Comparison of EF after exercise intervention. Comparisons of the following parameters after exercise intervention: (A) BMI, (B) Flanker task reaction (ms), (C) Flanker task accuracy (%), (D) SST reaction (ms), (E) 2-Back reaction (ms), (F) MOS task reaction (ms), (G) SST accuracy (%), (H) 2-Back accuracy (%), and (I) MOS task accuracy (%). N-C, Normal weight control group; N-E, normal weight exercise group; OV-C, overweight control group; OV-E, overweight exercise group; OB-C, obese control group; OB-E, obese exercise group; RT, reaction time; ACC, accuracy; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, and **** p < 0. 0001.
Jfmk 10 00152 g004
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the participants.
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the participants.
StudyGroupsNumberAgeBody Mass Index
(Female/Male)(Years Old)(kg/m2)
Part one: The correlation between body mass index and EFNormal weight group96 (50/46)13.11 ± 0.2819.10 ± 1.52
Overweight group47 (23/24)13.17 ± 0.2123.43 ± 1.06
Obese group23 (9/14)13.23 ± 0.2827.10 ± 1.49
Part two: The effect of an 8-week moderate-to-high-intensity rope skipping training in adolescentsNormal weight control group (N-C)23 (12/11)13.08 ± 0.3118.56 ± 1.38
Normal weight exercise group (N-E)23 (12/11)13.15 ± 0.2818.66 ± 1.37
Overweight control group (OV-C)13 (7/6)13.12 ± 0.1524.19 ± 0.88
Overweight exercise (OV-E)14 (6/8)13.16 ± 0.2824.15 ± 0.66
Obese control group (OB-C)12 (6/6)13.24 ± 0.3027.04 ± 1.05
Obese exercise group (OB-E)11 (5/6)13.19 ± 0.2726.72 ± 1.05
Note. All values are presented as means ± standard deviation (SD). EF, executive function; N-C: normal weight control group; N-E: normal weight exercise group; OV-C: overweight control group; OV-E: overweight exercise group; OB-C: obese control group; OB-E: obese exercise group.
Table 2. Exercise protocol.
Table 2. Exercise protocol.
ProcessWarm-UpRS TrainingRelaxation
Time10 min30 min10 min
Content(1) Jogging
(2) High knees
(3) Butt kicks
(4) Bounding
(5) Grapevines
(6) Slow skipping
(7) Lateral bounding
(8) Hamstring extension
Content of each set
(1) 30 s RS (60–80 jumps) 30 s rest
(2) 60 s RS (120–160 jumps) 60 s rest
(3) 90 s RS (180–240 jumps) 90 s rest
Repeat 5 sets
(1) Thigh stretch
(2) Calf stretch
(3) Side stretch
(4) Lunge stretch
Exercise intensity50% HRmax–59% HRmax
(105–124 beats/minute)
60% HRmax–79% HRmax
(125–165 beats/minute)
<50% HRmax
(<105 beats/minute)
Note. HRmax: Maximum heart rate; RS: rope skipping. The movements will be uploaded as video attachments.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and variance analysis of pre-test data.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and variance analysis of pre-test data.
GroupsNG (n = 42)OVG (n = 22)OBG (n = 19)Fp-Value
Flanker RT (ms)19.24 ± 5.3226.64 ± 4.5434.49 ± 4.6696.617<0.0001 ****
SST RT (ms)345.06 ± 22.48366.80 ± 19.50386.70 ± 24.0639.376<0.0001 ****
2-Back RT (ms)567.99 ± 50.00624.27 ± 56.41670.64 ± 51.0243.479<0.0001 ****
MOS RT (ms)335.93 ± 100.05428.37 ± 67.73475.67 ± 91.0530.387<0.0001 ****
Flanker ACC (%)95 ± 296 ± 296 ± 22.3750.096
SST ACC (%)90 ± 491 ± 391 ± 42.0310.135
2-Back ACC (%)85 ± 681 ± 681 ± 77.9060.001 ***
MOS ACC (%)88 ± 486 ± 485 ± 43.5810.030 *
Note. NG, normal weight group; OVG, overweight group; OBG, obese group; * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics and variance analysis of post-test data.
Table 4. Descriptive statistics and variance analysis of post-test data.
GroupsN-C
(n = 21)
N-E
(n = 21)
BonferroniOV-C
(n = 10)
OV-E
(n = 12)
BonferroniOB-C
(n = 10)
OB-E
(n = 9)
BonferroniANOVA
BMI GroupingIntervention WayMain
(BMI × Intervention)
95% CIp95% CIp95% CIpFpηP2FpηP2FpηP2
Flanker RT
(ms)
18.83 ± 5.6214.30 ± 4.681.461, 7.5950.004 **25.56 ± 6.0416.92 ± 4.204.379, 12.8890.0001 ****34.51 ± 4.4322.21 ± 4.267.735, 16.8670.0001 ****36.8120.0001 ****0.48953.1590.0001 ****0.4084.1890.019 *0.098
SST RT
(ms)
339.84 ± 22.87331.95 ± 15.37−4.567, 20.3580.211371.45 ± 29.80342.87 ± 15.6011.294, 45.8760.002 **385.36 ± 13.14353.54 ± 23.0213.261, 50.3700.001 ***20.1660.0001 ****0.34423.1600.0001 ****0.2313.1280.049 *0.075
2-Back RT
(ms)
566.87 ± 55.49542.74 ± 65.06−13.523, 61.7840.206622.42 ± 45.50563.45 ± 82.996.726, 111.2100.027 *658.72 ± 61.91607.93 ± 41.73−5.268, 106.8520.07511.1250.0001 ****0.2249.7510.003 **0.1120.6860.5070.081
MOS RT
(ms)
341.96 ± 69.68285.99 ± 86.3610.631, 101.3060.016 *426.89 ± 85.24320.69 ± 55.0143.294, 169.1010.001 ***459.10 ± 69.75372.29 ± 61.5719.311, 154.3120.012 *13.5840.0001 ****0.26123.2570.0001 ****0.2320.8950.4130.023
Flanker ACC
(%)
96 ± 196 ± 1−0.8, 1.20.65996 ± 296 ± 1−1.0, 1.70.59696 ± 196 ± 1−1.6, 1.30.8691.0950.3400.0280.1520.6970.0020.1200.8870.003
SST ACC
(%)
91 ± 493 ± 2−3.8, 0.80.18990 ± 492 ± 2−6.0, 0.40.08290 ±392 ±4−6.0, 0.90.1400.5400.5850.0146.9800.010 *0.0830.2610.7710.007
2-Back ACC
(%)
85 ± 686 ± 5−5.4, 2.30.43281 ± 482 ± 8−6.9, 3.80.56082 ± 483 ± 7−6.9, 4.60.6913.2210.045*0.0770.9370.3360.0120.0070.9930.000
MOS ACC
(%)
83 ± 390 ± 3−5.7, −1.20.003 **86 ± 490 ± 2−7.5, −1.20.007 **85 ± 389 ± 3−7.5, −50.025 *0.4150.6610.01120.7030.0001 ****0.2120.1030.9030.003
Note. All values are presented as means ± standard deviation (SD). N-C, normal weight control group; N-E, normal weight exercise group; OV-C, overweight control group; OV-E, overweight exercise group; OB-C, obese control group; OB-E, obese exercise group; RT, reaction time; ACC, accuracy; ms, millisecond; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yu, Q.; Wang, X.; Zhang, L. Executive Function Response to Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping in Overweight Adolescents Aged 12–14: A Preliminary Study. J. Funct. Morphol. Kinesiol. 2025, 10, 152. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk10020152

AMA Style

Yu Q, Wang X, Zhang L. Executive Function Response to Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping in Overweight Adolescents Aged 12–14: A Preliminary Study. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology. 2025; 10(2):152. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk10020152

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yu, Qian, Xiaodong Wang, and Lin Zhang. 2025. "Executive Function Response to Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping in Overweight Adolescents Aged 12–14: A Preliminary Study" Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology 10, no. 2: 152. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk10020152

APA Style

Yu, Q., Wang, X., & Zhang, L. (2025). Executive Function Response to Moderate-to-High-Intensity Rope Skipping in Overweight Adolescents Aged 12–14: A Preliminary Study. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology, 10(2), 152. https://doi.org/10.3390/jfmk10020152

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop