A Phenomenological Take on Mirror Self-Face Perception in Schizophrenia
Abstract
:Introduction
Discussions
“When I look myself at the mirror, I have the feeling that I do not exist […], as if the mirror makes me forget who I am... my image at the mirror is always the same, it doesn’t change... then I try to close my eyes but when I open them, I have the same feeling”(p. 319)
“There is an absolute fixity around me. I have even less mobility for the future than I have for the present and the past. There is a kind of routine in me which does not allow me to envisage the future. The creative power in me is abolished. I see the future as a repetition of the past”(p. 277)
- the primal impression, which is a mode of appearance of the intentional object that cannot provide us with any temporal information about it, as it is constituted by every single ’now’ in which a portion of the object is given to the senses;
- retention, namely a particular kind of non-representational, “primary memory that continuously attaches itself to the [primal] impression” (p. 32) and that, adding to the actual ’now’ of every perception, allows us to experience the intentional object as a phenomenon that extends across a time span;
- protention, which is the intuition that something is about to happen in the very next phase of the perceptual process; it is an anticipation based upon the combination of the retentional sense of the just-past moments and that manifests itself as the expectation we have towards the future modes of appearance of the intentional object.
“while watching TV […], though I can see every scene, I don’t understand the plot. Every scene jumps to the next, there is no connection. The course of time is strange, too. Time splits up and doesn’t run forward anymore”(p. 194)
“the frequency of strange-faces was higher in patients than in controls: hugely deformed features (100% vs. 71%), archetypal faces (50% vs. 19%), and monstrous faces (88% vs. 29%). Patients’ archetypical and monster faces were usually satanic beings. Conversely, […] controls tended to perceive apparitions of another person in the mirror instead of themselves more often than patients (patients 13% vs. controls 48%)”(Caputo et al. 2012, p. 49) [9]
Conclusions
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of Interest disclosure
References
- Baudouin, J.Y.; Martin, F.; Tiberghien, G.; Verlut, I.; Franck, N. Selective attention to facial emotion and identity in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia. 2002, 40, 503–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, F.; Baudouin, J.Y.; Tiberghien, G.; Franck, N. Processing emotional expression and facial identity in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2005, 134, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Akbarfahimi, M.; Tehrani-Doost, M.; Ghassemi, F. Emotional face perception in patients with schizophrenia: An event-related potential study. Neurophysiology 2013, 45, 249–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bortolon, C.; Capdevielle, D.; Raffard, S. Face recognition in schizophrenia disorder: A comprehensive review of behavioral, neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015, 53, 79–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Irani, F.; Platek, S.M.; Panyavin, I.S.; et al. Self-face recognition and theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and first-degree relatives. Schizophr Res. 2006, 88, 151–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kircher, T.T.; Seiferth, N.Y.; Plewnia, C.; et al. Self-face recognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007, 94, 264–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heinisch, C.; Wiens, S.; Gründl, M.; Juckel, G.; Brüne, M. Self-face recognition in schizophrenia is related to insight. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013, 263, 655–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bortolon, C.; Capdevielle, D.; Salesse, R.N.; Raffard, S. Self-Face Recognition in Schizophrenia: An Eye-Tracking Study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016, 10, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caputo, G.B.; Ferrucci, R.; Bortolomasi, M.; et al. Visual perception during mirror gazing at one’s own face in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012, 140, 46–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fonseca-Pedrero, E.; Badoud, D.; Antico, L.; et al. Strange-face-in-the-mirror illusion and schizotypy during adolescence. Schizophr Bull. 2015, 41 (Suppl 2), S475–S482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bortolon, C.; Capdevielle, D.; Altman, R.; Macgregor, A.; Attal, J.; Raffard, S. Mirror self-face perception in individuals with schizophrenia: Feelings of strangeness associated with one’s own image. Psychiatry Res. 2017, 253, 205–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poletti, M.; Raballo, A. Uncanny Mirroring: A Developmental Perspective on the Neurocognitive Origins of Self-Disorders in Schizophrenia. Psychopathology. 2019, 52, 316–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Courage, M.L.; Edison, S.C.; Howe, M.L. Variability in the early development of visual self-recognition. Infant Behavior and Development 2004, 27, 509–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Butler, D.L.; Mattingley, J.B.; Cunnington, R.; Suddendorf, T. Mirror, mirror on the wall, how does my brain recognize my image at all? PLoS ONE. 2012, 7, e31452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lacan, J. 1949 Le stade du miroir comme formateur de la fonction du Je [The mirror stage as formative of the function of the I as revealed in psychoanalytical experience]. In Écrits; Seuil: Paris, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Gallop, J. Lacan’s” mirror stage”: Where to begin. SubStance 1982, 11, 118–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rochat, P. Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. Conscious Cogn. 2003, 12, 717–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lobaccaro, L.; Bacaro, M. What is in the Mirror? On Mirror Self-Recognition, Semiotics, and Material Engagement. In Reti, saperi, linguaggi, Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences 2021, 8, 103–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sperry, R.W. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1950, 43, 482–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holst, E.V.; Mittelstaedt, H. The principle of reafference: Interactions between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. Naturwissenschaften 1950, 37, 464–476. [Google Scholar]
- Crapse, T.B.; Sommer, M.A. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008, 9, 587–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thakkar, K.N.; Schall, J.D.; Heckers, S.; Park, S. Disrupted Saccadic Corollary Discharge in Schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2015, 35, 9935–9945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stark, L.; Bridgeman, B. Role of corollary discharge in space constancy. Percept Psychophys. 1983, 34, 371–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wurtz, R.H. Corollary Discharge Contributions to Perceptual Continuity Across Saccades. Annu Rev Vis Sci. 2018, 4, 215–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feinberg, I.; Guazzelli, M. Schizophrenia--a disorder of the corollary discharge systems that integrate the motor systems of thought with the sensory systems of consciousness. Br J Psychiatry. 1999, 174, 196–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ford, J.M.; Mathalon, D.H.; Heinks, T.; Kalba, S.; et al. Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001, 158, 2069–2071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ford, J.M.; Gray, M.; Faustman, W.O.; Roach, B.J.; Mathalon, D.H. Dissecting corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology. 2007, 44, 522–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ford, J.M.; Mathalon, D.H. Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: Can it explain auditory hallucinations? Int J Psychophysiol. 2005, 58, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heinks-Maldonado, T.H.; Mathalon, D.H.; Houde, J.F.; Gray, M.; Faustman, W.O.; Ford, J.M. Relationship of imprecise corollary discharge in schizophrenia to auditory hallucinations. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007, 64, 286–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ford, J.M.; Mathalon, D.H. Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking. J Psychiatr Res. 2004, 38, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salomon, R.; Progin, P.; Griffa, A.; et al. Sensorimotor Induction of Auditory Misattribution in Early Psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2020, 46, 947–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giersch, A.; Lalanne, L.; Isope, P. Implicit Timing as the Missing Link between Neurobiological and Self Disorders in Schizophrenia? Front Hum Neurosci. 2016, 10, 303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Minkowski, E. Lived time: Phenomenological and psychopathological studies; Northwestern University Press: Evanston, IL, USA, 1970; ISBN -10. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs, T. The temporal structure of intentionality and its disturbance in schizophrenia. Psychopathology. 2007, 40, 229–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pennisi, G.; Gallagher, S. Embodied and disembodied rationality: What morbid rationalism and hyper-reflexivity tell us about human intelligence and intentionality. In Psychopathology and the Mind: What mental disorders can tell us about our minds; Cardella, V., Gangemi, A., Eds.; Routledge: eBook, 2021; pp. 263–286. ISBN 9781003009856. [Google Scholar]
- Husserl, E. 1966 Zur Phänomenologie des inneren Zeitbewußtseins (1893–1917). In Trans. J. Brough; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Colombetti, G. Psychopathology and the enactive mind. In The Oxford handbook of philosophy and psychiatry; Fulford, K.W.M., Davies, M., Gipps, R.G.T., Graham, G., Sadler, J.Z., Stanghellini, G., Thornton, T., Eds.; Oxford University Press, 2013; pp. 1083–1102. [Google Scholar]
- Sass, L.; Pienkos, E.; Skodlar, B.; Stanghellini, G.; Fuchs, T.; et al. EAWE: Examination of Anomalous World Experience. Psychopathology. 2017, 50, 10–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kimura, B. Psychopathologie der Zufaelligkeit oder Verlust des Aufenthaltsortes beim Schizophrenen. Daseinsanalyse 1994, 11, 192–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caputo, G.B. Apparitional experiences of new faces and dissociation of self-identity during mirror gazing. Percept Mot Skills. 2010, 110 3 Pt 2, 1125–1138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sass, L.A. Madness and modernism: Insanity in the light of modern art, literature, and thought; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1992; ISBN -10. [Google Scholar]
- Sass, L.A. Schizophrenia, self-experience, and the so-called negative symptoms. In Exploring the self: Philosophical and psychopathological perspectives on self-experience; Zahavi, D., Ed.; Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 149–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sass, L.A. Self and world in schizophrenia: Three classic approaches. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology 2001, 8, 251–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parnas, J. The self and intentionality in the pre-psychotic stages of schizophrenia: A phenomenological study. In Exploring the self: Philosophical and psychopathological perspectives on self-experience; Zahavi, D., Ed.; John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Urfer-Parnas, A. Eugéne Minkowski. In Oxford Handbook of Phenomenological Psychopatology; Stanghellini, G., Broome, M.R., Fernandez, A.V., Fusar-Poli, P., Raballo, A., Rosfort, R., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Blankenburg, W. Der Verlust der natürlichen selbstverständlichkeit: Ein Beitrag zur Psycho-pathologie symptomarmer Schizophrenien; Enke: Stuttgart, Germany, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Parnas, J.; Bovet, P.; Zahavi, D. Schizophrenic autism: Clinical phenomenology and pathogenetic implications. World Psychiatry. 2002, 1, 131–136. [Google Scholar]
Level 0: Confusion | The individual is unaware of the mirror, confusing the specular images with the surrounding environment and interacting with people’s reflections as if they were real persons |
Level 1: Differentiation | Infants start to sense a difference between the mirror and the surrounding environment, perceiving that there is a correspondence between their movements and those reflected by the surface |
Level 2: Situation | Infants engage in systematic exploration of the connection between their movements and the reflection in the mirror, developing a quasi-narcissistic contemplative stance towards the specular image |
Level 3: Identification | Children recognize their reflection as themselves, explicitly referring to themselves while exploring their specular image. This level includes passing the mirror-mark test |
Level 4: Permanence | The self is identified beyond the immediate mirror experience, and it is recognized as existing not only in relation to the contingency of one’s own reflection |
Level 5: Self-consciousness | The individual recognizes him/herself as a subject from both a first-person and third-person perspective, becoming aware of how he/she is perceived by others |
© 2022 by the author. 2022 Giovanni Pennisi
Share and Cite
Pennisi, G. A Phenomenological Take on Mirror Self-Face Perception in Schizophrenia. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2023, 10, 1-7. https://doi.org/10.22543/2392-7674.1389
Pennisi G. A Phenomenological Take on Mirror Self-Face Perception in Schizophrenia. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences. 2023; 10(1):1-7. https://doi.org/10.22543/2392-7674.1389
Chicago/Turabian StylePennisi, Giovanni. 2023. "A Phenomenological Take on Mirror Self-Face Perception in Schizophrenia" Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences 10, no. 1: 1-7. https://doi.org/10.22543/2392-7674.1389
APA StylePennisi, G. (2023). A Phenomenological Take on Mirror Self-Face Perception in Schizophrenia. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences, 10(1), 1-7. https://doi.org/10.22543/2392-7674.1389