Pilot Study on Gaze Characteristics of Older Drivers While Watching Driving Movies
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Recruitment of Participants
2.2. Creation of Driving Videos
2.3. Selection of Hazard Areas
2.4. Assessment Items
2.4.1. Basic Attribute Questionnaire
2.4.2. Cognitive and Attentional Function Tests
2.4.3. Eye Gaze Measurement during Driving Video Viewing
- (a)
- Coordinates of the Gaze Point (X, Y): The upper left corner of the monitor is the origin (0,0), and the lower right corner is (1920,1080). In Figure 1b, the horizontal direction is indicated by the X-axis, and the vertical direction by the Y-axis. The average, maximum, and minimum values for each scene of the driving video were calculated. After each value for a total of 10 scenes was added and averaged, the values were converted from values representing coordinates (pixel) to percentages (%). Normally, hazards appear at various locations and times, and drivers must pay attention to road conditions comprehensively. By performing an additive average, we obtained general gazing coordinates that are not limited to specific road situations. The maximum value of the X coordinate indicates the rightmost, and the minimum value indicates the leftmost, representing the horizontal search range; the maximum value of the Y-coordinate indicates the bottommost and the minimum value indicates the topmost, representing the vertical search range. All coordinates for cases in which the subject was gazing at something other than the driving video were excluded.
- (b)
- Total Number of Fixations: This is the number of gazing counts in the driving video (25 s) for each scene. A higher number of gazes indicates more eye movement.
- (c)
- Percentage of Participants who Gazed at AOI: The percentage of drivers who gazed at the AOI was calculated. The value is involved in the hazard detection rate.
- (d)
- Total Duration of Fixation in AOI: The total time spent gazing at the AOI was calculated. The duration of fixation was normalized at each AOI interval time because it depends on the time the hazard is represented [37]. For example, if the cumulative gazing time was 1.5 s in a 5.0 s AOI interval time, the normalized cumulative gazing time would be 30.0%.
- (e)
- Time to First Fixation in AOI: This is the time it took to gaze at the AOI for the first time and it involves the speed of reaction to a hazard.
- (f)
- Number of Visits in AOI: It is the number of revisits after leaving the AOI. The number of revisits was normalized by dividing by the number of gazing times calculated for “(b) Total Number of Fixations” [70]. For example, if the number of gazing times in “(b) Total Number of Fixations” is 50 and the number of visits to the AOI is 5, the normalized number of visits is 10.0%.
2.5. Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Participants
3.2. Basic Attributes
3.3. Cognitive and Attentional Functions
3.4. Eye Movement Data
3.4.1. Coordinates of the Gaze Point (X, Y)
3.4.2. Total Number of Fixations
3.4.3. Percentage of Participants Who Gazed at AOI
3.4.4. Total Duration of Fixation at AOI
3.4.5. Time to First Fixation in AOI
3.4.6. Number of Visits to AOI
3.4.7. Summary of Parameters for AOI
4. Discussion
4.1. Gazing Characteristics in Driving
4.2. Gaze Characteristics for Hazard Areas
4.3. Traffic Situation of Each AOI
4.4. Limitations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- e-Stat: Statistics on Road Traffic/Occurrence Situations of Traffic Crashes. Available online: https://www.e-stat.go.jp/stat-search/files?stat_infid=000040025755 (accessed on 30 January 2024).
- Chihuri, S.; Mielenz, T.J.; DiMaggio, C.J.; Betz, M.E.; DiGuiseppi, C.; Jones, V.C.; Li, G. Driving cessation and health outcomes in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2016, 64, 332–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Edwards, J.D.; Perkins, M.; Ross, L.A.; Reynolds, S.L. Driving status and three-year mortality among community-dwelling older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2009, 64, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Windsor, T.D.; Anstey, K.J.; Butterworth, P.; Luszcz, M.A.; Andrews, G.R. The role of perceived control in explaining depressive symptoms associated with driving cessation in a longitudinal study. Gerontologist 2007, 47, 215–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freeman, E.E.; Gange, S.J.; Muñoz, B.; West, S.K. Driving status and risk of entry into long-term care in older adults. Am. J. Public Health 2006, 96, 1254–1259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marottoli, R.A.; Mendes de Leon, C.F.; Glass, T.A.; Williams, C.S.; Cooney, L.M.; Berkman, L.F. Consequences of driving cessation: Decreased out-of-home activity levels. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2000, 55, S334–S340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marottoli, R.A.; Mendes de Leon, C.F.; Glass, T.A.; Williams, C.S.; Cooney, L.M., Jr.; Berkman, L.F.; Tinetti, M.E. Driving cessation and increased depressive symptoms: Prospective evidence from the New Haven EPESE. Established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1997, 45, 202–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siren, A.; Haustein, S. Driving licences and medical screening in old age: Review of literature and European licensing policies. J. Transp. Health 2015, 2, 68–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Flaherty, C.A. Transport Planning and Traffic Engineering; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Yaacob, N.F.F.; Rusli, N.; Bohari, S.N. A review analysis of accident factor on road accident cases using Haddon matrix approach. In Proceedings of the Second International Conference on the Future of ASEAN (ICoFA), 2017; Springer: Singapore, 2018; Volume 2, pp. 55–65. [Google Scholar]
- Habibzadeh, Y.; Yarmohammadian, M.H.; Sadeghi-Bazargani, H. Driving hazard perception components: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Bull. Emerg. Trauma 2023, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oron-Gilad, T.; Parmet, Y. Can a driving simulator assess the effectiveness of Hazard Perception training in young novice drivers? Adv. Transp. Stud. 2014, 1, 65. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson, L.; Chapman, P.; Crundall, D. What happens next? Predicting other road users’ behaviour as a function of driving experience and processing time. Ergonomics 2009, 52, 154–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horswill, M.S.; McKenna, F.P. Drivers’ hazard perception ability: Situation awareness on the road. A cognitive approach to situation awareness. Theor. Appl. 2004, 1, 155–175. [Google Scholar]
- Watzke, J.; Smith, D.B.D. Concern for and knowledge of safety hazards among older people: Implications for research and prevention. Exp. Aging Res. 1994, 20, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Quimby, A.; Watts, G.R. Human Factors and Driving Performance (Laboratory Report 1004); Transport and Road Research Laboratory: Crowthorne, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Horswill, M.S.; Anstey, K.J.; Hatherly, C.G.; Wood, J.M. The crash involvement of older drivers is associated with their hazard perception latencies. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2010, 16, 939–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scott-Parker, B.; De Regt, T.; Jones, C.; Caldwell, J. The situation awareness of young drivers, middle-aged drivers, and older drivers: Same but different? Case Stud. Transp. Pol. 2020, 8, 206–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.C.; Cian, J.Y. Effects of situation awareness under different road environments on young and elder drivers. J. Ind. Prod. Eng. 2014, 31, 253–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Habibzadeh Omran, Y.H.; Sadeghi-Bazargani, H.; Yarmohammadian, M.H.; Atighechian, G. Driving hazard perception tests: A systematic review. Bull. Emerg. Trauma 2023, 11, 51–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aksan, N.; Hacker, S.D.; Sager, L.; Dawson, J.; Anderson, S.; Rizzo, M. Correspondence between simulator and on-road drive performance: Implications for assessment of driving safety. Geriatrics 2016, 1, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, H.C.; Cameron, D.; Lee, A.H. Assessing the driving performance of older adult drivers: On-road versus simulated driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2003, 35, 797–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Savage, S.W.; Zhang, L.; Swan, G.; Bowers, A.R. The effects of age on the contributions of head and eye movements to scanning behavior at intersections. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2020, 73, 128–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Classen, S.; Bewernitz, M.; Shechtman, O. Driving simulator sickness: An evidence-based review of the literature. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2011, 65, 179–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borowsky, A.; Shinar, D.; Oron-Gilad, T. Age, skill, and hazard perception in driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2010, 42, 1240–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horswill, M.S.; Marrington, S.A.; McCullough, C.M.; Wood, J.; Pachana, N.A.; McWilliam, J.; Raikos, M.K. The hazard perception ability of older drivers. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2008, 63, P212–P218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McKenna, F.P.; Horswill, M.S.; Alexander, J.L. Does anticipation training affect drivers’ risk taking? J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2006, 12, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kunishige, M.; Fukuda, H.; Iida, T.; Kawabata, N.; Ishizuki, C.; Miyaguchi, H. Spatial navigation ability and gaze switching in older drivers: A driving simulator study. Hong Kong J. Occup. Ther. 2019, 32, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bao, S.; Boyle, L.N. Age-related differences in visual scanning at median-divided highway intersections in rural areas. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2009, 41, 146–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Underwood, G.; Phelps, N.; Wright, C.; Van Loon, E.; Galpin, A. Eye fixation scanpaths of younger and older drivers in a hazard perception task. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2005, 25, 346–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hakamies-Blomqvist, L.; Raitanen, T.; O’Neill, D. Driver ageing does not cause higher accident rates per km. Transp. Res. F 2002, 5, 271–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, D.L.; Pollatsek, A.P.; Pradhan, A. Can novice drivers be trained to scan for information that will reduce their likelihood of a crash? Inj. Prev. 2006, 12 (Suppl. S1), i25–i29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pradhan, A.K.; Hammel, K.R.; DeRamus, R.; Pollatsek, A.; Noyce, D.A.; Fisher, D.L. Using eye movements to evaluate effects of driver age on risk perception in a driving simulator. Hum. Factors 2005, 47, 840–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pradhan, A.; Pollatsek, A.; Fisher, D.L. Comparison of trained and untrained novice drivers’ gaze behaviour in risky and non-risky scenarios. In Proceedings of the Fourth International Driving Symposium in Human Factors and Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, Stevenson, WA, USA, 9–12 July 2007; pp. 328–334. [Google Scholar]
- Chapman, P.R.; Underwood, G. Visual search of driving situations: Danger and experience. Perception 1998, 27, 951–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, A.H.; Chapman, P.; Crundall, D. Producing a commentary slows concurrent hazard perception responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2014, 20, 285–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borowsky, A.; Oron-Gilad, T.; Meir, A.; Parmet, Y. Drivers’ perception of vulnerable road users: A hazard perception approach. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012, 44, 160–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chapman, P.; Underwood, G.; Roberts, K. Visual search patterns in trained and untrained novice drivers. Transp. Res. F 2002, 5, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ābele, L.; Haustein, S.; Martinussen, L.M.; Møller, M. Improving drivers’ hazard perception in pedestrian-related situations based on a short simulator-based intervention. Transp. Res. F 2019, 62, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carr, D.B.; Grover, P. The role of eye tracking technology in assessing older driver safety. Geriatrics 2020, 5, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marandi, R.Z.; Gazerani, P. Aging and eye tracking: In the quest for objective biomarkers. Future Neurol. 2019, 14, FNL33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Depestele, S.; Ross, V.; Verstraelen, S.; Brijs, K.; Brijs, T.; van Dun, K.; Meesen, R. The impact of cognitive functioning on driving performance of older persons in comparison to younger age groups: A systematic review. Transp. Res. F 2020, 73, 433–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A. Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychol. Aging 2019, 34, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cuenen, A.; Jongen, E.M.M.; Brijs, T.; Brijs, K.; Lutin, M.; Van Vlierden, K.; Wets, G. The relations between specific measures of simulated driving ability and functional ability: New insights for assessment and training programs of older drivers. Transp. Res. F 2016, 39, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cuenen, A.; Jongen, E.M.M.; Brijs, T.; Brijs, K.; Lutin, M.; Van Vlierden, K.; Wets, G. Does attention capacity moderate the effect of driver distraction in older drivers? Accid. Anal. Prev. 2015, 77, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anstey, K.J.; Wood, J. Chronological age and age-related cognitive deficits are associated with an increase in multiple types of driving errors in late life. Neuropsychology 2011, 25, 613–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wagner, J.T.; Müri, R.M.; Nef, T.; Mosimann, U.P. Cognition and driving in older persons. Swiss Med. Wkly. 2011, 140, w13136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kirova, A.-M.; Bays, R.B.; Lagalwar, S. Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. BioMed Res. Int. 2015, 2015, 748212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harada, C.N.; Natelson Love, M.C.; Triebel, K.L. Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2013, 29, 737–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salthouse, T.A. Selective review of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2010, 16, 754–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fjell, A.M.; Sneve, M.H.; Grydeland, H.; Storsve, A.B.; Walhovd, K.B. The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cereb. Cortex 2017, 27, 2303–2317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sigurdsson, S.; Aspelund, T.; Forsberg, L.; Fredriksson, J.; Kjartansson, O.; Oskarsdottir, B.; Jonsson, P.V.; Eiriksdottir, G.; Harris, T.B.; Zijdenbos, A.; et al. Brain tissue volumes in the general population of the elderly: The AGES-Reykjavik study. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 3862–3870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ward, N.S. Compensatory mechanisms in the aging motor system. Ageing Res. Rev. 2006, 5, 239–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Madden, D.J. Aging and visual attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 70–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Madden, D.J.; Whiting, W.L. Age-related changes in visual attention. Adv. Cell Aging Gerontol. 2003, 15, 41–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brockmole, J.R.; Logie, R.H. Age-related change in visual working memory: A study of 55,753 participants aged 8–75. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Takahashi, J.; Miura, K.; Morita, K.; Fujimoto, M.; Miyata, S.; Okazaki, K.; Matsumoto, J.; Hasegawa, N.; Hirano, Y.; Yamamori, H.; et al. Effects of age and sex on eye movement characteristics. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rep. 2021, 41, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coors, A.; Merten, N.; Ward, D.D.; Schmid, M.; Breteler, M.M.B.; Ettinger, U. Strong age but weak sex effects in eye movement performance in the general adult population: Evidence from the Rhineland study. Vision. Res. 2021, 178, 124–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lim, H.W.; Song, Y.; Kim, J.H.; Shin, Y.U.; Hwang, S.J.; Hong, S. Normal range of eye movement and its relationship to age. Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2017, 58, 747. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, W.J.; Kim, J.H.; Shin, Y.U.; Hwang, S.; Lim, H.W. Differences in eye movement range based on age and gaze direction. Eye 2019, 33, 1145–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ho, G.; Scialfa, C.T.; Caird, J.K.; Graw, T. Visual search for traffic signs: The effects of clutter, luminance, and aging. Hum. Factors 2001, 43, 194–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Underwood, G.; Humphrey, K.; van Loon, E. Decisions about objects in real-world scenes are influenced by visual saliency before and during their inspection. Vision. Res. 2011, 51, 2031–2038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horrey, W.J.; Lesch, M.F.; Garabet, A.; Simmons, L.; Maikala, R. Distraction and task engagement: How interesting and boring information impact driving performance and subjective and physiological responses. Appl. Ergon. 2017, 58, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dukic, T.; Ahlstrom, C.; Patten, C.; Kettwich, C.; Kircher, K. Effects of electronic billboards on driver distraction. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2013, 14, 469–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doroudgar, S.; Chuang, H.M.; Perry, P.J.; Thomas, K.; Bohnert, K.; Canedo, J. Driving performance comparing older versus younger drivers. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2017, 18, 41–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebnali, M.; Ahmadnezhad, P.; Shateri, A.; Mazloumi, A.; Ebnali Heidari, M.; Nazeri, A.R. The effects of cognitively demanding dual-task driving condition on elderly people’s driving performance; Real driving monitoring. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016, 94, 198–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Charlton, J.L.; Catchlove, M.; Scully, M.; Koppel, S.; Newstead, S. Older driver distraction: A naturalistic study of behaviour at intersections. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 58, 271–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karthaus, M.; Falkenstein, M. Functional changes and driving performance in older drivers: Assessment and interventions. Geriatrics 2016, 1, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Urlings, J.H.J.; Cuenen, A.; Brijs, T.; Lutin, M.; Jongen, E.M.M. Aiding medical professionals in fitness-to-drive screenings for elderly drivers: Development of an office-based screening tool. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2018, 30, 1211–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, S.S.Y.; Wood, J.M.; Black, A.A. Blur, eye movements and performance on a driving visual recognition slide test. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2015, 35, 522–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahana-Levy, N.; Shavitzky-Golkin, S.; Borowsky, A.; Vakil, E. The effects of repetitive presentation of specific hazards on eye movements in hazard perception training, of experienced and young-inexperienced drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2019, 122, 255–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mizumoto, A.; Takeuchi, O. Introduction to effect size and statistical power analysis: For the correct use of statistical tests. Rep. Methodol. Res. Comm. 2011, 1, 47–73. (In Japanese) [Google Scholar]
- Fritz, C.O.; Morris, P.E.; Richler, J.J. Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2012, 141, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicente, K.J.; Wang, J.H. An ecological theory of expertise effects in memory recall. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 105, 33–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Underwood, G. Visual attention and the transition from novice to advanced driver. Ergonomics 2007, 50, 1235–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mourant, R.R.; Rockwell, T.H. Strategies of visual search by novice and experienced drivers. Hum. Factors 1972, 14, 325–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Itoh, N.; Fukuda, T. Comparative study of eye movements in extent of central and peripheral vision and use by young and elderly walkers. Percept. Mot. Skills 2002, 94, 1283–1291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Igari, D.; Shimizu, M.; Fukuda, R. Eye movements of elderly people while riding bicycles. Gerontechnology 2008, 7, 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caird, J.K.; Edwards, C.J.; Creaser, J.I.; Horrey, W.J. Older driver failures of attention at intersections: Using change blindness methods to assess turn decision accuracy. Hum. Factors 2005, 47, 235–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horswill, M.S.; Pachana, N.A.; Wood, J.; Marrington, S.A.; McWilliam, J.; McCullough, C.M. A comparison of the hazard perception ability of matched groups of healthy drivers aged 35 to 55, 65 to 74, and 75 to 84 years. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 799–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Underwood, G.; Crundall, D.; Chapman, P. Selective searching while driving: The role of experience in hazard detection and general surveillance. Ergonomics 2002, 45, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Haan, T.; Stuiver, A.; Lorist, M.M.; de Waard, D. Other road users’ adaptations to increase safety in response to older drivers’ behaviour. Transp. Res. F 2022, 84, 277–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- ’t Hart, B.M.; Einhäuser, W. Mind the step: Complementary effects of an implicit task on eye and head movements in real-life gaze allocation. Exp. Brain Res. 2012, 223, 233–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Underwood, G.; Chapman, P.; Berger, Z.; Crundall, D. Driving experience, attentional focusing, and the recall of recently inspected events. Transp. Res. F 2003, 6, 289–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Falkenstein, M.; Karthaus, M.; Brüne-Cohrs, U. Age-related diseases and driving safety. Geriatrics 2020, 5, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coley, M.J. Self-Regulation as a Traffic Safety Strategy for the Older Driver. Ph.D. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Charlton, J.L.; Oxley, J.; Fildes, B.; Oxley, P.; Newstead, S.; Koppel, S.; O’Hare, M. Characteristics of older drivers who adopt self-regulatory driving behaviours. Transp. Res. F 2006, 9, 363–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Older | Middle-Aged | |
---|---|---|
age (year) | 73.9 (3.2) | 37.3 (2.9) |
driving history (year) | 49.1 (9.7) | 18.0 (2.4) |
nude/contact lens/glasses (n) | 12/1/3 | 4/5/3 |
eye surgery (yes/no) | 2/14 | 2/10 |
Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | |
---|---|---|---|
male/female (n) | 3/13 | 5/7 | n.s. |
driving frequency (count/week) | 7 (3–7) | 7 (7–7) | n.s. |
driving time (min/week) | 50 (30.0–60.0) | 40 (30.0–67.5) | n.s. |
experience of traffic violations (%) | 18.7 | 0.0 | n.s. |
experience of traffic crashes (%) | 12.5 | 8.3 | n.s. |
Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MMSE (scores) | 29.0 (27.8–30.0) | 30.0 (30.0–30.0) | 0.002 | 0.58 | 0.93 |
TMT-A (s) | 46.6 (41.7–65.7) | 31.0 (23.1–34.8) | <0.001 | 0.72 | 0.99 |
TMT-B (s) | 89.3 (75.8–113.6) | 39.3 (34.1–43.7) | <0.001 | 0.76 | 0.99 |
SDMT (%) | 37.3 (34.3–43.8) | 59.0 (55.0–64.7) | <0.001 | 0.80 | 0.99 |
Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
mean (%) | X | 52.2 (52.0–53.1) | 52.1 (51.5–52.4) | n.s. | 0.20 | 0.17 |
Y | 59.6 (57.9–61.7) | 57.9 (56.5–58.9) | 0.041 | 0.39 | 0.54 | |
max (%) | X | 73.3 (71.7–78.1) | 76.0 (74.1–76.9) | n.s. | 0.14 | 0.10 |
Y | 85.3 (83.6–90.9) | 75.9 (71.8–78.1) | <0.001 | 0.74 | 0.99 | |
min (%) | X | 28.9 (24.3–32.0) | 27.2 (23.8–28.7) | n.s. | 0.24 | 0.22 |
Y | 44.2 (39.1–47.8) | 43.5 (41.5–45.1) | n.s. | 0.11 | 0.08 |
Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Scene 8 (count) | 63.5 (57.8–71.0) | 57.0 (48.5–61.0) | 0.042 | 0.38 | 0.52 |
Scene 9 (count) | 60.5 (52.5–63.8) | 51.5 (46.5–55.0) | 0.043 | 0.38 | 0.52 |
Scene 10 (count) | 67.0 (60.3–73.0) | 57.5 (53.0–62.3) | 0.033 | 0.40 | 0.57 |
Traffic Situation | Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(h) | Power | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOI 17(%) (Scene 5) | 18.7 | 66.6 | 0.018 | 1.01 | 0.75 | |
AOI 21(%) (Scene 7) | 87.5 | 50.0 | 0.044 | 0.84 | 0.61 | |
AOI 31(%) (Scene 9) | 50.0 | 91.6 | 0.039 | 0.98 | 0.72 | |
AOI 33 (%) (Scene 9) | 12.5 | 83.3 | <0.001 | 1.57 | 0.98 |
Traffic Situation | Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOI 7(%) (Scene 2) | 7.8 (0.0–19.7) | 22.8 (13.0–31.0) | 0.042 | 0.38 | 0.52 | |
AOI 10(%) (Scene 3) | 11.3 (0.0–28.5) | 45.0 (26.8–51.5) | 0.010 | 0.48 | 0.76 | |
AOI 14(%) (Scene 4) | 43.8 (25.2–52.8) | 60.7 (43.9–74.8) | 0.017 | 0.45 | 0.69 | |
AOI 17(%) (Scene 5) | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 3.6 (0.0–11.4) | 0.023 | 0.48 | 0.76 | |
AOI 21(%) (Scene 7) | 19.4 (7.6–47.1) | 2.0 (0.0–23.9) | 0.029 | 0.42 | 0.62 | |
AOI 31(%) (Scene 9) | 0.9 (0.0–4.9) | 7.4 (3.7–10.0) | 0.013 | 0.47 | 0.74 | |
AOI 33(%) (Scene 9) | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 30.0 (14.2–43.4) | <0.001 | 0.76 | 0.99 | |
AOI 35(%) (Scene 10) | 18.8 (0.0–13.7) | 23.7 (11.8–34.8) | 0.029 | 0.42 | 0.62 | |
AOI 38 (%) (Scene 10) | 6.1 (4.2–15.1) | 24.8 (10.5–37.0) | 0.013 | 0.47 | 0.74 |
Traffic Situation | Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOI 4(sec) (Scene1) | 18.1 (17.9–21.4) | 15.9 (15.9–17.9) | 0.016 | 0.45 | 0.69 | |
AOI 14(sec) (Scene4) | 5.1 (3.4–5.4) | 3.5 (2.8–4.1) | 0.042 | 0.38 | 0.52 |
Traffic Situation | Older | Middle-Aged | p-Value | ES(r) | Power | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOI 5(%) (Scene 1) | 6.1 (3.6–7.7) | 8.8 (6.5–11.1) | 0.026 | 0.42 | 0.61 | |
AOI 10(%) (Scene 3) | 2.9 (0.0–4.0) | 4.5 (3.6–5.8) | 0.037 | 0.40 | 0.57 | |
AOI 17(%) (Scene 5) | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 2.6 (0.0–3.5) | 0.010 | 0.55 | 0.89 | |
AOI 18(%) (Scene 5) | 5.0 (3.3–6.8) | 7.8 (4.8–8.8) | 0.042 | 0.38 | 0.52 | |
AOI 30(%) (Scene 9) | 6.1 (3.8–8.2) | 8.0 (6.6–10.0) | 0.047 | 0.37 | 0.49 | |
AOI 31(%) (Scene 9) | 0.7 (0.0–2.4) | 2.8 (2.1–4.2) | 0.026 | 0.43 | 0.64 | |
AOI 33(%) (Scene 9) | 0.0 (0.0–0.0) | 4.1 (2.1–6.6) | <0.001 | 0.76 | 0.99 | |
AOI 35(%) (Scene 10) | 0.7 (0.0–2.4) | 3.3 (1.6–3.8) | 0.017 | 0.45 | 0.69 | |
AOI 38(%) (Scene 10) | 3.8 (1.5–7.4) | 7.7 (5.2–8.4) | 0.037 | 0.39 | 0.54 |
AOI | Hazard | Direction | Fixations | Percentage | Duration | Time | Visits |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
4 (Scene1) | users | straight | slow | ||||
5 (Scene1) | environment | straight | few | ||||
7 (Scene2) | users | straight | short | ||||
10 (Scene3) | environment | left turn | short | few | |||
14 (Scene4) | users | right turn | short | slow | |||
17 (Scene5) | users | straight | small | short | few | ||
18 (Scene5) | environment | straight | few | ||||
21 (Scene7) | environment | left turn | large | long | |||
30 (Scene9) | users | straight | many | few | |||
31 (Scene9) | users | straight | many | small | short | few | |
33 (Scene9) | environment | right turn | many | small | short | few | |
35 (Scene10) | users | right turn | many | short | few | ||
38 (Scene10) | users | straight | many | short | few |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kawabata, K.; Nakajima, Y.; Fujita, K.; Sato, M.; Hayashi, K.; Kobayashi, Y. Pilot Study on Gaze Characteristics of Older Drivers While Watching Driving Movies. Geriatrics 2024, 9, 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics9050132
Kawabata K, Nakajima Y, Fujita K, Sato M, Hayashi K, Kobayashi Y. Pilot Study on Gaze Characteristics of Older Drivers While Watching Driving Movies. Geriatrics. 2024; 9(5):132. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics9050132
Chicago/Turabian StyleKawabata, Kaori, Yuya Nakajima, Kazuki Fujita, Mamiko Sato, Koji Hayashi, and Yasutaka Kobayashi. 2024. "Pilot Study on Gaze Characteristics of Older Drivers While Watching Driving Movies" Geriatrics 9, no. 5: 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics9050132
APA StyleKawabata, K., Nakajima, Y., Fujita, K., Sato, M., Hayashi, K., & Kobayashi, Y. (2024). Pilot Study on Gaze Characteristics of Older Drivers While Watching Driving Movies. Geriatrics, 9(5), 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics9050132