Next Article in Journal
Preservation of Beef with Limonene-Rich Citrus Peel Extracts: Antioxidant, Antimicrobial and Textural Benefits
Previous Article in Journal
Design of Polymeric Delivery Systems for Lycium barbarum Phytochemicals: A Spray Drying Approach for Nutraceuticals
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationship Between Brand Presence and Emotions on Overall Acceptance and Purchase Intent of Commercial Chicken Noodle Soup

1
Department of Nutrition, Dietetics & Food Science, Brigham Young University, S133 Eyring Science Center, Provo, UT 84602, USA
2
JSA Nutrition, 138 E 12300 S #282, Draper, UT 84020, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2025, 14(20), 3505; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14203505
Submission received: 9 August 2025 / Revised: 8 October 2025 / Accepted: 10 October 2025 / Published: 15 October 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Sensory and Consumer Sciences)

Abstract

This study examined the influence of brand presence and discrete emotions on consumer acceptance and purchase intent of commercial chicken noodle soups. A total of 324 evaluations across three soup categories (chunky, low-sodium, condensed) were conducted under blind and unblinded conditions using a 42-term emotion lexicon. Brand presence did not exert moderate-to-large effects, though subtle brand-specific differences cannot be excluded. Instead, three emotions, “satisfied,” “disgusted,” and, for condensed soups, “bored,” emerged as the strongest predictors, together explaining a substantial proportion of variance in liking and purchase intent. Many other positive emotions clustered around “satisfied,” highlighting a parsimonious set of dominant drivers. Quiet positive emotions such as contentment, peacefulness, and warmth consistently aligned with both acceptance and purchase intent. These findings extend prior research by showing that consumer responses consolidate around a limited set of emotions, underscoring that evoking subtle, self-focused positive feelings may be more effective in comfort food marketing and product development than reliance on brand identity or nostalgia.

1. Introduction

Brand information is a powerful marketing tool that can shape consumer choices. Prior studies demonstrate that brand familiarity influences perceptions of quality, acceptance, and purchase intent across various food products [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. At the same time, emotions strongly affect food choice, consumption, and enjoyment, with both positive and negative feelings guiding consumer behavior [9,10,11,12,13].
Research suggests that brand cues may enhance liking and willingness to purchase via emotional pathways. For instance, brand information altered hedonic responses to pasta and beer [7,8]. while packaging influenced preferences for olive oils [14]. Similarly, emotions elicited during eating, such as satisfaction, contentment, or disgust—are closely tied to acceptance and purchase intent [15,16,17].
Various models have been developed to classify food-related emotions, including Ekman’s discrete theory [18], Russell’s circumplex model [19], and the HUMAINE framework [20]. Tools such as the Geneva Emotion and Odor Scale [21], the EsSence lexicon [18], and subsequent food-specific emotion lexicons [22,23] provide structured approaches for capturing consumer emotions.
Despite this progress, few studies have isolated the independent influence of brand presence on both emotions and purchasing, particularly for staple comfort foods. Understanding whether brand cues add value beyond intrinsic product qualities is relevant for both marketing and product development.
This study investigates how the relative influence of brand presence and discrete emotions on consumer acceptance and purchase intent for commercial chicken noodle soup. We hypothesized that (1) certain emotions would consistently align with overall acceptance, (2) brand knowledge would alter acceptance, emotions, and purchase intent, and (3) emotional profiles would vary across soup types (chunky, low-sodium, condensed).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Samples

Samples of commercial chicken noodle soup, with brands ranging from nationally recognized to those with private labels, were purchased from local grocery stores; one private label sample was obtained through a church-based organization serving people in need, which was generally recognized in the test area. Chicken noodle soup was selected as a model for this study because it is commonly associated with comfort during times of emotional distress or illness [24,25]. A wide range of samples was selected to represent the breadth of consumer experiences with commercial chicken noodle soup in the test area. Soups were divided into three categories: those with large pieces of chicken, vegetables and wide or spiral noodles [chunky (7 samples)], those with low-sodium, small chicken pieces and thin noodles [low-sodium (3 samples)], and those with small pieces of chicken and thin noodles [condensed (3 samples)], and were evaluated by category. The disparity between the number of soups in each category was due to market availability.

2.2. Sensory Evaluation

Panels were conducted at the Brigham Young University Sensory Laboratory (Provo, UT, USA). Two separate panels of 112 participants evaluated the low-sodium and condensed soups, while a separate panel of 110 participants assessed the chunky soups. The same individuals returned for both the blind and unblinded sessions within their assigned soup category, separated by two weeks, but no panelist participated in more than one soup category. This design ensured within-subject comparisons for each category while avoiding cross-category carryover effects. Panelists were recruited from a database of the campus and outlying communities and selected for their willingness to evaluate chicken noodle soup, brand familiarity, and reported eating it at least once every 3 months. Additionally, panelists for the low-sodium panels were screened for having consumed low-sodium chicken noodle soup within the past 3 months. For each panel, both genders were equally represented, with approximately equal representation across age categories from 20 to 29 years, 30 to 39 years, 40 to 49 years, 50 to 59 years, and 60 years and older. The study was approved by the University Institutional Review Board (IRB #000119), and panelists provided their informed consent. Soups were prepared, 3 or 4 cans at a time, according to the label instructions, and heated to 170 °F (76.67 °C) for 15 min on a gas stovetop in identical 3-quart stainless steel saucepans. Heated soups were then transferred to stainless steel pans and held on a steam table whose temperature cycled between 155 °F (68.33 °C) and 165 °F (73.89 °C). Fresh soups were prepared each hour.
Panelists received approximately 60 mL of each soup served into opaque plastic portion cups, in a randomized order, labeled with 3-digit blinding codes. Samples were received through pass-through compartments in isolated booths. Samples were presented sequentially and monadically in a completely randomized order. Panelists were instructed to use a bite of a cracker and a sip of bottled water to refresh their sense of taste between samples. The soup brand names were not displayed in the first panel (blind). In the second panel (unblinded), only the brand name of each soup, all in the same font and text, was revealed before evaluation.
Questions were presented one at a time on a computer screen, and data were collected using Compusense®5 (version 5.6) software (Compusense Inc., Guelph, ON, Canada). For each sample at both visits, panelists were asked to rate their overall acceptance of the soup on a discrete 9-point hedonic scale, with 1 indicating ‘dislike extremely’, 5 indicating ‘neither like nor dislike’, and 9 indicating ‘like extremely’. However, for the second panel, the brand name was disclosed before the questioning began. Subsequently, panelists were presented with an alphabetical list of forty-two emotions, 39 from King and Meiselman [22] and three more we chose to include—comfortable, content, and relaxed. Each emotion had an accompanying 100-point line scale on which panelists were asked to rank the intensity with which they felt each emotion on a 100-point continuous line scale labeled “not at all” on the far-left side to “extremely” on the far-right side. Finally, panelists were asked how likely/unlikely they were to purchase that soup on a discrete 9-point scale ranging from 1 = extremely unlikely, 5 = neither likely nor unlikely, to 9 = extremely likely. Panelists provided informed consent and were compensated monetarily for their time.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Initially, we performed stepwise regressions of the complete model to examine the influence of each emotion on consumers’ overall acceptance of the soups and their intent to purchase the soup in the future. Independent variables included soup category and a grouping of all types, as well as blind versus unblinded presentation. Dependent variables were hedonic scores and emotion intensity. Initial full-model stepwise regression indicated that brand presence was not a significant predictor. The final reduced model revealed that the effect on overall acceptance and purchase intent was primarily driven by three emotions. A post hoc sensitivity power analysis was conducted using G*Power 3.1 to determine the minimum effect sizes that could be detected with the available sample sizes at α = 0.05 and power = 0.80. For paired blind vs. unblinded comparisons (N = 100–112), the design was sensitive to within-subject effects of dz ≈ 0.27–0.28 (Cohen’s d ≈ 0.23–0.37). For across-category analyses (N = 324), the study was sensitive to omnibus effects of f ≈ 0.174 (η2 ≈ 0.029). Thus, the study was adequately powered to detect small to moderate effects, although smaller effects may not have been detected.

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Brand Presence

Brand presence had no practical positive or negative impact on panelists’ emotional responses regarding changes in overall acceptance and purchase intent for all soups combined and individual soup types. While Table 1 and Table 2 show variation between the blind and unblinded panels, the differences in the values of the incremental change in key emotions per unit change in overall acceptance were not statistically significant. The results of this study contrast those of previous studies, which concluded that brand and packaging information on both food products, such as dried semolina pasta, and non-food products could encourage positive emotions and significantly improve liking [7,13]. Such phenomena have been described as a result of “Brand Comfort”, where brands that are perceived to be capable of eliciting positive emotions are more likely to be sought out by consumers [26]. However, based on the results of this study, it is possible that brand information on certain types of food products (such as canned chicken noodle soup in this case) does not elicit a sufficient emotional response to affect overall acceptance or purchase intent. This, perhaps, could be because brand information affects certain types of food more than others due to consumer expectations surrounding different types of foods. Canned chicken noodle soup could fall into the category of “daily food”, where brand information matters less because consumers do not tend to view private label brands or major brands with much discernment or discrimination, thus explaining why brand presence did not seem to have any practical effects on panelists’ emotional responses. Although brand presence showed no overall effect, this null result may reflect a balance of opposing brand-specific responses, where stronger positive associations with nationally recognized brands were offset by less favorable responses to private labels, resulting in a net neutral outcome.
Additionally, the different types of soups did not all yield the same patterns of difference in responses between blind and unblinded panels. For the low-sodium and chunky soups, panelists expressed a greater positive change in feeling “satisfied” and a smaller change in feeling “disgusted” when the brand was revealed. In contrast, the opposite pattern was observed for condensed soups. Regardless of this disparity, the difference in relationships has no practical application. It should be noted, however, that the different emotional response profiles among the varieties of chicken noodle soups are consistent with a study by Spinelli et al. [13], who found that the emotional changes between informed and blind tasting differed depending on the hazelnut spread sample. Furthermore, while the analysis does not contrast the responses for blind and unblinded panels between popular and private label brands, the present analysis suggests that those distinct values would be similar to the reported combined values and that no additional helpful information to the marketing and study of chicken noodle soups, popular or private label, would emerge.
Although brand presence exerted no significant influence in aggregate, this null effect may reflect a balance of opposing brand-specific responses. For example, stronger positive associations with nationally recognized brands could have been nullified by less favorable responses to private labels. Given our design and statistical power, detailed stratification by brand or age group was not feasible. However, future studies should investigate these subgroup differences, as prior work suggests that brand effects are not uniform across consumer segments [1,2].

3.2. Emotions and Overall Acceptance

A stepwise regression relating the intensity with which panelists felt each of the emotions to overall acceptance, resulted in an average R-squared value of 0.62 for all soup types combined (Table 1).
Similarly, a second stepwise regression comparing responses for the same key emotions with consumer purchase intent yielded an average R-squared value of 0.67 (Table 2). Thus, we can understand that the change in overall acceptance and purchase intent, respectively, can be explained by changes in the perceptions of the 42 emotions. However, only 3 of the 42 emotions contributed with partial R-squared values greater than 0.01: satisfied, disgusted, and, for the condensed category, bored.
Despite the low partial R-squared values of the other 39 emotions, we wondered if these three emotions were more influential in determining both overall acceptance and purchase intent than our initial analyses revealed. We subsequently performed a pairwise correlation of the hedonic scores of the reported prevalence of each emotion at the time of consumption with the reported prevalence of the emotion “Satisfied” (Table 3). Most of the emotions included in the panel were positive emotions, so we interpreted a higher correlation of each emotion to “Satisfied” to represent a greater influence on both overall acceptance of the product and purchase intent. Using the Emotion Annotation and Representation Language presented by HUMAINE [19], eleven of the thirteen emotions most correlated with the emotion “satisfied” were characterized as “quiet positive” emotions.
Table 4 shows that emotions in this category were characterized as “caring”, “positive thoughts”, “positive and lively”, and “reactive”. “Worried”, the only emotion to have a significant correlation with “disgusted”, was characterized as an emotion that was “negative and not in control”. Although some of these relationships may appear intuitive, their quantified strength and consistency across multiple soup categories demonstrate that confirming and consolidating such effects provides both theoretical clarity and practical guidance for product development and marketing.
A Mixed Models Analysis of Covariance complemented the previous tests, confirming a positive relationship between responses for the emotion “satisfied” and both overall acceptance (Table 5) and purchase intent (Table 6). On average, a 26-point increase in the panelists’ response for this key emotion was associated with a one-point increase in overall acceptance. In contrast, there was an 18-point increase for each 1-point increase in purchase intent. Conversely, the responses to both “disgusted” and “bored” displayed a negative relationship with overall acceptance and purchase intent, albeit with a lesser magnitude. A 31-point increase in the panelists’ response for “disgusted” was associated with a 1-point decrease in overall acceptance, while a 42-point increase for that emotion reflected a 1-point decrease in purchase intent.
Confirmed by the stepwise regressions presented in Table 1 and Table 2, all types of chicken noodle soup displayed a significant relationship between emotional responses and both overall acceptance and purchase intent. Despite yielding the lowest combined R-squared value in both tests, the condensed chicken noodle soup panels maintained that 58% and 67% of the changes in responses for overall acceptance and purchase intent, respectively, could be attributed to changes in the panelists’ responses for the 42 emotions. These values are accepted as statistically significant. Thus, influencing the emotions perceived in the consumption of condensed, chunky, or low-sodium chicken noodle soup would also allow an individual to influence the overall acceptance and purchase intent of a consumer for that soup, perhaps indicating that for this product, intrinsic emotions are more influential than branding.
Because quiet positive emotions have been shown to have the greatest influence on overall acceptance and purchase intent of consumers for condensed, chunky, and low-sodium chicken noodle soup, marketers and product developers representing soup-producing companies should attempt to instill these emotions in consumers at the point of purchase and consumption. Suppose marketers enable consumers to feel content, pleasant, peaceful, secure, and warm at the time of purchase and attribute those emotions to the chicken noodle soup. In that case, consumers will be more likely to purchase the soup than if they had felt any other type of emotion. Additionally, by formulating their soups to evoke the same emotions in consumers at the time of consumption, product developers may increase the acceptance that consumers have for that soup, resulting in a greater likelihood of consumers becoming repeat customers.
A possible application for these results could be in the advertising of comfort foods, which tend to lift the spirits by inducing positive feelings. Several food market reports and studies have discussed consumer preferences for comfort foods [27,28]. The results of the present study suggest, however, that for this advertising to be especially successful, comfort food, such as chicken noodle soup, should especially evoke emotions categorized as “quiet positive emotions.”
Interestingly, “nostalgia” did not have as strong an influence on overall acceptance and purchase intent for chicken noodle soup as the quiet positive emotions. Past marketing campaigns for many popular brands of chicken noodle soup have attempted to evoke feelings of nostalgia among viewers, reminding them of possible fond memories. However, such tactics are unlikely to be the most successful in convincing consumers to purchase chicken noodle soup. Equally less successful is the promotion of interpersonal emotions, whether they are positive and lively or caring. Instead, the more successful marketer will attempt to generate quiet, positive emotions that involve the consumers themselves in the present, rather than focusing on relationships with others or the past. Some companies are already investing in creating new product lines and flavors that could create these quiet positive emotions by observing market trends, especially among millennials and Gen Z as they become a larger share of consumer spending, such as boba tea, canned seafood, and spicy or bold and strong flavors in existing product lines [28,29].
It should be noted that while this study presents findings indicating a relationship between certain perceived emotions and both overall acceptance and purchase intent, it does not review or make any claims about relationships involving the emotions that consumers expect to feel when eating chicken noodle soup at the time of purchase. Rick and Loewenstein [30] explain that when presentations and displays make consumers aware of the emotional pain of opportunity cost associated with forfeiting money in exchange for a good, consumers are less likely to choose to make the transaction. However, research is lacking to verify whether the expectation of positive emotions associated with using a product might induce consumers to complete a purchase. We recommend that subsequent studies account for this disparity by examining how consumers perceive a product and how they anticipate feeling if they choose to consume it. Consumers should be asked to rate their actual emotions after consumption, as well as to indicate any intention to make a repeat purchase.

3.3. Limitations

This study has several limitations that should be considered when interpreting the findings. First, the number of samples in each category was uneven due to market availability in the test area. Second, panelists were recruited from a single geographic area, primarily a university community and surrounding neighborhoods. Although the sample included a range of ages and a balanced gender, socioeconomic background, and cultural food practices, prior brand familiarity was not systematically controlled, which may limit generalizability. Third, participants took part in multiple sessions within a soup category, introducing the possibility of carryover effects. Panels were scheduled two weeks apart, and participants were not informed that the sessions were related; however, residual familiarity cannot be entirely ruled out. Fourth the emotion lexicon contained 42 terms, which—while comprehensive—may have been cognitively demanding and included overlapping descriptors (e.g., calm vs. peaceful). Fifth, different scales were used for acceptance (a 9-point hedonic scale) and emotions (a 100-point line scale). This mixed approach reflects common practice in sensory science but may have introduced complexity for participants.
Finally, the statistical analyses were exploratory in nature. Stepwise regression can inflate Type I error; although complementary methods (pairwise correlations, mixed-model ANCOVA) supported the findings, results should be interpreted as associative rather than causal. The post hoc sensitivity power analysis indicated that the study was adequately powered to detect small to moderate effects; however, subtle brand or category effects may have gone undetected. Finally, although brand presence showed no overall effect, we cannot exclude the possibility that positive responses to some brands were offset by negative responses to others, resulting in a net neutral outcome. More granular analyses by brand type or demographic subgroup may be needed to reveal such effects.

4. Conclusions

This study highlights that brand presence may have minimal practical influence on consumer acceptance of chicken noodle soup. At the same time, a parsimonious set of emotions, particularly ‘quiet positive’ feelings such as satisfaction, contentment, and warmth, serve as the strongest predictors of purchase intent. By employing a cross-category design and a comprehensive 42-term emotion lexicon, we extend previous sensory research and highlight the importance of linking discrete emotions to both liking and purchasing decisions. These findings suggest that product developers and marketers should prioritize fostering subtle, self-focused positive emotions over brand identity or nostalgia when promoting comfort foods. Future research should employ larger, more diverse samples, refine emotion lexicons, and examine both expected and realized emotions to clarify how emotional anticipation translates into actual consumption and repeat purchasing.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.P. and L.J.; methodology, A.P. and L.J.; formal analysis, L.J.; data curation, A.P.; writing—original draft preparation, D.W.L. and A.P.; writing—review and editing, D.W.L. and L.J.; supervision, L.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects of Brigham Young University (#000119, 30 October 2014).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to privacy restrictions.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Ryan Elder and Michelle Lloyd Call for consulting on methods, and Dennis Eggett for statistical analysis.

Conflicts of Interest

Author Adam Parker was employed by the company JSA Nutrition. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Choi, C.-J.; Xu, J.; Min, D.-G. Investigating of the influence process on consumer’s active engagement through emotional brand attachment and brand love. Asia Pac. J. Mark. Logist. 2024, 36, 3061–3080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Zandstra, E.H.; Willems, A.A.; Lion, R. Making salt-reduced products more appealing to consumers: Impact of front-of-pack messages on liking and table salt use over time. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 2762–2772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Quintal, V.; Phau, I. Brand leaders and me-too alternatives: How do consumers choose? Mark. Intell. Plan. 2013, 31, 367–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hoyer, W.D.; Brown, S.P. Effects of brand awareness on choice for a common, repeat-purchase product. J. Consum. Res. 1990, 17, 141–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Tan, L.; Li, H.; Chang, Y.-W.; Chen, J.; Liou, J.-W. How to motivate consumers’ impulse buying and repeat buying? The role of marketing stimuli, situational factors and personality. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 32524–32539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Di Monaco, R.; Cavella, S.; Di Marzo, S.; Masi, P. The effect of expectations generated by brand name on the acceptability of dried semolina pasta. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 429–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Guinard, J.-X.; Uotani, B.; Schlich, P. Internal and external mapping of preferences for commercial lager beers: Comparison of hedonic ratings by consumers blind versus with knowledge of brand and price. Food Qual. Prefer. 2001, 12, 243–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ai, Y.; Jiang, M. Effects of busy mindset on preference for high-calorie foods. Sci. Rep. 2024, 14, 16838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dicker-Oren, S.D.; Gelkopf, M.; Greene, T. The dynamic network associations of food craving, restrained eating, hunger and negative emotions. Appetite 2022, 175, 106019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Francis, L.A.; Rollins, B.Y.; Epel, E.S.; Lozinski, R.H. Stress-induced eating in rural adolescents: Unique variability among boys and adolescents with obesity. Appetite 2024, 203, 107705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Huang, F.; Wang, H.; Du, W.; Zhang, B. Insufficient capacity to cope with stressors decreases dietary quality in females. BMC Psychol. 2024, 12, 668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Wonderlich, J.A.; Breithaupt, L.; Thompson, J.C.; Crosby, R.D.; Engel, S.G.; Fischer, S. The impact of neural responses to food cues following stress on trajectories of negative and positive affect and binge eating in daily life. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2018, 102, 14–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Spinelli, S.; Masi, C.; Zoboli, G.P.; Prescott, J.; Monteleone, E. Emotional responses to branded and unbranded foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 42, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Delgado, C.; Gómez-Rico, A.; Guinard, J.-X. Evaluating bottles and labels versus tasting the oils blind: Effects of packaging and labeling on consumer preferences, purchase intentions and expectations for extra virgin olive oil. Food Res. Int. 2013, 54, 2112–2121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Westenhoefer, J.; Pudel, V. Pleasure from food: Importance for food choice and consequences of deliberate restriction. Appetite 1993, 20, 246–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ivens, B.S.; Leischnig, A.; Muller, B.; Valta, K. On the role of brand stereotypes in shaping consumer response toward brands: An empirical examination of direct and mediating effects of warmth and competence. Psychol. Mark. 2015, 32, 808–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Vaccaro, A.G. Feelings are messy: The feelings we study in affective science should be too. Affect. Sci. 2024, 5, 190–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Guthrie Yarwood, M. Chapter 5: Dimensional Models. In Psychology of Human Emotion: An Open Access Textbook; The Pennsylvania University Press: University Park, PA, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  19. Douglas-Cowie, E.; Cowie, R.; Sneddon, I.; Cox, C.; Lowry, O.; McRorie, M.; Martin, J.-C.; Devillers, L.; Abrilian, S.; Batliner, A.; et al. The HUMAINE Database: Addressing the collection and annotation of naturalistic and induced emotional data. In Proceedings of the Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction, Lisbon, Portugal, 12–14 September 2007; pp. 488–500. [Google Scholar]
  20. Douglas-Cowie, E.; Cox, C.; Martin, J.-C.; Devillers, L.; Cowie, R.; Sneddon, I.; McRorie, M.; Pelachaud, C.; Peters, C.; Lowry, O.; et al. The HUMAINE Database: Addressing the Collection & Annotation of Naturalistic & Induced Emotional Data. In Emotion-Oriented Systems: The HUMAINE Handbook; Petta, P., Pelachaud, C., Cowie, R., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 243–284. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chrea, C.; Grandjean, D.; Delplanque, S.; Cayeux, I.; Le Calvé, B.; Aymard, L.; Velazco, M.I.; Sander, D.; Scherer, K.R. Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to odors. Chem. Senses 2008, 34, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. King, S.C.; Meiselman, H.L. Development of a method to measure consumer emotions associated with foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 168–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bhumiratana, N.; Adhikari, K.; Chambers, E. The development of an emotion lexicon for the coffee drinking experience. Food Res. Int. 2014, 61, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dolan, K.D.; Ogasawara, Y.; Yamamoto, T. Consumers’ sensory perception and emotional response towards animal and plant-based soups with the addition of shio-koji. Int. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2024, 59, 8561–8571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Troisi, J.D.; Gabriel, S. Chicken soup really is good for the soul: “Comfort food” fulfills the need to belong. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 747–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Aiken, K.D.; Meuter, M.L.; Sukhdial, A. An exploration of comfort brands and the theory of brand comfort. J. Brand Strategy 2023, 11, 344–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Modica, E.; Cartocci, G.; Rossi, D.; Martinez Levy, A.C.; Cherubino, P.; Maglione, A.G.; Di Flumeri, G.; Mancini, M.; Montanari, M.; Perrotta, D.; et al. Neurophysiological responses to different product experiences. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2018, 2018, 9616301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Spence, C. Comfort food: A review. Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 2017, 9, 105–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Frez-Muñoz, L.; Fogliano, V.; Steenbekkers, B.L.P.A. Consumers’ familiarity level shapes motives and contexts for preparing and consuming dishes. J. Food Sci. 2024, 89, 6677–6693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Rick, S.; Loewenstein, G. The role of emotion in economic behavior. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd ed.; Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J.M., Barrett, L.F., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 138–156. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Stepwise Regression displaying the Partial R-Square between responses for key emotions and overall acceptance for three soup types and combined soup categories. Overall acceptance was measured on a scale where 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely. N = 112.
Table 1. Stepwise Regression displaying the Partial R-Square between responses for key emotions and overall acceptance for three soup types and combined soup categories. Overall acceptance was measured on a scale where 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely. N = 112.
Influence of Pertinent Key Emotions on Overall Hedonic Acceptance
EmotionLow-Sodium SoupCondensed SoupChunky SoupAll Soups
Satisfied0.49950.35840.46930.4515
Disgusted0.09670.15650.15640.1402
Bored0.0032 0.01430.0026 0.0048
All Emotions0.63950.58050.65460.6197
Table 2. Stepwise Regression displaying the Partial R-Square between responses for key emotions and purchase intent for three soup types and combined soup categories. Purchase intent was measured on a scale where 1 = extremely unlikely, 5 = neither likely nor unlikely, 9 = extremely likely. N = 112.
Table 2. Stepwise Regression displaying the Partial R-Square between responses for key emotions and purchase intent for three soup types and combined soup categories. Purchase intent was measured on a scale where 1 = extremely unlikely, 5 = neither likely nor unlikely, 9 = extremely likely. N = 112.
Influence of Pertinent Key Emotions on Purchase Intent
EmotionLow-Sodium SoupCondensed SoupChunky SoupAll Soups
Satisfied0.55720.49460.52250.5243
Disgusted0.02370.10950.10830.1004
Bored0.08070.01720.01070.0152
All Emotions0.70030.66600.67380.6661
Table 3. Pairwise Correlation of R-values. Each was categorized using the Emotion Annotation and Representation Language presented by HUMAINE. “Combined” signifies a composite of the responses for all three panels. For cells shaded in green, R-values > 0.90. For cells shaded blue, R-values > 0.80. For cells shaded pale orange, R-values > 0.70. For cells shaded light gray, R-values > 0.60. * Values for “Worried” give the correlation with “Disgusted”.
Table 3. Pairwise Correlation of R-values. Each was categorized using the Emotion Annotation and Representation Language presented by HUMAINE. “Combined” signifies a composite of the responses for all three panels. For cells shaded in green, R-values > 0.90. For cells shaded blue, R-values > 0.80. For cells shaded pale orange, R-values > 0.70. For cells shaded light gray, R-values > 0.60. * Values for “Worried” give the correlation with “Disgusted”.
Correlation of All Emotions to Response for “Satisfied”
Low-SodiumCondensedChunkyCombinedEmotional Group
Content0.89430.87200.90760.8964Quiet positive
Pleasant0.90830.87560.87900.8852Quiet positive
Good0.87860.84870.88310.8743Quiet positive
Relaxed0.81340.87010.89500.8692Quiet positive
Peaceful0.83460.83950.87310.8561Quiet positive
Glad0.85380.82560.86320.8524Quiet positive
Comfortable0.83840.83990.86430.8523Quiet positive
Secure0.82880.84560.83190.8341Quiet positive
Whole0.82150.79990.82010.8161Quiet positive
Good natured0.81990.81350.81230.8147Quiet positive
Warm0.79040.72820.84490.8052Quiet positive
Calm0.70040.78380.78550.7639Quiet positive
Quiet0.60300.63710.64310.6321Quiet positive
Mild0.55160.62540.66890.6314Quiet positive
Pleased0.93220.89400.93230.9233Positive thoughts
Steady0.75420.78180.79180.7807Positive thoughts
Nostalgic0.66480.67720.76370.7170Positive thoughts
Free0.66660.59390.63990.6355Positive thoughts
Friendly0.78660.72450.75610.7561Caring
Loving0.74430.78880.73950.7519Caring
Understanding0.70810.72380.69680.7059Caring
Affectionate0.72420.70050.69510.7030Caring
Tender0.67970.69430.69380.6909Caring
Interested0.76050.66910.74680.7286Reactive
Polite0.69820.69860.70910.7039Reactive
Happy0.85660.82970.87250.8589Positive and lively
Joyful0.78030.76950.77040.7721Positive and lively
Merry0.73860.71870.70890.7176Positive and lively
Enthusiastic0.73690.62670.70280.6911Positive and lively
Energetic0.69620.55880.67510.6491Positive and lively
Active0.65220.52400.64460.6153Positive and lively
Eager0.61090.50140.55490.5539Positive and lively
Adventurous0.61680.48190.61230.5793Unassigned
Daring0.50190.35450.39810.4109Unassigned
Aggressive0.30600.15430.14190.1810Unassigned
Guilty0.12210.06250.02110.0516Negative thoughts
Tame0.53660.47740.59850.5552Negative and not in control
Wild0.37660.16280.19830.2349Negative and not in control
Worried *0.65090.65340.67870.6668Negative and not in control
Table 4. Values are the average for all emotions in each emotional group, calculated from the results given in Table 3, except the listed value for “Worried”. For cells shaded blue, the R values range from 0.8 to 0.8999. Pale orange indicates 0.7–0.7999. Light gray indicates 0.6–0.6999.
Table 4. Values are the average for all emotions in each emotional group, calculated from the results given in Table 3, except the listed value for “Worried”. For cells shaded blue, the R values range from 0.8 to 0.8999. Pale orange indicates 0.7–0.7999. Light gray indicates 0.6–0.6999.
Average Correlation of Emotions to “Satisfied” by Emotional Group
Low-SodiumCondensedChunkyCombined
Quiet positive0.79550.80040.82660.8131
Positive thoughts0.75440.73670.78190.7641
Caring0.72860.72640.71620.7215
Reactive0.72930.68390.72800.7162
Positive and lively0.72450.64700.70420.6940
Unassigned0.47490.33020.38410.3904
Negative thoughts0.12210.06250.02110.0516
Negative and not in control0.27110.15260.20270.2075
Table 5. Mixed Models Analysis of Covariance yielding the average change in responses to key emotions per one point change in overall acceptance. Emotions are measured on a scale of 0 to 100. Overall acceptance was measured on a 1 to 9 scale where 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely.
Table 5. Mixed Models Analysis of Covariance yielding the average change in responses to key emotions per one point change in overall acceptance. Emotions are measured on a scale of 0 to 100. Overall acceptance was measured on a 1 to 9 scale where 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely.
Incremental Relationship Between Pertinent Emotions and
Overall Hedonic Acceptance
EmotionLow-Sodium SoupCondensed SoupChunky SoupAll Soups
BlindNot BlindBlindNot BlindBlindNot BlindBlindNot Blind
Satisfied25.1925.8437.1731.3524.6925.4525.7726.46
Disgusted−46.73−32.36−32.89−38.31−35.09−29.67−36.50−31.15
Table 6. Mixed Models Analysis of Covariance yielding the average change in responses to key emotions per one point change in purchase intent. Emotions are measured on a scale of 0 to 100. Purchase intent was measured on a 1 to 9 scale 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely.
Table 6. Mixed Models Analysis of Covariance yielding the average change in responses to key emotions per one point change in purchase intent. Emotions are measured on a scale of 0 to 100. Purchase intent was measured on a 1 to 9 scale 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely.
Incremental Relationship Between Pertinent Emotions and Purchase Intent
EmotionLow-Sodium SoupCondensed SoupChunky SoupAll Soups
BlindNot BlindBlindNot BlindBlindNot BlindBlindNot Blind
Satisfied16.6418.7320.4919.3416.7817.2717.4818.18
Disgusted−52.36−44.64−37.59−38.91−46.08−43.29−44.84−41.84
Bored−63.69−80.00−97.09−69.44−123.46−112.36−93.46−90.09
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Loh, D.W.; Parker, A.; Jefferies, L. Relationship Between Brand Presence and Emotions on Overall Acceptance and Purchase Intent of Commercial Chicken Noodle Soup. Foods 2025, 14, 3505. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14203505

AMA Style

Loh DW, Parker A, Jefferies L. Relationship Between Brand Presence and Emotions on Overall Acceptance and Purchase Intent of Commercial Chicken Noodle Soup. Foods. 2025; 14(20):3505. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14203505

Chicago/Turabian Style

Loh, Derui Wendell, Adam Parker, and Laura Jefferies. 2025. "Relationship Between Brand Presence and Emotions on Overall Acceptance and Purchase Intent of Commercial Chicken Noodle Soup" Foods 14, no. 20: 3505. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14203505

APA Style

Loh, D. W., Parker, A., & Jefferies, L. (2025). Relationship Between Brand Presence and Emotions on Overall Acceptance and Purchase Intent of Commercial Chicken Noodle Soup. Foods, 14(20), 3505. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14203505

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop