Next Article in Journal
Therapeutic Potential of Kelp Fucoidan in Rebiosis of Gut Microflora and Immune Homeostasis in Cyclophosphamide-Induced Immunosuppressed Mice
Previous Article in Journal
Evaluating the Effect of Thermal Treatment on Phenolic Compounds in Functional Flours Using Vis–NIR–SWIR Spectroscopy: A Machine Learning Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Agri-Food By-Products as Ingredients: Exploring Purchase Intentions Among a Sample of Italian Consumers

Department of Agricultural, Forestry, Food and Environmental Science, University of Basilicata, Via dell’Ateneo Lucano 10, 85100 Potenza, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2025, 14(15), 2664; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14152664
Submission received: 26 June 2025 / Revised: 20 July 2025 / Accepted: 26 July 2025 / Published: 29 July 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Sensory and Consumer Sciences)

Abstract

Consumer perceptions of upcycled foods, as well as the determinants of food choices, are still not well understood. The aim of this study was to evaluate the impact of psychological and personal traits on purchase intention (PI) towards upcycled foods of Italian consumers. Additionally, willingness to try (WTT), willingness to buy (WTB), and willingness to pay (WTP) for yogurt, bread, and biscuits made with by-products, namely, hazelnut skin and grape pomace powder, were collected. A web-based survey involving 505 consumers was conducted, and partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) was used to evaluate the model. It resulted that food neophobia and attitude towards upcycled foods had a significant impact on PI. In turn, attitude was affected by food neophobia as well as by objective knowledge about food by-products. Unexpectedly, frugality and environmental concern did not affect PI. WTP was product-specific; when WTP was compared to a reference price, it was higher for the yogurt prototype, followed by the bread and biscuits prototypes. Food neophobia affected WTT, WTB, and WTP. This study contributes to providing new insights into the determinants of consumers’ purchase intention for upcycled foods, which are an emerging category of products.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

In the face of growing concerns about food waste and sustainability, upcycling agri-food by-products into ingredients has emerged as a promising approach to reduce food losses and promote a more sustainable food system [1,2]. Upcycling involves transforming ingredients that otherwise would not have gone to human consumption into new and valuable products, extending the life cycle of food resources, and minimizing their environmental impact.
To date, a universal definition of upcycling still does not exist. A first attempt to provide a definition of upcycled food products was made in 2020 by a team of American researchers [3] interviewing food manufacturers affiliated with Upcycled Food Association. The study led to a definition comprising the aspect relating to the elevation of food that would be wasted, as well as the benefits for the environment and society. More recently, the concept of upcycling has been further elucidated. Indeed, Aschemann-Witzel et al. [4] provided a comprehensive definition of upcycled foods, distinguishing between “alternative use sense” and “novel use sense”: “The first is upcycling in an ‘alternative use sense,’ because it is about food or ingredients that could as well be eaten and are rescued from the threat of disposal, upcycling it in one way or other into alternative foods, and contributing value to society through avoidance of food being wasted. The second is upcycling in a ‘novel use sense.’ The difference to the first is that it starts with ingredients that are not regarded or commonly seen as edible, but by upcycling these in one way or other, results in new foods” [4]. Upcycling can involve food by-products; by repurposing these by-products into ingredients, manufacturers can create value from what was previously considered waste.
Upcycling can lead to three advantages, akin to the three pillars of sustainability [2]: economical, by giving new value to recovered raw materials cheaper than the original; environmental, by reducing the pressure on the ecosystem; and on a social level, by reducing food waste. In addition, food by-products, when used as food ingredients, may act as functional ingredients [5,6]. Indeed, they could represent a source of fiber, protein, and many other bioactive compounds.
The market of upcycled food is young, and foods obtained from upcycled raw materials are not widespread. Anyway, a growing number of research studies highlight the importance of the topic; additionally, market agencies foresee a robust growth in the next 10 years for the upcycled food product market [7,8]. Different companies, both new and existing ones, are focusing their efforts on the production of upcycled foods or ingredients, emphasizing in their campaigns the importance of their approach to the environment.
The acceptance of these products is intended as the intention to purchase them, and the liking of the product itself should not be taken for granted. Indeed, different dimensions are involved in the acceptance of this new category of foods, such as consumers’ characteristics, marketing strategies, political measures, as well as the product itself.
The topic is complex, and consumers’ preferences and behaviors towards upcycled foods are still unclear. To date, different studies have tried to explore factors affecting them. An aspect not to be neglected is that when products are made from recycled materials, consumers may perceive them as of lower value [9]. Moreover, the acceptance of the use of by-products in food production is complex due to consumers’ traits such as food neophobia and technophobia [10]. Aschemann-Witzel and Stangherlin [9] collected, from published papers (2010–2020), the factors that influence consumers’ acceptance of waste-to-value products, classifying them as “individual factors”, “context factors”, and “product-related factors”. These factors and their interaction can shape consumer acceptance of waste-to-value food products. For example, Grasso et al. [11] compared consumers’ attitudes of China and the US towards upcycled foods, highlighting differences in the attitudes and preferences among the two groups; Stelick et al. [12] reported that providing information on sustainability affected the PI of bars with brewers’ spent grains more than nutritional information, whereas Zhang et al. [13] reported that upcycled food quality is perceived differently by different consumer generations. Other researchers from Italy [14] highlighted that sense of retribution, general health interest, and moral domains could be linked to PI of upcycled foods. These are just some examples underlining the complexity of the topic. Therefore, it is crucial to understand the determinants of consumer PIs and attitudes towards upcycled foods. This new category of products and all the related topics represent something that producers, consumers, and governments began to address in the last few years and will increasingly confront in the near future. Therefore, this study aims to explore the relationships between several psychological traits and the purchase intention for foods containing agri-food by-products. To this end, a theoretical model was tested on a sample of Italian consumers to elucidate the key determinants of intention and willingness to pay for such products.
To ensure clarity, the questionnaire specifically addressed the use of by-products as ingredients rather than adopting the term “upcycled foods”. In fact, upcycled food does not have a direct Italian translation; the closest equivalent is “cibi riciclati” (recycled foods), which may evoke negative or misleading connotations. This issue is not unique to Italian. For instance, in the Norwegian context, the absence of a direct and widely accepted translation for upcycled food has been noted [15]. Similarly, in Italian, the terminology is still evolving and lacks a universally adopted equivalent. This work contributes to broadening the information on the consumers’ attitudes towards upcycled foods.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample, Data Collection, and Measurement

The questionnaire was addressed to the Italian adult population, and participants were recruited by snowball sampling, a non-probability sampling technique. It was spread by means of mailing lists, social networks, instant messaging apps, and word of mouth. The questionnaire was built and distributed by means of Microsoft Forms (Microsoft 365, Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). Data were collected between March and July 2023.
The questionnaire measured the impact of attitude, food neophobia, environmental concern, green practices, frugality, and previous knowledge on the PI of foods obtained from by-products of the agri-food industry. Additionally, three examples of upcycled food products were proposed to evaluate consumers’ willingness to try, to buy, and to pay (WTT, WTB, and WTP). All measurements were made on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree), except for attitude (7-point bipolar scale), green practices (5-point Likert frequency scale), and objective knowledge (true/false questionnaire).
The various constructs were drawn, or adapted, from previous works in the literature. Table 1 illustrates the structure of the questionnaire, the scale employed for each construct, and the references.
The first part of the questionnaire included an introduction to the survey and informed consent. Participants were informed about the estimated completion time, the aim of the study, risks, and benefits, and that all data would be de-identified and only reported in aggregated form. Participants who did not give their consent were not able to proceed with completing the questionnaire.
Then, questions to measure 7 psychometric characteristics (food neophobia, environmental concern, green practices, frugality, subjective and objective knowledge, and attitude towards the use of food by-products as ingredients) were submitted to consumers. A short text including a description of by-products (Supplementary Material File S1) was provided before attitude constructs. This was deemed necessary to ensure a baseline understanding of the topic, given the expected low consumer familiarity, and to avoid responses based on misunderstanding. Subsequently, a question oriented to collect the PI of foods containing by-products as ingredients was submitted to consumers. The question was not on a specific food product but on the broader category of “foods containing by-products as ingredients”. Then, the last part of the questionnaire was used for collecting willingness to try, to buy, and to pay for three examples of foods containing by-products. Food product examples were taken from scientific literature, among the categories of products that are generally consumed by the Italian population. A dairy product and two baked goods were considered; namely, yogurt with hazelnut skin [23], bread with grape pomace powder [24], and biscuits with brewers’ spent grains [25] were selected. Concerning WTP, consumers were asked to indicate the price they would be willing to pay for the products while provided with an external reference price that was the medium selling price for the “conventional” version of each product. The medium price provided for each product was calculated as the mean of prices in all Italian provinces, reported by the Ministry of Enterprises and Made in Italy on its price observatory for consumer goods, as of November 2022. At the very end of the questionnaire, demographic information was collected.

Subjects

The questionnaire was checked and revised by the Transparency Office and Data Protection Officer of the University of Basilicata to verify compliance with European data protection legislation (Reg EU 2016/679). Due to the negligible risk posed to participants, no formal ethical approval was required. Participants gave their consent to participate prior to completing the questionnaire. The study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki and the European Code of Conduct for Research Integrity.

2.2. Theoretical Framework and Hypothesis Development

The following paragraphs briefly illustrate the hypotheses and their background, whereas Figure 1 summarizes the conceptual model of the research.

2.2.1. Attitude

Attitude refers to the degree to which a person has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation of a specific behavior. Previous research has shown that a positive attitude towards specific products, such as organic milk [26], organic meat [27], plant-based alternatives to yogurt [28], insects [29], and expired products that are still edible [30], positively influences consumers’ PI towards them. Similarly, a positive attitude towards functional foods was found to be a key driver of the consumption of foods containing grape pomace powder [31]. Based on the above, it was hypothesized that:
H1. 
A positive attitude towards the use of food by-products as food ingredients has a positive impact on consumers’ PI towards them.

2.2.2. Food Neophobia

Food neophobia is the reluctance to eat new foods or their avoidance [17]. Together with disgust, it may hinder the acceptability of new food products due to aversion to sensory characteristics, danger, fear of the consequences of eating unfamiliar foods, or disgust arising from ideas about the nature or origin of the food [31]. Consumers may reject or be skeptical of “recycled” foods because of their fear of new or unfamiliar [32,33,34]. Therefore, food neophobia is expected to be a barrier to the purchase of food products containing by-products of the agri-food industry, as well as the attitude towards them. Hence, it was hypothesized that:
H2. 
Food neophobia has a negative impact on consumers’ attitudes towards the use of by-products in food formulations.
H3. 
Food neophobia negatively influences consumers’ PI for food products containing by-products of the agri-food industry.

2.2.3. Environmental Concern

Environmental concern refers to the degree to which consumers are aware of and concerned about environmental problems, and their willingness to take action to address them [35]. Previous research has shown that environmental concern has a significant positive impact on PIs for veggie burgers [36] and eco-friendly products [37]. This is likely because consumers who are more concerned about the environment are more prone to purchase foods that are produced in a sustainable way. Indeed, it was previously reported that a higher PI of functional foods with pomace powder in consumers with a higher degree of concern for the environment [31]. Based on this evidence, it was hypothesized that:
H4. 
Environmental concern positively influences consumers’ PI for food products containing by-products of the agri-food industry.

2.2.4. Green Practices

Green practices are everyday actions that are taken to protect the environment, such as energy saving, recycling, waste reduction, and sustainable transportation [38]. Research has shown that consumers who engage in more green practices have greater satisfaction with and more frequent purchases of green products [38]. White, Habib, and Hardisty [39] suggested a positive spillover effect in green consumer behavior, where adopting one environmentally friendly practice increases the likelihood of adopting others. Hence, it could be expected that consumers who engage in more daily actions to protect the environment will also be more likely to purchase food products containing by-products of the food industry. This is because upcycling by-products into food ingredients is a way to reduce waste and promote environmental sustainability. Thus, it was hypnotized that:
H5. 
Green practices positively influence consumers’ PI for food products containing by-products of the agri-food industry.

2.2.5. Frugality

Frugality is the behavior of reducing the waste of resources, including money, and making use of what one has. It can lead to reduced consumption and the implementation of reuse, repair, and recycling behaviors [40]. Several authors have pointed out that frugality should not be confused with avarice [41,42]. Instead, it is a voluntary restriction on consumption for the efficient use of resources and the avoidance of waste [20], which can drive more sustainable consumption choices.
Upcycled foods may fall within the concept of frugality. Aschemann-Witzel et al. [20] reported that people with a more frugal orientation have a more positive attitude towards this type of food. It is therefore likely that consumers with frugal traits are more prone to purchase foods that avoid the waste of resources. Based on the above, the following hypothesis was proposed:
H6. 
Frugality has a positive impact on consumers’ PI for foods containing by-products of the agri-food industry.

2.2.6. Product Knowledge—Objective and Subjective Knowledge

Subjective knowledge is what an individual believes he knows about a topic or product, while objective knowledge is based on the accurate understanding of the information stored in memory. Product knowledge, even in the absence of direct experience, can lead to the formation of a specific attitude based on the information and knowledge that the consumer has. Product knowledge influences consumer attitudes because it allows them to better understand the products and think rationally about them. For example, consumers who are well informed about renewable products are better able to understand that these can save resources and energy than those who are not [43].
Regarding food products, both objective and subjective knowledge have been found to influence the intention to purchase insect-based products [29]. Baldissera et al. [31] also reported that greater prior knowledge of functional foods and whole grain products translates into a higher intention to purchase products obtained with grape pomace powder. Diverging information has been reported on the role of previous knowledge on the PI of and attitude towards organic food products. Nautiyal and Lal [21] found that objective knowledge has a crucial role in shaping Indian consumers’ purchasing intentions for organic products, unlike subjective knowledge, which has no significant effect. Similarly, Bhutto et al. [27] reported that knowledge of organic production practices did not have a significant effect on the development of intention to purchase organic meat. Based on the above considerations, it was hypothesized that:
H7. 
Objective knowledge of food industry by-products positively influences consumers’ PI for foods containing agri-food by-products as ingredients.
H8. 
Objective knowledge of food industry by-products positively influences consumer attitude towards the use of by-products in food formulations.
H9. 
Subjective knowledge of topics related to food industry processing by-products has a positive impact on consumers’ PI for foods with agri-food by-products as ingredients.
H10. 
Subjective knowledge of topics related to food industry processing by-products has a positive impact on the PI of foods with agri-food by-products as ingredients.

2.3. Data Analysis

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) technique was used to test the theoretical model with the software Smart PLS (v. 4.0.9.6, SmartPLS GmBH, Bönningstedt, Germany) [44]. Measurement model assessment included the evaluation of construct reliability and validity. Construct reliability was established through Cronbach’s Alpha and Composite Reliability. Convergent validity was assessed through average variance extracted (AVE), whereas discriminant validity was assessed through Fornell–Larcker Criterion and heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio of the correlations. The causal relationship presumed was tested through analysis of the structural model. Subsequently, bootstrapping with 5000 subsamples was used during hypothesis testing [45].
To test for the potential moderating effects of gender and age, a multi-group analysis (MGA) was performed. Path coefficients were estimated separately for each subgroup via bootstrapping (5000 resamples), and the differences between the coefficients were assessed for statistical significance using a t-test.
To further investigate the hierarchical relationship among behavioral intentions, a Path Analysis was implemented within the PLS-SEM framework. Three separate models, one for each product example, were estimated to test the sequential causal chain PI→WTT→WTB→WTP. The statistical significance of the path coefficients was determined via bootstrapping (5000 resamples).
One-Way ANOVA (Analysis of Variance), followed by Tukey HSD test (p < 0.05), was used to compare both WTT and WTB mean values of yogurt, bread, and biscuits with by-products. Student-t test (p < 0.05) was conducted to evaluate differences between WTT and WTB for each sample. Moreover, respondents were clustered based on their food neophobia (neophilic, neutral, and neophobic); a non-parametric Kruskal–Wallis test, followed by Dunn test (p < 0.05), was used to compare WTT, WTB, and WTP mean values expressed by each cluster. These tests were carried out with XLSTAT Premium Version (2019.4.2, Addinsoft, Paris, France) in Excel 365 (Microsoft 365; Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA).

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Characteristics of the Sample

The survey was submitted to the Italian adult population by means of an online questionnaire: 534 people answered the questionnaire, 13 did not give consent, whereas 16 answers were removed due to the very short time (<6 min) employed to answer the questionnaire; 505 answers were considered for data analysis. Sociodemographic information of the sample is reported in Table 2. The utilized sampling technique led to a slight overrepresentation of females (62.57%) and younger age groups (approximately 54% of respondents were under 34 years old, while about 18% were over 55 years old).
It should be noted that this is an explorative study and does not claim to be generalizable to the entire Italian population. Younger respondents (18–34) are strongly overrepresented, while individuals over 65 years of age, who account for approximately 24% of the Italian population [46], make up only 4.4% of the sample. Similarly, students and student–workers account for nearly 40% of the sample, which is not reflective of national employment statistics. The study employed a non-probability sampling technique, and data were collected exclusively through online channels. This approach likely favored the participation of younger and more digitally literate individuals, leading to a lower response rate among older consumers. While we recognize that other methods might have yielded a more representative sample, resource constraints prevented the implementation of such an approach.

3.2. Descriptive Statistics, Reliability, and Validity Tests

An analysis of the descriptive statistics (Supplementary Material File S2) was conducted to assess the variability in responses. Responses had sufficient variability to discriminate among consumers. Nevertheless, the results show two patterns: On the one hand, constructs, such as Environmental Concern (EC) and Frugality (FRU), exhibited low variability, indicating a stronger consensus compared to other items. This could be potentially influenced by social desirability. On the other hand, constructs representing individual traits, such as Food Neophobia (NP) and Knowledge (SK, OK), showed high variability. This heterogeneity confirms that these variables effectively discriminate between respondents. Also, the crucial constructs of Attitude and Purchase Intention displayed sufficient variability, justifying the use of PLS-SEM to model their relationships.
The measurement model was evaluated to ensure the reliability and validity of the constructs. Hair et al. [47] suggested desirable factor loadings of over 0.708, with a minimum acceptable value of 0.50. Anyway, it is recommended to check the impact of removing items with loadings below 0.7 on composite reliability and Average Variance Extracted (AVE) before automatic removal [41,42]. Acceptable AVE is at least 0.500, indicating that the construct explains at least 50% of its items’ variance [48].
All factor loadings in the current study (Supplementary Material File S2) were above 0.5. However, items GP1 (0.540) and GP2 (0.631) were removed because the initial AVE value was below 0.5. Their removal increased AVE to 0.539. Specifically, the items removed from the Green Practices construct were “I buy products with recyclable packaging” (GP1) and “I recycle plastic, paper, and glass” (GP2). Although related to environmentalism, these two items describe habitual, low-effort behaviors that are often dictated by local regulations. Similarly, item NP2 with a slightly lower loading (0.572) was removed since its removal had a positive impact on AVE (0.5 vs. 0.486). While fully acknowledging the validated and international status of the Food Neophobia Scale, the item NP2, “In the choice of food I do not trust novelty”, showed weaker statistical performance. This may depend on a linguistic difference: The concept of not trusting can capture a broader dimension of skepticism, which is distinct from the fear or avoidance that is core to neophobia. Other items with outer loadings slightly lower than 0.7 were retained because overall construct reliability and validity were higher than the recommended threshold values. Table 3 summarizes reliability and validity values after the removal of the above-mentioned items.
Reliability was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha, rho_a, and rho_c. The Cronbach’s alpha of each construct, except for GP, was greater than 0.708, suggesting the reliability of the scales. Even though GP’s Cronbach’s alpha was slightly lower than the recommended threshold, its composite reliability exceeded HTMT and the Fornell–Larcker criterion [44]. Discriminant validity was established through cross-loadings analysis (Supplementary Material File S2).

3.3. Structural Model Analysis

Path analysis was conducted to test the hypothesis; the bootstrap resampling method with 5000 resamples was employed to assess the path significance levels.
Overall, the model was able to explain a substantial portion of the variance in purchase intention (R2 = 0.555). The summary of hypothesis testing results is reported in Table 4.
A preliminary overview of the path coefficients indicates that the primary drivers in the model are psychological and knowledge-based. Attitude emerged as the most powerful predictor of PI and was itself strongly influenced by Objective Knowledge and Food Neophobia. Conversely, paths originating from Environmental Concern and Frugality, as well as Subjective Knowledge, are weak and not statistically significant. This finding offers a strategic insight for managers and policy makers: Communication efforts based on these general values are unlikely to be effective levers for driving purchase intention.
Attitude had a significant positive impact on the intention to purchase foods containing by-products as ingredients (H1). This aligns with previous research highlighting the significant role of consumer attitudes in influencing purchase decisions [45], and it underscores the importance of consumer attitudes in shaping their purchasing decisions. Though showing a good variability, the mean scores for attitude in our sample were noticeably positive. This generally favorable predisposition is consistent with aspects of Italian culture, where the no-waste principle is a deep-rooted social norm inherited from past generations.
Food neophobia, on the other hand, had a significant negative impact on attitude and PI (H2, H3). The result is consistent with previous research [46,47]; food neophobia is a barrier to the acceptance of new foods, including foods containing upcycled ingredients. In a recent scoping review [46], the factors affecting consumers’ acceptance of upcycled food products were investigated; the authors highlighted a strong relationship between neophobic tendencies and acceptance of upcycled food products. The individual trait of food neophobia is particularly interesting when interpreted in the context of Italian culinary norms. Italy is characterized by a strong culinary tradition and a deep attachment to specific recipes and ingredients. This polarization makes neophobia a crucial psychological trait to consider when introducing food innovations into a market with such a deep-rooted gastronomic identity. Contrary to the tested hypothesis (H4), environmental concern was not found to have a significant impact on PI. A possible explanation is that participants may not yet recognize a direct link between purchasing foods containing upcycled ingredients and reducing environmental impact. Furthermore, this mismatch between the concern and the behavior (in this case, the statement of purchase intention) may reflect the “attitude–behavior gap” [49], with consumers declaring to be concerned about the environment but struggling to translate this concern into concrete action [50].
Green practices, which somehow can be seen as a counterpart of environmental concern, showed a significant effect on PI (H5). The more consumers engage in practices aimed at protecting the environment, the higher is PI for upcycled foods. Although the path coefficient is modest (0.086), it does not indicate a lower importance of the construct; it suggests that existing green behaviors, while not the primary driver, can act as an enabling factor for this type of purchase, mediated or moderated by other factors. As a practical implication, this suggests that consumers who actively engage in sustainable practices could represent a key initial and receptive target segment for early marketing and communication efforts.
The concept of frugality in the context of upcycled food production is quite new [20]. The authors found that when frugality framing was used in the experiment, the attitude towards upcycled foods was higher. In this work, frugality did not have a significant impact on PI (H6). As per the EC construct, this seems an apparent contradiction, especially for a sustainable product, but it could be explained by the value–action gap [51]. Consumer behavior literature suggests that general and abstract “values” often fail to translate into specific behaviors. Other factors, such as the specific attitude towards the product or food neophobia, among others, carry greater weight, leaving background values behind when it comes to making a decision on the product [49].
Objective knowledge had a significant effect on attitude towards the use of by-products as food ingredients (H7). Indeed, it has been previously seen that a previous knowledge of a topic contributes to shaping consumers’ attitudes [21,23,38]. Nevertheless, the direct path from objective knowledge to PI (H8) was not significant. This suggests a mediation effect of attitude between OK and PI. Hence, a new hypothesis was added to the original framework (H11) about the mediating role of attitude in the relationship between objective knowledge and purchase intention. The results supported full mediation. Subjective knowledge (H9), which is what one thinks to know about a specific topic, did not have a significant effect on attitude and PI (H10). In this study, what consumers think they know about the topic seems not to be relevant to their decision-making process.
To deepen the understanding of the model, a Multi-Group Analysis (MGA) was conducted to examine whether the hypothesized causal relationships differed significantly across key demographic characteristics, namely, gender and age MGA revealed a moderating effect of gender on several key paths of the model: Specifically, the impact of food neophobia on attitude (NP→ATT, difference F-M: −0.144, p < 0.05), that of subjective knowledge on attitude (SK→ATT, difference F-M: −0.280, p < 0.01), and the link between attitude and purchase intention (ATT→PI, difference F-M: −0.136, p < 0.05) were found to be statistically different between men and women. Age, conversely, showed a moderating role in the relationship between attitude and purchase intention (ATT→PI, difference “18–44”–“45+”: −0.138, p < 0.05) only for the older consumer segment.

3.4. Willingness to Try, Buy, and Pay

To investigate consumer attitudes towards specific food products containing by-products, three products were presented to consumers, and their WTT, WTB, and WTP were collected. Namely, a short description of a yogurt with hazelnut skin, a bread containing grape pomace powder, and biscuits with brewers’ spent grains was presented to consumers.
Significant differences were found between mean WTT and WTB expressed for each product (Table 5), indicating that consumers are more prone to express their intention to try the new products but not to buy them. Indeed, consumers may be more willing to try new products without committing to a purchase, to avoid buying something that could disappoint them. However, no significant differences (p > 0.05) were observed in WTB or WTT among the three products (Table 5).
Over 65% of consumers expressed a positive WTB and WTT for the proposed products, scoring above 4 on a 7-point Likert scale. However, WTB scores were slightly lower, with scores above 4 ranging from 53.66% to 59.01%. While positive responses dominated, a portion of consumers expressed the lowest possible score (1 = not at all) on the WTT scale for each product: 7.92% for yogurt, 12.08% for bread, and 9.90% for biscuits.
While most consumers appear receptive to trying food products with by-products (over 65% scored positively in our study), a portion did not want to try at all (between 7.92% and 12.08%), likely due to unfamiliarity or specific personal traits. Actions aimed at educating consumers about the benefits of these products, such as sustainability and potential health benefits, could be crucial for a wider acceptance [46]. While WTT and WTB values were quite encouraging, WTP values showed a slightly different trend. In this study, WTP for yogurt, bread, and biscuits made with by-products was collected via an open-ended question. Consumers were asked how much they would be willing to pay for each product presented while provided with the average market price of a similar conventional product as a reference point. Consumers expressed a different WTP based on the product presented; Table 6 reports the mean WTP values expressed by consumers and the difference with the reference price for the conventional products.
On average, consumers are willing to pay more for the yogurt with hazelnut skin compared to the reference price provided, but less for bread with grape pomace powder and biscuits with brewers’ spent grains. It is important to note that each example product combines a unique food matrix with a specific by-product. As such, it is not possible to determine whether consumers’ willingness to pay was influenced more by the nature of the food product (e.g., yogurt vs. bread) or by the type of by-product (e.g., hazelnut skin vs. grape pomace). Nonetheless, some methodological precautions were taken to reduce potential bias. First, all the selected food matrices—yogurt, bread, and biscuits—are widely recognized and regularly consumed in Italy, which reduces the influence of unfamiliarity on participants’ responses. Second, a reference price was provided for each product. This allowed respondents to evaluate the price for products containing by-products against a realistic baseline. As reported above, the collected data do not provide any information on the drivers of WTP, which could depend on both the product category and the type of by-product added. It could be supposed that consumers may be willing to pay more for a product with a by-product/ingredient they are already familiar with or have eaten at home (e.g., hazelnuts with their skin on). On the other hand, consumers may be unwilling to pay more for staple products such as bread and biscuits, or for products containing something that is usually wasted, or they are not familiar with, such as grape pomace powder and brewers’ spent grains. In a recent study, Grasso et al. [11] reported that some food categories are preferred by consumers as food, where by-products can be added. For example, breakfast, snack, pasta, and soup were the top choices in the United States, whereas breakfast, snack, dairy, and baked goods were the top choices in China. Additionally, the same study investigated the preferred combinations of by-products and food categories, showing that consumers can have different preferences, and some combinations of food category/type of by-product are preferred compared to others. Generally, WTP for upcycled foods is lower than WTP for conventional foods, but product-specific differences could exist [49,50]. Hence, differences in WTP could be due to consumers who are more likely to accept upcycled alternatives for some food categories than others, as previously reported by Bhatt et al. [52]. In this regard, Ghazanfar et al. [53] reported that consumers’ WTP was significantly less for upcycled granola bars, chicken nuggets, and ice cream but not for muffins and pasta sauce. Additionally, there is also a deep-rooted idea that products containing by-products should cost less compared to the conventional ones. In fact, by-products are often considered to be waste materials, and consumers may believe that these products should be priced lower as a result. Nonetheless, it is not known that, from an economic perspective, in many cases, wasting by-products is economically more advantageous than saving them [26]. As expected, when clustering consumers based on their food neophobia, consumers with a higher level of food neophobia scored lower (p < 0.05) for WTT and WTB for all three products considered. Additionally, food neophobia level affected WTP of bread and biscuits, with neophobic consumers scoring low for WTP of bread and biscuits (p < 0.05). Nevertheless, no significant differences were observed among the three groups for yogurt; this result could depend on the “low” external reference price provided. When analyzing WTT and WTB data for each product and within each food neophobia level, this result was not confirmed. Indeed, neophobic consumers scored lower for WTT and WTB for yogurt as well as for bread and biscuits. This indicates that food neophobia level affected these parameters regardless of the product considered.

Path Analysis of Behavioral Intentions

The Path Analysis, conducted to explore the sequential chain of behavioral intentions, showed consistent results across all three product prototypes. As reported in Table 7, all causal paths in the PI→WTT→WTB→WTP model were found to be positive and statistically significant (p < 0.001) in all three cases. Results showed that a general intention to purchase upcycled products (PI) effectively translates into a willingness to try (WTT), and that, in our hypothetical scenario—without the real tasting of the product—a strong WTB is generated. This latter link is particularly strong across all products.

4. Conclusions

This study contributes to consumer science by demonstrating that for novel food categories like upcycled products, psychological traits are more critical than general pro-environmental values. Specifically, it was found that food neophobia and consumer attitude are primary drivers of the purchase intention for foods containing agri-food by-products, whereas environmental concern did not show a statistically significant impact on PI. Furthermore, the perceived value of foods containing by-products as ingredients is not absolute but is product-dependent, with some prototypes able to command a price premium. These findings suggest that marketing strategies should focus on shaping favorable attitudes and be tailored to each product’s specific value proposition. However, it is important to note that these results reflect the responses of a minimally informed consumer group; we acknowledge that this single-group informed design limits the generalizability of the findings to an uninformed population.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/foods14152664/s1, File S1: Information on by-products; File S2: Table S1. Descriptive statistics of questionnaire items; Table S2. Results of measurement model evaluation—Reliability and validity analysis; Table S3. Cross loadings.

Author Contributions

M.D.C.: Conceptualization, data curation, methodology, investigation, formal analysis, writing—original draft, and writing—reviewing and editing. N.C.: Resources, conceptualization, methodology, and writing—review and editing. A.L.: Visualization and writing—review and editing. F.G.: Resources, conceptualization, project administration, funding acquisition, and writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The co-financing of the European Union—FSE-REACT-EU, PON Research and Innovation 2014–2020 DM1062/2021 is acknowledged. The funding source co-financed the salary of one of the authors.

Informed Consent Statement

Participants gave their consent to participate prior to completing the questionnaire.

Data Availability Statement

Raw data are not deposited in a public repository due to their potential use in subsequent publications and in adherence to the informed consent provided to participants.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to everyone who filled out the questionnaire. Additionally, they express their gratitude to colleagues and friends who helped spread the questionnaire, and to Adele Coppola for her critical reading of the very first draft of the questionnaire. The authors are also grateful to the reviewers who peer-reviewed the manuscript; their observations constituted valuable suggestions to improve the first submitted version of the manuscript. During the preparation of this work, the authors used Google Gemini in order to revise the English of some sentences and improve the readability of the work. After using this tool, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and took full responsibility for the content of the published article.

Conflicts of Interest

Di Cairano Maria reported that financial support was provided by the European Union. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results. The other authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Rakesh, B.; Mahendran, R. Upcycling of food waste and food loss—A sustainable approach in the food sector. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2024, 143, 104274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Thorsen, M.; Mirosa, M.; Skeaff, S.; Goodman-Smith, F.; Bremer, P. Upcycled food: How does it support the three pillars of sustainability? Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2024, 143, 104269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Spratt, O.; Suri, R.; Deutsch, J. Defining Upcycled Food Products. J. Culin. Sci. Technol. 2021, 19, 485–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Asioli, D.; Banovic, M.; Perito, M.A.; Peschel, A.O.; Stancu, V. Defining upcycled food: The dual role of upcycling in reducing food loss and waste. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2023, 132, 132–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lau, K.Q.; Sabran, M.R.; Shafie, S.R. Utilization of Vegetable and Fruit By-products as Functional Ingredient and Food. Front. Nutr. 2021, 8, 661693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Martins, Z.E.; Pinho, O.; Ferreira, I.M.P.L.V.O. Food industry by-products used as functional ingredients of bakery products. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2017, 67, 106–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Allied Market Research Upcycled Food Products Market. Available online: https://www.alliedmarketresearch.com/upcycled-food-products-market-A53592 (accessed on 27 February 2024).
  8. Global Market Insights Upcycled Food Products Market. Available online: https://www.gminsights.com/industry-analysis/upcycled-food-products-market (accessed on 27 February 2024).
  9. Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Stangherlin, I.D.C. Upcycled by-product use in agri-food systems from a consumer perspective: A review of what we know, and what is missing. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 2021, 168, 120749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Perito, M.A.; Coderoni, S.; Russo, C. Consumer attitudes towards local and organic food with upcycled ingredients: An Italian case study for olive leaves. Foods 2020, 9, 1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Grasso, S.; Fu, R.; Goodman-Smith, F.; Lalor, F.; Crofton, E. Consumer attitudes to upcycled foods in US and China. J. Clean. Prod. 2023, 388, 135919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Stelick, A.; Sogari, G.; Rodolfi, M.; Dando, R.; Paciulli, M. Impact of sustainability and nutritional messaging on Italian consumers’ purchase intent of cereal bars made with brewery spent grains. J. Food Sci. 2021, 86, 531–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zhang, J.; Ye, H.; Bhatt, S.; Jeong, H.; Deutsch, J.; Ayaz, H.; Suri, R. Addressing food waste: How to position upcycled foods to different generations. J. Consum. Behav. 2021, 20, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cela, N.; Giorgione, V.; Fassio, F.; Torri, L. Impact of circular economy information on sensory acceptability, purchase intention and perceived value of upcycled foods by young consumers. Food Res. Int. 2024, 175, 113765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Garcia-Valencia, M.; Svartebekk, K.M.; Altintzoglou, T.; Gaarder, M. A Perspective of Upcycled Food by Norwegian Consumers: The Meaning Without the Word. J. Int. Food Agribus. Mark. 2025, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Laureati, M.; Spinelli, S.; Monteleone, E.; Dinnella, C.; Prescott, J.; Cattaneo, C.; Proserpio, C.; De Toffoli, A.; Gasperi, F.; Endrizzi, I.; et al. Associations between food neophobia and responsiveness to “warning” chemosensory sensations in food products in a large population sample. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 68, 113–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pliner, P.; Hobden, K. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1992, 19, 105–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Siraj, A.; Taneja, S.; Zhu, Y.; Jiang, H.; Luthra, S.; Kumar, A. Hey, did you see that label? It’s sustainable!: Understanding the role of sustainable labelling in shaping sustainable purchase behaviour for sustainable development. Bus. Strateg. Environ. 2022, 31, 2820–2838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Difonzo, G.; Grassi, S.; Paciulli, M. Upcycling of Agro-Food Chain By-Products to Obtain High-Value-Added Foods. Foods 2022, 11, 2043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Asioli, D.; Banovic, M.; Perito, M.A.; Peschel, A.O. Communicating upcycled foods: Frugality framing supports acceptance of sustainable product innovations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2022, 100, 104596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nautiyal, S.; Lal, C. Product knowledge as a facilitator of organic purchase intention in emerging markets: Empirical evidence from India. J. Clean. Prod. 2022, 372, 133782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Sesini, G.; Castellini, G.; Iannello, P.; Lombi, L.; Lozza, E.; Lucini, L.; Graffigna, G. Determinants of the willingness to buy products certified by omics technology: Differences between regular and occasional consumers of organic food. Food Res. Int. 2023, 164, 112324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bertolino, M.; Belviso, S.; Dal Bello, B.; Ghirardello, D.; Giordano, M.; Rolle, L.; Gerbi, V.; Zeppa, G. Influence of the addition of different hazelnut skins on the physicochemical, antioxidant, polyphenol and sensory properties of yogurt. LWT 2015, 63, 1145–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Tolve, R.; Simonato, B.; Rainero, G.; Bianchi, F.; Rizzi, C.; Cervini, M.; Giuberti, G. Wheat Bread Fortification by Grape Pomace Powder. Foods 2021, 10, 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Sileoni, V.; Alfeo, V.; Bravi, E.; Belardi, I.; Marconi, O.; Tolve, R.; Simonato, B.; Rainero, G.; Bianchi, F.; Rizzi, C.; et al. Upcycling of a by-product of the brewing production chain as an ingredient in the formulation of functional shortbreads. J. Funct. Foods 2022, 98, 1145–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Carfora, V.; Cavallo, C.; Caso, D.; Del Giudice, T.; De Devitiis, B.; Viscecchia, R.; Nardone, G.; Cicia, G. Explaining consumer purchase behavior for organic milk: Including trust and green self-identity within the theory of planned behavior. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 76, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bhutto, M.Y.; Zeng, F.; Khan, M.A.; Ali, W. Chinese Consumers’ Purchase Intention for Organic Meat: An Extension of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Asian Acad. Manag. J. 2022, 27, 155–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pandey, S.; Ritz, C.; Perez-Cueto, F.J.A. An application of the theory of planned behaviour to predict intention to consume plant-based yogurt alternatives. Foods 2021, 10, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Piha, S.; Pohjanheimo, T.; Lähteenmäki-Uutela, A.; Křečková, Z.; Otterbring, T. The effects of consumer knowledge on the willingness to buy insect food: An exploratory cross-regional study in Northern and Central Europe. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 70, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Schmidt, K. Predicting the consumption of expired food by an extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 78, 103746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Baldissera, C.; Hoppe, A.; Carlini, N.R.B.S.; Sant’Anna, V. Factors influencing consumers’ attitudes towards the consumption of grape pomace powder. Appl. Food Res. 2022, 2, 100103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Asioli, D.; Banovic, M.; Perito, M.A.; Peschel, A.O. Consumer understanding of upcycled foods—Exploring consumer-created associations and concept explanations across five countries. Food Qual. Prefer. 2023, 112, 105033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Asioli, D.; Grasso, S. Do consumers value food products containing upcycled ingredients? The effect of nutritional and environmental information. Food Qual. Prefer. 2021, 91, 104194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Coderoni, S.; Perito, M.A. Approaches for reducing wastes in the agricultural sector. An analysis of Millennials’ willingness to buy food with upcycled ingredients. Waste Manag. 2021, 126, 283–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Dunlap, R.E.; Jones, R.E. Environmental Concern: Conceptual and Measurement Issues. Handb. Environ. Sociol. 2002, 3, 482–524. [Google Scholar]
  36. Shen, Y.C.; Chen, H.S. Exploring consumers’ purchase intention of an innovation of the agri-food industry: A case of artificial meat. Foods 2020, 9, 745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Pagiaslis, A.; Krontalis, A.K. Green Consumption Behavior Antecedents: Environmental Concern, Knowledge, and Beliefs. Psychol. Mark. 2014, 31, 335–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Dangelico, R.M.; Nonino, F.; Pompei, A. Which are the determinants of green purchase behaviour? A study of Italian consumers. Bus. Strateg. Environ. 2021, 30, 2600–2620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. White, K.; Habib, R.; Hardisty, D.J. How to SHIFT consumer behaviors to be more sustainable: A literature review and guiding framework. J. Mark. 2019, 83, 22–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Muiños, G.; Suárez, E.; Hess, S.; Hernández, B. Frugality and psychological wellbeing. The role of voluntary restriction and the resourceful use of resources. Psychology 2015, 6, 169–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gatersleben, B.; Murtagh, N.; Cherry, M.; Watkins, M. Moral, Wasteful, Frugal, or Thrifty? Identifying Consumer Identities to Understand and Manage Pro-Environmental Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 24–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Goldsmith, R.E.; Reinecke Flynn, L.; Clark, R.A. The etiology of the frugal consumer. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2014, 21, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hasheem, M.J.; Wang, S.; Ye, N.; Farooq, M.Z.; Shahid, H.M. Factors influencing purchase intention of solar photovoltaic technology: An extended perspective of technology readiness index and theory of planned behaviour. Clean. Responsible Consum. 2022, 7, 100079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ringle, C.M.; Wende, S.; Becker, J.-M. SmartPLS 4; SmartPLS GmbH: Bönningstedt, Germany, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hair, J.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); SAFE Publications Inc.: Brentford, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  46. Istituto Nazionale di Statistica. Indicatori Demografici: Anno 2022; Istituto Nazionale di Statistica: Rome, Italy, 2023.
  47. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M.; Danks, N.P.; Ray, S. Evaluation of Reflective Measurement Models. In Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) Using R: A Workbook; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2021; pp. 75–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hair, J.F.; Risher, J.J.; Sarstedt, M.; Ringle, C.M. When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. Eur. Bus. Rev. 2019, 31, 2–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Joshi, Y.; Rahman, Z. Factors Affecting Green Purchase Behaviour and Future Research Directions. Int. Strateg. Manag. Rev. 2015, 3, 128–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bernardes, J.P.; Ferreira, F.; Marques, A.D.; Nogueira, M. “Do as I say, not as I do”—A systematic literature review on the attitude-behaviour gap towards sustainable consumption of Generation Y. IOP Conf. Ser. Mater. Sci. Eng. 2018, 459, 012089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kollmuss, A.; Agyeman, J. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 2002, 8, 239–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bhatt, S.; Lee, J.; Deutsch, J.; Ayaz, H.; Fulton, B.; Suri, R. From food waste to value-added surplus products (VASP): Consumer acceptance of a novel food product category. J. Consum. Behav. 2018, 17, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ghazanfar, S.; Abdullah, M.; Ummar, R.; Shabbir, R.; Saqib, S. Effect of Sustainability Claim on Willingness to Pay for Upcycled Food in Digital Era: Differential Effect of Sustainability Claim Between Virtue and Vice Product Category. Front. Environ. Sci. 2022, 10, 870401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Research framework.
Figure 1. Research framework.
Foods 14 02664 g001
Table 1. Questionnaire structure, references, and scales used.
Table 1. Questionnaire structure, references, and scales used.
SectionsItems (n)ScaleReferences
Introduction and informed consent
Food neophobia (FN)107-point Likert[16,17]
Environmental concern (EC)57-point Likert[18]
Green practices (GPs)55-point frequency[19]
Frugality (FRU)37-point Likert[20]
Subjective knowledge (SK)57-point LikertAdapted from [21]
Objective knowledge (OK)6True or false|do not know-
Information on by-products
Attitude (ATT)47-point bipolar adjectives[22]
Purchase intention (PI)17-point Likert-
Willingness to try (WTT) 7-point Likert-
Willingness to buy (WTB) 7-point Likert-
Willingness to pay (WTP) Open-ended question-
Demographics Multiple choice questions
Table 2. Respondents’ profile.
Table 2. Respondents’ profile.
VariableCategoriesFrequencyFrequency (%)
GenderMale18937.43
Female31662.57
Age18–249218.21
25–3418236.04
35–448617.03
45–545310.46
55–647013.86
>65224.36
EmploymentStudent14027.72
Student–worker5811.48
Public employee12624.95
Private employee9518.81
Self-employed458.91
Retired295.74
Unemployed122.38
EducationMiddle school or lower122.38
High school14929.50
Bachelor degree11622.97
Master or equivalent14829.31
PhD–other8015.84
Domicile city inhabitants<500018236.04
5000–49,99912123.96
>50,00020240.00
Table 3. Reliability and validity analysis.
Table 3. Reliability and validity analysis.
Cronbach’s AlphaComposite Reliability (rho_a)Composite Reliability (rho_c)Average Variance Extracted (AVE)
ATT0.9070.9090.9350.782
EC0.7990.8020.8620.556
FRU0.7380.8250.8480.652
GP0.6070.6390.7870.539
FN0.8780.8930.9020.509
OK0.8490.8600.8870.568
SK0.9290.9390.9460.780
ATT, attitude; EC, environmental concern; FRU, frugality; GPs, green practices; FN, food neophobia; OK, objective knowledge; SK, subjective knowledge.
Table 4. Summary of hypothesis testing results.
Table 4. Summary of hypothesis testing results.
PathPath CoefficientStandard DeviationT Statistics p ValuesResults
H1ATT→PI0.6660.03519.2720.000Supported
H2NP→ATT−0.2340.0376.1340.000Supported
H3NP→PI−0.1170.0343.4240.001Supported
H4EC→PI−0.0050.0320.2840.776Not supported
H5GP→PI0.0860.0352.3650.018Supported
H6FRU→PI−0.0060.0360.2670.789Not supported
H7OK→PI0.060.0880.6130.540Not supported
H8OK→ATT0.690.1116.1560.000Supported
H9SK→PI0.030.0420.7950.427Not supported
H10SK→ATT0.0730.0551.330.183Not supported
H11OK→ATT→PI0.2040.0385.470.001Supported
PI, purchase intention; ATT, attitude; NP, food neophobia; EC, environmental concern; GPs, green practices; FRU, frugality; OK, objective knowledge; SK, subjective knowledge.
Table 5. Willingness to try (WTT) and to buy (WTB) for yogurt with hazelnut skin, bread with grape pomace powder, and biscuits with brewers’ spent grains.
Table 5. Willingness to try (WTT) and to buy (WTB) for yogurt with hazelnut skin, bread with grape pomace powder, and biscuits with brewers’ spent grains.
Willingness to TryWillingness to Buy
Yogurt with hazelnut skin5.25 ± 1.80 aA4.81 ± 1.77 aB
Bread with grape pomace powder5.12 ± 1.87 aA4.70 ± 1.85 aB
Biscuits with brewers’ spent grains5.24 ± 1.89 aA4.86 ± 1.89 aB
Different lowercase letters along the column indicate differences among the samples; different capital letters along the row indicate differences in WTT and WTP for the same sample (p < 0.001).
Table 6. Willingness to pay for the proposed products in comparison to a reference price.
Table 6. Willingness to pay for the proposed products in comparison to a reference price.
ProductReference Price * (EUR)Mean WTP (EUR)Δ
Yogurt with hazelnut skin0.951.04 ± 0.48+9.5%
Bread with grape pomace powder3.953.34 ± 1.22−15.4%
Biscuits with brewers’ spent grains4.163.66 ± 1.20−12.0%
* Mean values of conventional product; data were collected from “Osservatorio prezzi al consumo” of the Ministry of Enterprises and Made in Italy (data November 2022).
Table 7. Path analysis of behavioral intentions.
Table 7. Path analysis of behavioral intentions.
ProductPathPath CoefficientStandard DeviationT Statisticsp Values
Yogurt with hazelnut skinPI→WTT0.5360.03913.896<0.001
WTT→WTB0.8440.01751.015<0.001
WTB→WTP0.2260.0703.156<0.001
Bread with grape pomace powderPI→WTT0.5670.03317.070<0.001
WTT→WTB0.8690.01655.829<0.001
WTB→WTP0.4450.04310.315<0.001
Biscuits with brewers’ spent grainsPI→WTT0.5660.03715.407<0.001
WTT→WTB0.8800.01653.594<0.001
WTB→WTP0.4570.04510.154<0.001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Di Cairano, M.; Condelli, N.; Lomonaco, A.; Galgano, F. Agri-Food By-Products as Ingredients: Exploring Purchase Intentions Among a Sample of Italian Consumers. Foods 2025, 14, 2664. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14152664

AMA Style

Di Cairano M, Condelli N, Lomonaco A, Galgano F. Agri-Food By-Products as Ingredients: Exploring Purchase Intentions Among a Sample of Italian Consumers. Foods. 2025; 14(15):2664. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14152664

Chicago/Turabian Style

Di Cairano, Maria, Nicola Condelli, Angela Lomonaco, and Fernanda Galgano. 2025. "Agri-Food By-Products as Ingredients: Exploring Purchase Intentions Among a Sample of Italian Consumers" Foods 14, no. 15: 2664. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14152664

APA Style

Di Cairano, M., Condelli, N., Lomonaco, A., & Galgano, F. (2025). Agri-Food By-Products as Ingredients: Exploring Purchase Intentions Among a Sample of Italian Consumers. Foods, 14(15), 2664. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods14152664

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop