Next Article in Journal
Turkish Adaptation of the Ghosting Questionnaire and Its Impact on Relationship Satisfaction: Serial Mediation Effects of Negative Affect and Loneliness
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of an Educational Intervention on Pupil’s Knowledge, Attitudes, Perceptions, and Behavior on Air Pollution in Public Schools in Pristina
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Influence of Loneliness, Social Support and Income on Mental Well-Being

by
Eider Egaña-Marcos
1,
Ezequiel Collantes
2,
Alina Diez-Solinska
3 and
Garikoiz Azkona
1,*
1
Department of Basic Psychological Processes and Their Development, Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU), Tolosa Hiribidea 70, 20018 Donostia, Spain
2
Department of Architecture, Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU), Oñati Plaza 2, 20018 Donostia, Spain
3
Department of Health Sciences, Universidad Pública de Navarra (UPNA), Campus de Arrosadía, 31006 Pamplona, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15(5), 70; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050070
Submission received: 28 March 2025 / Revised: 26 April 2025 / Accepted: 5 May 2025 / Published: 6 May 2025

Abstract

:
Mental well-being is a multifaceted concept that reflects emotional stability, psychological resilience and social connectedness. This study examines how demographic factors, perceived loneliness, and social support influence mental well-being in Spain. Participants were surveyed online and provided personal information along with responses to the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale, the Medical Outcomes Study Social Support Survey (MOS-SSS), and the Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS). Our findings support previous research on mental well-being in Spain and again show significant associations between income, loneliness, social support and overall mental health. In particular, perceived loneliness was found to be a strong predictor of mental well-being. Furthermore, income and social support were found to partially mediate the relationship between loneliness and mental well-being. These findings highlight the critical role of social connections and financial stability in promoting mental health. Overall, this research contributes to the growing understanding of the factors influencing mental well-being and provides valuable insights for improving mental health outcomes.

1. Introduction

Mental well-being is a complex concept that includes emotional, psychological, and social health. It plays an essential role in an individual’s overall quality of life and significantly influences physical health, functioning, and stress management (Gautam et al., 2024). Mental well-being is not merely the absence of mental disorders but a state of thriving that encompasses emotional balance, psychological resilience, and social connectedness. Emotional health involves the ability to effectively manage and express emotions, while psychological health includes cognitive abilities, self-worth, and a sense of purpose. Social health refers to the ability to build and maintain meaningful relationships and actively engage in community life (Keyes, 2002). Together, these elements contribute to a person’s overall sense of well-being and their capacity to handle life’s challenges.
Over the past decade, the importance of mental well-being has garnered increasing recognition due to its profound impact on both individual and societal health outcomes. As a result, it has become a central topic in public policy, economics, and healthcare, with improving the mental well-being of communities becoming a key societal objective (Ryff, 2014; Steptoe et al., 2015). A wide range of factors, including demographic variables, social context, and access to support systems, influences maintaining mental well-being.
The relationship between demographic characteristics and mental well-being is complex and varies significantly across different populations. For instance, women tend to report lower levels of mental health compared to men (Toribio Caballero et al., 2022). This phenomenon has been attributed to differences in socialization patterns, societal expectations, and the prevalence of gender-based discrimination (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012). These gender disparities are further influenced by cultural and social contexts, with some studies suggesting that women in more egalitarian societies report better mental health outcomes than those in patriarchal settings (Kiely et al., 2019). Similarly, the relationship between age and well-being is nuanced and varies across different cultural and social contexts (Grossmann et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2021; Ryff, 2014; Saadeh et al., 2020). For example, in some cultures, older adults are highly revered and enjoy robust social support networks, which can significantly enhance their mental well-being. In contrast, in societies where aging is stigmatized, older adults may experience increased feelings of loneliness and social isolation, leading to poorer mental health outcomes (Diener et al., 2013; Donovan & Blazer, 2020; Graham et al., 2011). Previous research in Spain has shown that these demographic factors have little or no effect on mental well-being (Castellví et al., 2014; Forero et al., 2014; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021), suggesting that other factors may influence subjective mental well-being.
Recent studies have shown that lower-income working-age adults are particularly susceptible to poor mental health, highlighting the need for targeted interventions to address socioeconomic disparities worldwide (Thomson et al., 2022), and in Spain (Castellví et al., 2014; Macía et al., 2022; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). Individuals with lower incomes and fewer educational opportunities are more likely to experience mental health challenges, including depression and anxiety (Clarke et al., 2000; Keyes et al., 2002). This is often due to the chronic stress associated with financial insecurity, limited access to health care, and exposure to adverse living conditions.
A growing body of research suggests that experiences of loneliness and the availability of social support are key factors influencing mental health outcomes (Wang et al., 2018). Loneliness, often described as the subjective feeling of social isolation, can profoundly affect mental well-being. Unlike objective measures of social isolation, loneliness reflects an individual’s perceived lack of meaningful and satisfying relationships. The mechanisms through which loneliness affects mental well-being are multifaceted. Loneliness can exacerbate existing mental health conditions by amplifying feelings of worthlessness and hopelessness. It can also contribute to the development of new mental health disorders by disrupting sleep patterns, impairing cognitive functioning, and increasing vulnerability to stress (Cacioppo et al., 2010). Furthermore, loneliness often creates a vicious cycle, where individuals who feel lonely are less likely to seek out social interactions, thereby perpetuating their isolation and worsening their mental health. Chronic loneliness is associated with a range of negative psychological outcomes, including depression, anxiety, and increased stress (Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009; Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008; Mann et al., 2022). These effects are not merely transient; prolonged loneliness can lead to significant and lasting damage to mental health, underscoring the importance of addressing this issue in mental health interventions (Cacioppo et al., 2002; Hawkley et al., 2008; Hawkley et al., 2009).
In contrast to loneliness, social support has consistently been identified as a protective factor for mental health, buffering the harmful effects of stress and loneliness (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Uchino, 2006; Zhang & Dong, 2022). Individuals with strong social support networks report higher levels of life satisfaction, greater emotional stability, and improved coping mechanisms in the face of adversity (Gable & Bedrov, 2022; Thoits, 1995). Social support also plays a crucial role in mitigating the negative effects of loneliness. For example, research has shown that the presence of supportive relationships can partially offset the mental health risks associated with loneliness, highlighting the importance of fostering social connections in mental health interventions (Hutten et al., 2021).
The relationship between mental well-being, loneliness, and social support is complex, influenced by individual differences and contextual factors that affect how social support mitigates loneliness-related distress. While social support can alleviate the negative effects of loneliness, its effectiveness depends on the quality and availability of support networks, as well as individual differences in coping styles and personality traits (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Uchino, 2006). Contextual variables also play a crucial role in shaping the interaction between these factors. In collectivist cultures, where community and family ties are highly valued, social support may be more readily available, providing a stronger buffer against loneliness. In contrast, individualistic societies, which emphasize independence and self-reliance, may leave individuals more vulnerable to the adverse effects of loneliness (Barreto et al., 2021; Ryff, 2014; Schönfeld et al., 2017). This paper explores the relationship between mental well-being, demographic factors, loneliness, and social support in Spain, a country with a moderate score (51 out of 100) on the Hofstede individualism-collectivism scale (Chen-Xia et al., 2023).

2. Materials and Methods

Participants were recruited online between July 2023 and January 2025 using a snowball sampling method, where initial respondents were asked to invite others to participate in the survey. The study was limited to individuals aged 18 and older residing in Spain. All participants gave voluntary informed consent before completing the questionnaire, which took approximately 15 min via the Google Drive platform. The study adhered to the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and received approval from the Ethics Committee for Human-Related Research (CEISH) of the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU) under protocol number M10/2023/222.
The survey collected participants’ personal information, including gender, sexual orientation, age (categorized as 18–32, 33–45, 46–60, and over 60), sentimental relationship status (yes/no), household composition (living alone or with others), living area (rural or urban), education (primary school, secondary school, vocational training, undergraduate degree, PhD), employment status (retired, studying, both studying and working, unemployed, or working), and annual salary range (less than €28,000, €28,000–€52,000, or over €52,000). In Spain, the average annual salary in 2022 was 26,948.87 euros (INE, 2024). Loneliness was measured using the Spanish version of the UCLA (University of California, Los Angeles) Loneliness Scale (Velarde-Mayol et al., 2016), which consists of 10 items rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = never; 4 = often), categorized as low (<20), average (20–30), or high (>30). Social support was evaluated using the Spanish version of the Medical Outcomes Study Social Support Survey (MOS-SSS) (Macía et al., 2022), which comprises 19 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always). MOS-SSS scores were classified as low (≤38), average (39–57), or high (≥58). Subjective mental well-being was assessed using a 14-item scale rated on a five-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always) (Castellví et al., 2014). WEMWBS scores were categorized as low (≤40), average (41–58), or high (≥59). Participants were grouped into temporal cohorts (quarterly and semi-annually), and their mental well-being scores (WEMWBS) were compared. No statistically significant differences were found between the groups (p > 0.05). The lack of significant differences in WEMWBS scores across recruitment periods indicates that recruitment timing did not bias participants’ mental well-being outcomes.
All statistical analyses were conducted using Jamovi (version 2.3.21.0, Sydney, Australia) and GraphPad Prism (version 10.3.1, La Jolla, CA, USA), with a significance level set at p < 0.05. Descriptive statistics, including frequencies (%), means ± standard deviations (SD), medians, and ranges, were used to summarize the data. The Shapiro–Wilk test indicated non-parametric distributions for all variables. Consequently, Mann–Whitney U tests were used for comparisons between two groups, while Kruskal–Wallis one-way ANOVA was applied for variables with more than two categories. Effect sizes were calculated using rank biserial correlation (rrb) and Squared-Epsilon coefficient (ε2), with interpretations as follows: rrb < 0.3 (small effect), 0.3–0.5 (moderate effect), >0.5 (large effect); ε2 0.01–<0.06 (small effect), 0.06–<0.14 (moderate effect), ≥0.14 (large effect). Associations between variables were analyzed using bivariate Spearman correlation (rho), with interpretations as follows: <0.29 (small effect), 0.30–0.49 (moderate effect), and >0.50 (large effect). Small effect sizes were not considered. Linear regression analyses were performed to determine the independent influence of the variables. Only significant differences between groups are presented in the results, with small effect sizes noted but not emphasized. The statistical analysis excludes items where participants chose not to respond (prefer not to say), as well as groups with only one or two participants (non-binary; asexual; pansexual).
From this first analysis, we observed that three factors (loneliness, social support, and income) can influence mental well-being. They fulfilled the criteria of being statistically significant in the variance analysis, furthermore, they had at least a medium effect size and correlation coefficient. To determine the influence of these three variables independently, we next performed regression and generalized linear model (GLM) analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Participants’ Information

A total of 506 individuals completed the survey (see Table 1), with the majority identifying as women (71.5%), heterosexual (79.1%), and in the age group 18 to 32 (38.1%). Just over half of the participants were in a relationship (55.3%), and the vast majority lived accompanied at home (83.8%). The majority were working (68.6%) and earned an annual income of less than EUR 28,000 (41.9%). Half of the participants reported experiencing low levels of loneliness (20.1 ± 5.84, median 19, 10–40) and high levels of social support (78.4 ± 14.7, median 82, 26–95).

3.2. Mental Well-Being

The WEMWBS (Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale) scores indicate that slightly more than half of the participants (256/50.6%) reported high levels of well-being, while 5.3% (27) scored low (mean = 57.4 ± 9.5; median = 59; range = 14–70). Supplementary Table S1 summarizes categorized WEMWBS results by participants’ personal information.
Significant differences were observed, with a small effect size, regarding household composition (U = 13,783, p = 0.003, rbb = 0.207). Participants living accompanied (58.1 ± 8.97, median 59, 21–70) scored higher on the WEMBS compared to those living alone (54.1 ± 11.3, median 54, 14–70). The same trend was observed regarding sentimental status (U = 23,661, p < 0.001, rbb = 0.252). Those participants involved in a sentimental relationship score higher (59.3 ± 8.68, median 61.5, 23–70 vs. 55.1 ± 9.98, median 56, 14–70). Similarly, significant differences were observed, with a small effect size, in employment status (X2(4) = 14.5, p = 0.006, ε2 = 0.029). Unemployed individuals (50.8 ± 6.90, median 53, 38–64) scored significantly lower than the university students (56.7 ± 7.98, median 56, 37–70; p = 0.032), working university students (56.8 ± 10.9, median 58, 14–70; p = 0.042), employed (57.9 ± 9.68, median 60, 21–70; p = 0.009) and retirees (59.3 ± 9.03, median 62, 42–70; p = 0.036).
Significant differences were found across salary ranges, with a moderate effect size (X2(4) = 45.3, p < 0.01, ε2 = 0.0930). Participants in the lower salary range had the lowest scores (55.0 ± 9.91, median 56, 21–70), followed by university students who were not working (56.5 ± 7.88, median 56, 37–70), those in the intermediate salary range (59.4 ± 9.38, median 62, 14–70), and, lastly, participants in the higher salary range (64.4 ± 5.71, median 66, 48–70) (Figure 1a). The Spearman correlation revealed a moderate positive relationship between salary ranges and WEMBS (0.256, p < 0.001). Additionally, significant statistical differences with large effect sizes were observed concerning feelings of loneliness (X2(2) = 120, p < 0.01, ε2 = 0.237). Participants who scored highest on the loneliness scale had the lowest WEMBS scores (45.4 ± 10.8, median 42.5, 21–68), which were significantly lower than those who scored the average (54 ± 8.78, median 54, 31–70) and low (61.6 ± 7.51, median 63, 14–70) ranges on the loneliness scale (Figure 1b). The Spearman correlation revealed a large negative correlation between loneliness scores and WEMBS (−0.593, p < 0.001). Finally, significant differences with moderate effect sizes were noted regarding social support (X2(2) = 46.3, p < 0.01, ε2 = 0.0916). Significant differences in WEMBS scores were observed between participants with low (44 ± 6.10, median 43, 34–54) and average (48.4 ± 9.62, median 48.5, 21–64) social support compared to those with high social support (58.4 ± 8.93, median 60, 14–70) (Figure 1c). The Spearman correlation revealed a large positive correlation between social support scores and WEMBS (0.558, p < 0.001). No statistical differences were observed for the other variables described in Table 1. Each mean score for these variables is described in Supplementary Table S2.

3.3. Linear Regression

Previous analyses have shown that income, loneliness, and social support variables influence WEMBS scores. We performed a regression analysis to determine the influence of each of these variables. The model was adjusted for age, gender, education level, and employment status to account for potential confounding effects. The linear regression model yielded that approximately 29.9% of the variability in total WEMBS is explained by the independent variables (F(19,464) = 11.9; p < 0.001; Adj.R2 = 0.299). None of the two-way or three-way interaction terms were statistically significant, suggesting that the effects of loneliness, social support, and income on mental well-being operate independently rather than synergistically (Table 2).
To better understand how loneliness influences mental well-being, we conducted a mediation analysis examining the roles of social support and income. The results revealed a partially complementary mediation pattern: while both mediators contributed to explaining the effect of loneliness on mental health, a significant direct effect of loneliness remained (Table 3 and Figure 2). This indicates that loneliness impacts mental well-being not only through reduced social and economic resources but also through other, unmeasured pathways. Among the two mediators, social support showed a stronger indirect effect than income, highlighting its central role in buffering the negative impact of loneliness. Overall, the findings suggest that both social and economic mechanisms partially account for the detrimental effect of loneliness on mental well-being, without fully explaining it.

4. Discussion

In our study, perceived mental well-being scores closely align with those reported in the Spanish population (Castellví et al., 2014; Forero et al., 2014; Goñi-Balentziaga & Azkona, 2023; López et al., 2013; Macía et al., 2022; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). These findings suggest that, on average, Spanish citizens may experience higher levels of mental well-being than individuals living in several other European countries, such as Denmark (Koushede et al., 2019), France (Trousselard et al., 2016), Italy (Fazia et al., 2020), and the United Kingdom (Forero et al., 2014; Garrett et al., 2023). As we will discuss below, this could be attributed to differences in social factors.
Consistent with previous studies in Spain, we did not observe gender differences (Castellví et al., 2014; Forero et al., 2014; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021), but we were unable to confirm the previously reported negative age-related differences (Castellví et al., 2014; Forero et al., 2014; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). However, it has been noted that the influence of gender and age on mental well-being in the Spanish population is largely attributed to indirect effects, which suggest social inequalities, as other factors mediate their relationship (Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). Concerning the other demographic factors examined in our study, unemployment, lack of a partner, or living alone have a small effect on mental well-being, as previously observed in our country (Castellví et al., 2014; Forero et al., 2014; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). Our findings confirm that, in general, demographic factors have little to no effect on mental well-being in the Spanish population, highlighting that other factors, mainly social ones, play a more substantial role in shaping mental well-being. In this regard, and consistent with previous studies, we found that higher-income individuals scored significantly higher on mental well-being measures (Castellví et al., 2014; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). This association could mean that being wealthier may provide individuals with greater access to resources such as psychotherapy, leisure activities, and reduced financial stress, all of which contribute to improved mental well-being, or that better mental well-being allows greater opportunities for income generation. The relationship between income and mental well-being observed in our study provides further insight into the role of socioeconomic factors in mental health.
Our findings confirm that perceived loneliness appears to reduce overall mental well-being, which is consistent with previous research highlighting the detrimental effects of loneliness on mental health (Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009; Cacioppo et al., 2002; Lyyra et al., 2021; Mushtaq et al., 2014). Conversely, our results show that perceived social support has a positive effect on mental well-being. These results are consistent with the widely held belief that social support greatly enhances happiness (Veenhoven, 2015). Social support acts as a buffer against the detrimental effects of loneliness, significantly enhancing mental well-being. This is consistent with a large body of literature highlighting the protective role of social support in alleviating stress and promoting psychological well-being (Bruss et al., 2024; Cohen & Wills, 1985; Davies et al., 2024; Ginja et al., 2018; Hombrados-Mendieta et al., 2019; Jackman et al., 2023; Loayza-Rivas & Fernández-Castro, 2020; Macía et al., 2022; Soldevila-Domenech et al., 2021). Individuals who perceive themselves as having strong social networks and reliable support systems are better equipped to cope with life’s challenges, enhancing their mental well-being. Thus, the Spanish population may enjoy higher levels of mental well-being compared to the above-mentioned European countries due to higher levels of social support reported (Fernández-Ballesteros, 2002; Salinero-Fort et al., 2011).
One limitation of this study is its reliance on self-reported measures, which may be subject to bias. Additionally, the sample was predominantly female, a trend consistent with many of the studies referenced in the literature. Future research should aim for more balanced gender representation to improve generalizability and provide a broader perspective. Furthermore, the cross-sectional design of this study prevents the establishment of causal relationships between the variables examined. Although instrumental variable (IV) regression is a valuable tool for addressing endogeneity, the current dataset lacks suitable instruments for this approach. Consequently, the findings are framed as correlational and exploratory rather than causal. To better understand the directionality of effects and potential reciprocal relationships, future studies should employ longitudinal or experimental designs.
Despite these limitations, our data confirms the importance of social factors over demographic ones in mental well-being. The interaction between these variables suggests that mental well-being is influenced by a combination of social and economic factors. For example, while higher income may provide individuals with more opportunities to engage in social activities and build supportive networks, the absence of such networks may still lead to feelings of loneliness and reduced well-being.

5. Conclusions

Our findings underscore the significant influence of perceived loneliness on mental well-being, with perceived social support and income serving as important mediators. These results emphasize the intricate relationship between individual experiences of loneliness, the availability of social support, financial stability, and overall mental well-being.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ejihpe15050070/s1, Table S1: Categorized WEMWBS results based on participants’ personal information; Table S2: WEMWBS score results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and methodology, G.A.; formal analysis, E.E.-M., E.C., A.D.-S. and G.A.; writing—original draft preparation, E.E.-M., E.C., A.D.-S. and G.A.; writing—review and editing, G.A.; supervision, G.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU) M10/2023/222.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The study data will be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all respondents for participating in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
MOS-SSSMedical Outcomes Study Social Support Survey
SDStandard Deviation
UCLAUniversity of California, Los Angeles
WEMWBSWarwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale

References

  1. Barreto, M., Victor, C., Hammond, C., Eccles, A., Richins, M. T., & Qualter, P. (2021). Loneliness around the world: Age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences, 169, 110066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bruss, K. V., Seth, P., & Zhao, G. (2024). Loneliness, lack of social and emotional support, and mental health issues—United States, 2022. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 73, 539–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., Kowalewski, R. B., Malarkey, W. B., Van Cauter, E., & Berntson, G. G. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(3), 407–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 453–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection. W W Norton & Co. [Google Scholar]
  7. Castellví, P., Forero, C. G., Codony, M., Vilagut, G., Brugulat, P., Medina, A., Gabilondo, A., Mompart, A., Colom, J., Tresserras, R., Ferrer, M., Stewart-Brown, S., & Alonso, J. (2014). The Spanish version of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS) is valid for use in the general population. Quality of Life Research, 23(3), 857–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chen-Xia, X. J., Betancor, V., Rodríguez-Gómez, L., & Rodríguez-Pérez, A. (2023). Cultural variations in perceptions and reactions to social norm transgressions: A comparative study. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1243955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Clarke, P. J., Marshall, V. W., Ryff, C. D., & Rosenthal, C. J. (2000). Well being in Canadian seniors: Findings from the Canadian study of health and aging. Canadian Journal on Aging, 19(2), 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 310–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Davies, R. M., Knoll, M. A., & Kyranides, M. N. (2024). A moderated mediation analysis on the influence of social support and cognitive flexibility in predicting mental wellbeing in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 70, 102560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Ryan, K. L. (2013). Universals and cultural differences in the causes and structure of happiness: A multilevel review. In Mental well-being: International contributions to the study of positive mental health (pp. 153–176). Springer Science + Business Media. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Donovan, N. J., & Blazer, D. (2020). Social isolation and loneliness in older adults: Review and commentary of a national academies report. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(12), 1233–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fazia, T., Bubbico, F., Salvato, G., Berzuini, G., Bruno, S., Bottini, G., & Bernardinelli, L. (2020). Boosting psychological well-being through a social mindfulness-based intervention in the general population. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(22), 8404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Fernández-Ballesteros, R. (2002). Social support and quality of life among older people in Spain. Journal of Social Issues, 58(4), 645–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Forero, C. G., Adroher, N. D., Stewart-Brown, S., Castellví, P., Codony, M., Vilagut, G., Mompart, A., Tresseres, R., Colom, J., Castro, J. I., & Alonso, J. (2014). Differential item and test functioning methodology indicated that item response bias was not a substantial cause of country differences in mental well-being. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 67(12), 1364–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gable, S. L., & Bedrov, A. (2022). Social isolation and social support in good times and bad times. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 89–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Garrett, J. K., Rowney, F. M., White, M. P., Lovell, R., Fry, R. J., Akbari, A., Geary, R., Lyons, R. A., Mizen, A., Nieuwenhuijsen, M., Parker, C., Song, J., Stratton, G., Thompson, D. A., Watkins, A., White, J., Williams, S. A., Rodgers, S. E., & Wheeler, B. W. (2023). Visiting nature is associated with lower socioeconomic inequalities in well-being in Wales. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 9684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gautam, S., Jain, A., Chaudhary, J., Gautam, M., Gaur, M., & Grover, S. (2024). Concept of mental health and mental well-being, it’s determinants and coping strategies. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 66(Suppl. S2), S231–S244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ginja, S., Coad, J., Bailey, E., Kendall, S., Goodenough, T., Nightingale, S., Smiddy, J., Day, C., Deave, T., & Lingam, R. (2018). Associations between social support, mental wellbeing, self-efficacy and technology use in first-time antenatal women: Data from the BaBBLeS cohort study. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 18(1), 441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Goñi-Balentziaga, O., & Azkona, G. (2023). Perceived professional quality of life and mental well-being among animal facility personnel in Spain. Laboratory Animals, 58(1), 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Graham, C., Higuera, L., & Lora, E. (2011). Which health conditions cause the most unhappiness? Health Economics, 20(12), 1431–1447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Grossmann, I., Karasawa, M., Kan, C., & Kitayama, S. (2014). A cultural perspective on emotional experiences across the life span. Emotion, 14(4), 679–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, R. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63(6), S375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Hawkley, L. C., Thisted, R. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: Cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychology, 28(3), 354–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Hombrados-Mendieta, I., Millán-Franco, M., Gómez-Jacinto, L., Gonzalez-Castro, F., Martos-Méndez, M. J., & García-Cid, A. (2019). Positive influences of social support on sense of community, life satisfaction and the health of immigrants in Spain. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hutten, E., Jongen, E. M. M., Vos, A. E. C. C., van den Hout, A. J. H. C., & van Lankveld, J. J. D. M. (2021). Loneliness and mental health: The mediating effect of perceived social support. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(22), 11963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. INE. (2024). Encuesta de Estructura Salarial (EES). Año 2022. Datos definitivos. Available online: https://www.ine.es/dyngs/INEbase/es/operacion.htm?c=Estadistica_C&cid=1254736177025&menu=ultiDatos&idp=1254735976596#:~:text=%C3%9Altima%20Nota%20de%20prensa&text=El%20salario%20medio%20anual%20fue,que%20el%20del%20a%C3%B1o%20anterior (accessed on 13 February 2025).
  29. Jackman, P. C., Slater, M. J., Carter, E. E., Sisson, K., & Bird, M. D. (2023). Social support, social identification, mental wellbeing, and psychological distress in doctoral students: A person-centred analysis. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 47(1), 45–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 43(2), 207–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Keyes, C. L. M., Shmotkin, D., & Ryff, C. D. (2002). Optimizing well-being: The empirical encounter of two traditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 1007–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kiely, K. M., Brady, B., & Byles, J. (2019). Gender, mental health and ageing. Maturitas, 129, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kim, E. S., Tkatch, R., Martin, D., MacLeod, S., Sandy, L., & Yeh, C. (2021). Resilient aging: Psychological well-being and social well-being as targets for the promotion of healthy aging. Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, 7, 23337214211002951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Koushede, V., Lasgaard, M., Hinrichsen, C., Meilstrup, C., Nielsen, L., Rayce, S. B., Torres-Sahli, M., Gudmundsdottir, D. G., Stewart-Brown, S., & Santini, Z. I. (2019). Measuring mental well-being in Denmark: Validation of the original and short version of the Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS and SWEMWBS) and cross-cultural comparison across four European settings. Psychiatry Research, 271, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Loayza-Rivas, J., & Fernández-Castro, J. (2020). Perceived stress and well-being: The role of social support as a protective factor among Peruvian immigrants in Spain. Ansiedad y Estrés, 26(2), 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. López, M. A., Gabilondo, A., Codony, M., García-Forero, C., Vilagut, G., Castellví, P., Ferrer, M., & Alonso, J. (2013). Adaptation into Spanish of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS) and preliminary validation in a student sample. Quality of Life Research, 22(5), 1099–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lyyra, N., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Eriksson, C., Madsen, K. R., Tolvanen, A., Löfstedt, P., & Välimaa, R. (2021). The association between loneliness, mental well-being, and self-esteem among Adolescents in Four Nordic Countries. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(14), 7405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Macía, P., Goñi-Balentziaga, O., Vegas, O., & Azkona, G. (2022). Professional quality of life among Spanish veterinarians. Veterinary Record Open, 9(1), e250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mann, F., Wang, J., Pearce, E., Ma, R., Schlief, M., Lloyd-Evans, B., Ikhtabi, S., & Johnson, S. (2022). Loneliness and the onset of new mental health problems in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57(11), 2161–2178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mushtaq, R., Shoib, S., Shah, T., & Mushtaq, S. (2014). Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 8(9), WE01–WE04. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 161–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ryff, C. D. (2014). Psychological well-being revisited: Advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83(1), 10–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Saadeh, M., Welmer, A. K., Dekhtyar, S., Fratiglioni, L., & Calderón-Larrañaga, A. (2020). The role of psychological and social well-being on physical function trajectories in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 75(8), 1579–1585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Salinero-Fort, M., del Otero-Sanz, L., Martín-Madrazo, C., de Burgos-Lunar, C., Chico-Moraleja, R. M., Rodés-Soldevila, B., Jiménez-García, R., Gómez-Campelo, P., & HEALTH & MIGRATION Group. (2011). The relationship between social support and self-reported health status in immigrants: An adjusted analysis in the Madrid Cross Sectional Study. BMC Family Practice, 12, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Schönfeld, P., Brailovskaia, J., & Margraf, J. (2017). Positive and negative mental health across the lifespan: A cross-cultural comparison. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 17(3), 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Soldevila-Domenech, N., Forero, C. G., Alayo, I., Capella, J., Colom, J., Malmusi, D., Mompart, A., Mortier, P., Puértolas, B., Sánchez, N., Schiaffino, A., Vilagut, G., & Alonso, J. (2021). Mental well-being of the general population: Direct and indirect effects of socioeconomic, relational and health factors. Quality of Life Research, 30(8), 2171–2185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Steptoe, A., Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2015). Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet, 385(9968), 640–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Thoits, P. A. (1995). Stress, coping, and social support processes: Where are we? What next? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 53–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Thomson, R. M., Igelström, E., Purba, A. K., Shimonovich, M., Thomson, H., McCartney, G., Reeves, A., Leyland, A., Pearce, A., & Katikireddi, S. V. (2022). How do income changes impact on mental health and wellbeing for working-age adults? A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 7(6), e515–e528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Toribio Caballero, S., Cardenal Hernáez, V., Ávila Espada, A., & Ovejero Bruna, M. M. (2022). Gender roles and women’s mental health: Their influence on the demand for psychological care. Annals of Psychology, 38(1), 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Trousselard, M., Steiler, D., Dutheil, F., Claverie, D., Canini, F., Fenouillet, F., Naughton, G., Stewart-Brown, S., & Franck, N. (2016). Validation of the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS) in French psychiatric and general populations. Psychiatry Research, 245, 282–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Uchino, B. N. (2006). Social support and health: A review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29(4), 377–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Veenhoven, R. (2015). Social conditions for human happiness: A review of research. International Journal of Psychology, 50(5), 379–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Velarde-Mayol, C., Fragua-Gil, S., & García-de-Cecilia, J. M. (2016). Validación de la escala de soledad de UCLA y perfil social en la población anciana que vive sola. Medicina de Familia. SEMERGEN, 42(3), 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Wang, J., Mann, F., Lloyd-Evans, B., Ma, R., & Johnson, S. (2018). Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 18(1), 156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Zhang, X., & Dong, S. (2022). The relationships between social support and loneliness: A meta-analysis and review. Acta Psychologica, 227, 103616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. WEMWBS score results by (a) salary range, (b) UCLA loneliness scale, and (c) MOS-SSS scores. Data are presented as group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 1. WEMWBS score results by (a) salary range, (b) UCLA loneliness scale, and (c) MOS-SSS scores. Data are presented as group median (min to max). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and **** p < 0.0001.
Ejihpe 15 00070 g001
Figure 2. Mediation of the relationship between loneliness and mental well-being, being social support and income mediator. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Mediation of the relationship between loneliness and mental well-being, being social support and income mediator. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ejihpe 15 00070 g002
Table 1. Participant’s personal information.
Table 1. Participant’s personal information.
n (%)
Gender
Female362 (71.5%)
Male140 (27.7%)
Non-binary1 (0.2%)
Prefer not to say3 (0.6%)
Sexual orientation
Asexual1 (0.2%)
Bisexual47 (9.3%)
Heterosexual400 (79.1%)
Homosexual43 (8.5%)
Pansexual2 (0.4%)
Prefer not to say13 (2.6%)
Age
18–32193 (38.1%)
33–45139 (26.9%)
46–60128 (25.3%)
Over 6049 (9.7%)
Sentimental relationship
No226 (44.7%)
Yes280 (55.3%)
Household composition
Living accompanied424 (83.8%)
Living alone82 (16.2%)
Living area
Rural91 (18%)
Urban415 (82%)
Education
Primary school12 (2.4%)
Secondary school58 (11.5%)
Vocational training62 (12.3%)
Undergraduate degree318 (62.8%)
PhD56 (11.1%)
Employment status
Employed 347 (68.6%)
Retired25 (4.9%)
Unemployed17 (3.4%)
University student78 (15.4%)
Working and studying at the University38 (7.7%)
Salary range (EUR/year)
Prefer not to say18 (3.6%)
Students not working81 (16%)
<28,000212 (41.9%)
28,000–<52,000165 (32.6%)
≥52,00030 (5.9%)
Loneliness
Low256 (50.6%)
Average222 (43.9%)
High28 (5.5%)
Social support
Low9 (1.8%)
Average44 (8.7%)
High453 (89.5%)
Table 2. Results of the linear regression analysis for mental well-being involving predictor variables of loneliness, social support, and income. Reference level: low, <€28,000, male, 18–32, primary school, employed. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Results of the linear regression analysis for mental well-being involving predictor variables of loneliness, social support, and income. Reference level: low, <€28,000, male, 18–32, primary school, employed. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
β95% CItp
Loneliness
Average−0.686−0.84–−0.52−8.38<0.001 ***
High−1.424−1.78–−1.06−7.75<0.001 ***
Social support
Average0.097−0.52–0.700.310.754
High0.5890.02–1.152.040.025 *
Income
Students not working0.085−0.16–0.330.680.495
€28,000–€52,0000.3680.17–0.563.72<0.001 ***
€52,0000.6800.33–1.023.88<0.001 ***
Gender−0.054−0.23–0.12−0.610.542
Age
33–45−0.071−0.29–0.15−0.630.527
46–60−0.047−0.28–0.18−0.400.689
Over 60−0.248−0.55–0.05−1.600.109
Education
Secondary school−0.17−0.71–0.38−0.610.540
Vocational training−0.11−0.64–0.42−0.420.670
Undergraduate degree−0.10−0.60–0.40−0.400.683
PhD−0.07−0.62–0.47−0.280.779
Employment status
Retired0.39−0.06–0.851.700.088
Unemployed0.03−0.44–0.510.140.889
University student0.17−0.38–0.730.620.532
Working and studying at the University−0.06−0.38–0.26−0.390.695
Loneliness * Social Support0.047−0.01–0.02-0.600.545
Loneliness * Income−0.004−0.82–0.75−0.080.932
Social Support * Income−0.046−0.25–0.17−0.390.691
Loneliness * Social Support * Income0.002−0.009–0.0090.060.947
Table 3. Direct and indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals for the mediation analyses. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Direct and indirect effects and 95% confidence intervals for the mediation analyses. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Pathwayβ95% CIzp
Direct effect
Loneliness → Mental Well-being−0.386−0.77–−0.49−9.07<0.001 ***
Indirect effects
Loneliness → Social Support → Mental Well-being−0.173−0.36–−0.20−6.68<0.001 ***
Loneliness → Income → Mental Well-being−0.024−0.07–−0.02−2.700.007 **
Total−0.582−1.06–0.83−16.09<0.001 ***
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Egaña-Marcos, E.; Collantes, E.; Diez-Solinska, A.; Azkona, G. The Influence of Loneliness, Social Support and Income on Mental Well-Being. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15, 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050070

AMA Style

Egaña-Marcos E, Collantes E, Diez-Solinska A, Azkona G. The Influence of Loneliness, Social Support and Income on Mental Well-Being. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2025; 15(5):70. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050070

Chicago/Turabian Style

Egaña-Marcos, Eider, Ezequiel Collantes, Alina Diez-Solinska, and Garikoiz Azkona. 2025. "The Influence of Loneliness, Social Support and Income on Mental Well-Being" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 15, no. 5: 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050070

APA Style

Egaña-Marcos, E., Collantes, E., Diez-Solinska, A., & Azkona, G. (2025). The Influence of Loneliness, Social Support and Income on Mental Well-Being. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 15(5), 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15050070

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop