Next Article in Journal
Portuguese Workers of Private Institutions of Social Solidarity and Affective Job Satisfaction: An Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Cyber Child-to-Parent Violence and Child-to-Parent Violence: Bidirectional Trajectories and Associated Longitudinal Risk Factors
Previous Article in Special Issue
Deficits in Duration Estimation in Individuals Aged 10–20 Years Old with Idiopathic Mild Intellectual Disability: The Role of Inhibition, Shifting, and Processing Speed
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Time Perspective Profile and Study Engagement

by
Zara-Anna Mathieu
1,
Emilie Dujardin
1,
Nicolas Noiret
1,
Rébecca Shankland
2 and
Marie-Amélie Martinie
1,*
1
CeRCA (Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition et l’Apprentissage), Université de Poitiers, Université de Tours, CNRS (Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique), 86000 Poitiers, France
2
DIPHE (Développement Individu Processus Handicap Education), Université Lyon 2, 69676 Lyon, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15(10), 191; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15100191
Submission received: 25 June 2025 / Revised: 10 September 2025 / Accepted: 15 September 2025 / Published: 23 September 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Subjective Time: Cognition, Emotion and Beyond)

Abstract

Academic dropout in French universities is significant. The lack of study engagement partly explains this phenomenon. Pursuing academic studies requires switching effectively among temporal orientations (past, present, and future). Although the relationships between study engagement and each temporal orientation have been studied, to the best of our knowledge, the association of all temporal profiles (present in all individuals) has not been considered in the relationship with study engagement. This study aimed to address this gap in the literature. In total, 451 French first- and second-year students enrolled in the humanities and social sciences Bachelor’s program completed a questionnaire including scales measuring time perspectives and study engagement. Using latent profile analyse, we obtained five profiles. We considered three of these as problematic profiles, including 40% of the students, and two had no problematic profiles. Among the latter, there is one in which 26% of the students are relatively oriented toward all temporal dimensions, and one balanced profile including 33% of the students. As expected, the balanced time perspective profile presented the highest study engagement scores, unlike past negative profiles, which showed lower scores. We discuss the implications of this new result for student academic success.

1. Introduction

The proportion of students who leave French universities without completing their first year is estimated at 31.8% (Ministère de l’Enseignement Supérieur et de la Recherche, 2020). Academic dropout is a complex and multifactorial phenomenon (e.g., parents’ social background, level of study, or working alongside studies; (Zaffran & Aigle, 2020) with one potential contributing factor being a lack of study engagement (Archambault et al., 2009). Enhancing study engagement, therefore, appears to be a relevant strategy to reduce academic dropout. Moreover, pursuing academic studies requires keeping in mind future goals, often for a very long term, while completing tasks in the present that may be unpleasant or boring in order to achieve the longer-term goals.
The relation between temporal orientations (i.e., past, present, and future) and study engagement has been mainly investigated using Zimbardo’s Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI) (Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999). The ZTPI is designed to capture individual differences in time perception. This questionnaire distinguishes five temporal orientations: past positive (i.e., a positive view of the past, a warm and sentimental view of the past); past negative (i.e., a negative and aversive view of the past); present hedonistic (i.e., a focus on immediate pleasure with little consideration of the future); present fatalistic (i.e., an attitude of helplessness and a feeling of limited control over one’s life), and future (i.e., striving for future rewards and goals). Some temporal orientations are more desirable than others. For instance, present hedonist, present fatalist, and past negative are positively related to alcohol consumption, while a future orientation is negatively linked with alcohol consumption (Daugherty & Brase, 2010; Henson et al., 2006; Linden et al., 2014). A positive association has been observed between future time orientation and study engagement (Denovan et al., 2020; Hong, 2025; Liu et al., 2023; Peng & Zhang, 2022), suggesting that future time orientation could serve to detect or predict study engagement. Nevertheless, these studies did not consider the dynamic between past, present, and future, while time perspective is a continuous cognitive process that allocates attentional resources across different temporal perspectives in response to situational demands (Zimbardo & Boyd, 2008). To the best of our knowledge, the relationship between study engagement and the interplay of the past, present, and future has not yet been investigated. To this end, two objectives were set. First, we aimed to identify the temporal perspective of French students by considering the interplay between these perspectives using latent profile analyses. Secondly, we sought to examine the relationship between these profiles and study engagement, with the aim of identifying the profile that best predicts study engagement. Identifying the different latent profiles could be useful to detect the students who most need support during their studies.

1.1. Study Engagement and Time Perspective

Study engagement refers to a “positive, fulfilling, work-related state of mind that is characterized by vigour, dedication, and absorption in the activity” (Schaufeli et al., 2002, p. 72). High levels of energy and resilience during one’s studies refer to vigor, high inspiration and commitment refer to dedication, and being fully concentrated on and immersed in one’s study activities refers to absorption. Engagement encompasses three dimensions: emotional, behavioral, and cognitive. Positive emotional reactions to the academic study refers to the emotional dimension (Sinatra et al., 2015); the involvement in academic effort and persistence during the planning and attainment of learning activities refers to the behavioral dimension (Skinner et al., 2008), and the implication in learning and use of deep learning strategies refers to the cognitive dimension of study engagement (Xu et al., 2020). Study engagement is negatively linked to academic dropout and positively associated with academic success (Çali et al., 2024; Wang & Eccles, 2012), and well-being (Chaudhry et al., 2024; Denovan et al., 2020; Nolberto-Quispe et al., 2021). In sum, study engagement could be considered as a protective factor for students.
In academic settings, future time orientation appears to be particularly relevant for optimal academic outcomes, because it “provides the motivational resource needed to attain future goals” (Denovan et al., 2020, p. 1536). Indeed, being future-oriented is positively associated with self-regulated learning (de Bilde et al., 2011; Denovan et al., 2020), academic performance (Hong, 2025; Hong & Hanafi, 2024), school grades (Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999), and study engagement (Denovan et al., 2020; Haririzade et al., 2023; Hong, 2025; Liu et al., 2023; Peetsma, 2000; Peng & Zhang, 2022). These correlational studies are informative. Nevertheless, investigating only one specific temporal orientation or each of them independently is too restrictive. Indeed, in the same situation, individuals are not exclusively focused on one specific temporal orientation. In other terms, these correlational studies do not consider the interplay of the different time orientations, which are called time perspectives (TPs).
TP refers to the interplay between three temporal orientations present in individuals: past, present, and future. For Zimbardo and Boyd (1999, 2015), the psychological construction of the past and the anticipation of future events shape the concrete and practical representation of the present. TP is a pervasive filter that serves encoding, sorting, and recalling events to form expectations and objectives, and to construct scenarios. Therefore, it impacts behaviors and attitudes and may influence achievement, goal setting, and well-being. It is considered both as a disposition, when individuals have a habitual tendency to focus on one specific orientation, and as a state, when they focus on one orientation depending on situation features (Stolarski et al., 2018).

1.2. Balanced Time Perspective

Individuals can simultaneously manage all temporal orientations to varying degrees and switch from one to the other depending on personal resources and situational context. To achieve the most adaptive outcomes, Zimbardo and Boyd (1999) suggested that individuals should score high on past positive and future, while simultaneously scoring low on present hedonistic, present fatalist, and past negative. This profile would be optimal and corresponds to a balanced time perspective (BTP).
To operationalize BTP, four methods have been proposed. First, the cut-off scores method was used (Drake et al., 2008), which involves categorizing the five TP into low and moderate or high scores (i.e., below versus above the 33rd percentile). The second method consists of using cluster analyses that permit the identification of profiles. The third method consists of measuring the extent to which an individual’s ZTPI score profile deviates from an optimal score pattern associated with a BTP (Stolarski et al., 2011). It is widely used to operationalize BTP (for a review of research, Stolarski et al., 2020). The optimal score values are justified by Stolarski et al. (2011) as follows: “Following Zimbardo and Boyd’s proposal www.thetimeparadox.com/surveys (accessed on 25 March 2025) and based on Zimbardo and Boyd’s collective cross-cultural database, we defined a ‘high’ score on past positive as 4.60, a ‘moderately high’ score on present hedonism and future as 3.90 and 4.00 respectively, and ‘low’ score on past negative and present fatalism as 1.95 and 1.50 respectively” (p. 354). The smaller the DBTP value—reflecting minimal deviation from optimal profile—the closer an individual’s score profile is to BTP. Finally, a little-used method consists of establishing latent profiles. Latent profile analysis (LPA) is a categorical latent variable modeling approach that is used to identify a number of profiles in a sample and estimate the probability of membership of each profile by analyzing different configurations of several variables (see Spurk et al., 2020).
Some authors (Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999; Boniwell & Zimbardo, 2004) suggested that individuals with BTP should show the most positive outcomes. Regarding mental health, this hypothesis has been tested with different methods to operationalize BTP. It has been observed that with the cut-off scores, a BTP is crucial to have high levels of well-being; participants with moderate or high BTP scores reported more positive states than those with low BTP scores (Drake et al., 2008). With the DBTP value, the lesser individuals deviate from the BTP profile, the higher their level of well-being (Fuentes et al., 2022; for a review of research, see Stolarski et al., 2020), and greater resilience regarding mental health problems such as anxiety, stress, and depression (Griffin & Wildbur, 2020). Also, with latent profile analyses (LPA), the balanced latent profile was linked with less drinking behavior (Braitman & Henson, 2015). Recently, Kossewska et al. (2023) observed that individuals who scored high on future, present hedonistic, past positive, and low on present fatalistic and past negative reported the least educational, social, and mental health problems. This latent profile is not completely balanced. Nevertheless, it was obtained in a specific context. In fact, the study was carried out during the COVID-19 pandemic. In this context, in which death was salient, it is not surprising to observe that students were more oriented towards a hedonistic present. Taken together, these results support the view of Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Nevertheless, with cluster analyses, some results do not support this view. For instance, it has been reported that adolescents with a future profile displayed the most positive outcomes (i.e., problematic drinking behaviors) (McKay et al., 2014), while students with a balanced profile displayed the most positive outcomes (i.e., well-being). These differences in results can be explained by differences in populations (adolescents or students) and differences in outcomes (problematic drinking behaviors or well-being) (Boniwell et al., 2010). Moreover, the cut-off-scores method, cluster analyses, and DBPT formula have been compared. It has been observed that DBTP was the best predictor of subjective well-being (Zhang et al., 2013).
Although in the field of mental health, the DBPT formula is the most widely used, it is methodologically highly questionable. Indeed, the values of optimal scores have been established on the collective cross-cultural database without demographic information provided, while the age of participants is a crucial element to consider. In fact, TP is a psychological process that develops throughout life (Steinberg et al., 2009) and, for instance, for teenagers, it is influenced by interactions with significant others such as parents or teachers (Luyckx et al., 2010). Jankowski et al. (2020) point out that the values of optimal scores are identical for each individual and are established arbitrarily. In fact, the values correspond to the percentile distribution (10th, 80th, or 90th) from the large cross-cultural database of Zimbardo and Boyd (2008). Moreover, in some studies, TP subscales were more linked with psychological outcomes than DBTP (see, for instance, Stolarski, 2016). This calls into question the usefulness of DBTP. Furthermore, the cut-off scores are methodologically highly questionable. More precisely, the cut-off criteria were selected arbitrarily, and nothing permits us to conclude that they are optimal (Boniwell et al., 2010). Moreover, concerning cluster analyses, one of its limits is that studies provide differences in temporal profiles depending on the country. For instance, a profile similar to the BTP has been observed among an Australian sample and not among British, American, and Slovenian samples (McKay et al., 2018). Moreover, Sircova et al. (2015) observed a balanced profile in 24 countries (17 from Europe, 3 from Asia and America, and 1 from Africa), with the prevalence of a balanced profile for some countries, such as Estonia and Israel, and a prevalence of a present-orientation profile for a French sample.
In light of the various criticisms of the DBTP formula and the cut-off scores method, the latent profile analyses seem to be the most relevant (see Spurk et al., 2020). Moreover, comparatively to cluster analysis, latent profile analyses offer several methodological advantages. For instance, latent profiles assign individuals to profiles based on probabilities, while cluster analyses assign each individual to a single cluster, without uncertainty. So, latent profiles allow for soft classification, which better reflects real-world ambiguity in group membership. Moreover, latent profiles are based on underlying statistical models (e.g., mixed models), enabling inference, hypothesis testing, and model fit assessment, while clusters rely on distance-based algorithms with no statistical model behind them. With latent profile analyses, statistical indices like AIC/BIC can be used to choose the number of latent profiles that can include measurement error in the modeling process, while clusters treat the data as exact and noise-free.

1.3. Purpose of This Research

In the present research, we investigated the relation between the dynamic process of temporal perception and study engagement among French students. To the best of our knowledge, this relationship has not yet been investigated in any population. Given that study engagement promotes positive outcomes such as academic achievement (Lei et al., 2018; Serrano & Andreu, 2016) and well-being (Chaudhry et al., 2024; Denovan et al., 2020; Nolberto-Quispe et al., 2021), and that the BTP should lead to the most positive outcomes (Zimbardo & Boyd, 2008), we hypothesized that the balanced latent profile would be best associated with study engagement. To test this hypothesis, we 1/identified among French students the different TP latent profiles, 2/investigated the relation between TP latent profiles and study engagement.

2. Method

2.1. Ethics Approval

This research received no funding. It was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local university’s Ethics Committee (CER-TP 2024-02-05). Written consent was obtained from all participants. The data are publicly available on the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/nxvty/?view_only=1e68ed2df202439c84d75e9eee5a3acb (accessed on 8 September 2025).

2.2. Participants

The study was conducted in a French university among first- and second-year students in the humanities and social sciences Bachelor’s program (psychology). The sample total was composed of 451 participants (412 females). The participants were all native French speakers and aged 18 or older (Mage = 19.2, SEage = 2.71).

2.3. Procedure

The participants took part in this study in exchange for course credits. The study was performed in a lab room in collective sessions with 8 participants at a time. Each participant was seated in front of a computer, 2 m apart, and completed a computerized questionnaire elaborately programmed on OpenSemame 0.24 (Mathôt et al., 2012). In this questionnaire, the study engagement and time perspective scales were counterbalanced across participants.

2.4. Measures

2.4.1. Study Engagement

The Study Engagement Scale (Salmela-Aro & Upadaya, 2012) is composed of 9 items. Three items assess absorption (e.g., “Time flies when I’m studying”), three items vigor (e.g., “When I’m studying, I feel mentally strong”), and three items dedication (e.g., “My studies inspire me”). All items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true for me) to 5 (totally true for me). The plausibility of the overall model was assessed based on several goodness-of-fit measures, such as Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), the Root-Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Squared (SRMS) with a 90% confidence interval. Guidelines for values indicative of good fit are CFI and TLI equal or greater than 0.90, and RMSEA and SRMR less than or equal to 0.08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). With the 3 dimensions of study engagement (i.e., absorption, vigor, and dedication), the goodness of fit indices indicated that the model had no satisfactory fit: CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.10, SRMR = 0.05. Modification indices analysis suggested removing the items “When I get up in the morning, I feel like going to class” and “I get carried away when I am studying”. In this case, the model had a satisfactory fit, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.07, and SRMS = 0.03. Internal consistency was satisfactory for absorption (r = 0.43), vigor (r = 0.62), and dedication (ω = 0.88). Given that the three dimensions of study engagement were highly intercorrelated (p < 0.001), mean score of study engagement was calculated.

2.4.2. Time Perspective

We used the French short version of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI) (Fritsch & Cuervo-Lombard, 2022) to assess time perspective. The scale is composed of 15 items, with 3 items assessing each time orientation: past positive (e.g., “thinking about my past gives me pleasure”), past negative (e.g., “I think about the bad things that happened to me in the past”), present hedonistic (e.g., “I always find myself driven by the excitement of the moment”), present fatalistic (e.g., “Since what must happen will happen, it really doesn’t matter what I do”), and future (e.g., “I bring my projects to fruition on time, taking things one step at a time”). All items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all true for me) to 5 (totally true for me). The goodness of fit indices indicated that the model did not have a satisfactory fit: CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMR = 0.06. Modification index analysis suggested removing the item “I no longer enjoy doing things if I have to think about objectives, consequences and results”. In this case, the model had a satisfactory fit, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.05, and SRMR = 0.05. Internal consistency was satisfactory for past negative (ω = 0.76), past positive (ω = 0.75), present fatalistic (r = 0.44), present hedonistic (ω = 0.66), and future (ω = 0.70). The mean score for each temporal dimension was calculated.

3. Results

3.1. Analytic Plan

We used the RStudio 2024.12.1. (Posit Team, 2024) and Mplus 8.10 software (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017) to process the results. Correlation analyses were conducted to examine relations among variables using Pearson’s r. To investigate the existence of temporal perspective profiles, we conducted a latent profile analysis (LPA, package “MplusAutomation”, Hallquist & Wiley, 2018) to identify distinct profiles of temporal perspective with the Mixture Modeling Method, and we used robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLR) estimator because the data did not follow a normal distribution (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2005). Finally, to address our last research question, concerning the relationship between each profile and engagement, we employed the Bolck–Croon–Hagenaars (BCH) method to assess whether profile membership predicted academic engagement (Bakk & Vermunt, 2016). Specifically, we conducted equality tests to compare the mean engagement scores of participants across all latent profiles. Its main advantage lies in the third step, where an analysis of variance is performed using individual weights derived from classification uncertainty. These weights serve as an improved latent profile indicator, offering a more accurate alternative to relying solely on modal class assignment.

3.2. Descriptive Statistics

First, the data were checked to ensure no missing values or anomalies in the cases. Six participants with aberrant or missing data were excluded. Age data was missing for 15 participants; therefore, we imputed the mean value of 19.2 years. Means, standard deviations, and correlations are presented in Table 1. Correlation analyses showed that study engagement was positively correlated with past positive (p < 0.001), present hedonistic (p < 0.01), and future (p < 0.001), and negatively correlated with past negative (p < 0.001) and present fatalistic (p < 0.01) (see Table 1).

3.3. Latent Profile Analysis

The LPA was conducted to determine the number of profiles related to students’ temporal perspective, using factor scores previously extracted from a confirmatory factor analysis of the ZTPI scale (with five indicators: past positive, past negative, present fatalistic, present hedonistic, and future). Means were allowed to be different across classes, and residual variance was allowed to be equal but freely estimated. To select the optimal number of profiles, we have considered the theoretical meaning of the profiles and the need to obtain a statistically satisfactory solution (see Spurk et al., 2020). Different criteria were applied to evaluate the fit of the LPA model and to select the best reading profile models: 1/Information criteria: Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), and sample-size adjusted BIC (SSABIC). The model with the lowest values on these criteria was selected as the best-fitting model. 2/Entropy: Values approaching 1 indicate high classification accuracy. 3/Statistical tests: Parametric bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT), Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin test (VLMR), and adjusted Lo–Mendell–Rubin test (A-LMR). A p-value inferior to 0.05 suggests that adding an extra profile improves model fit. 4/Proportion of participants in each profile: profiles with at least 5% of the sample were considered meaningful.
Table 2 provides the fit statistics for possible latent profile structures. Although the likelihood-based indices (AIC, BIC, CAIC, and SSABIC) continue to decrease as the number of classes increases, the rate of improvement slows after five classes. The entropy value at five classes (0.78) indicates good classification quality. Moreover, the VLMR-LRT and ALMR tests are non-significant beyond four classes, suggesting that adding more profiles does not yield statistically significant improvement. Considering the balance between model fit, classification accuracy, and theoretical interpretability, the five-class solution was selected as the optimal model.
Table 3 displays the results of the profile analysis. Based on the above criteria, we stopped at a 5-profile structure. Although VLMR—LRT and ALMR are not significant at profile 3, indicating that we can stop adding profiles, the indices are significant at a 4-profile structure, but not at larger numbers of profiles. Other arguments led us to choose the 5-profile solution as it has a lower AIC, BIC, and SSABIC than the 4-profile solution, and has a higher entropy index. Although one profile has less than 5% of members (4.6%, Profile 1), we consider this profile as pertinent theoretically, as discussed below.
Profile 1—High Past Negative: It is characterized by a past negative score well above the mean and a past positive score at the opposite extreme, well below the mean. It includes 21 students (4.60%). We kept this profile because it is not theoretically abstract. Indeed, the undesirable orientations (past negative and present fatalistic) oppose the desirable orientations (past positive, present hedonistic, and future). Profile 2—Balanced: It is characterized by high scores on past positive and future, a present hedonistic score slightly above the mean, and lower-than-average scores on past negative and present fatalistic. It includes 149 students (33%). This profile corresponds to the “balanced” temporal perspective described in the scientific literature by Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Profile 3—Past Negative: It is characterized by a very high past negative score relative to the other dimensions, along with below-average scores on past positive and future. It includes 82 students (18%). Profile 4—Low Past Positive–Low Present Hedonistic: All temporal perspective dimensions are below the mean, with particularly low scores for past positive and present hedonistic. It includes 79 students (17.50%). Profile 5—Past Positive—Present Hedonistic: It reflects students who are relatively oriented toward all temporal dimensions. Past positive and present hedonistic are slightly above the mean, past negative and future are lower, and present fatalistic is slightly above the mean. It includes 120 students (26.60%). Figure 1 illustrates the selected 5-profile solution, and Table 4 shows the estimated parameters for the 5-profile model.

3.4. Bolck–Croon–Hagenaars—Differences in Sex, Age, and Engagement Across the Five Temporal Perspective Profiles

Differences in sex, age, and academic engagement between the five temporal perspective profiles were examined using the BCH method, which allows for accurate comparison of distal continuous and categorical outcomes while accounting for classification uncertainty in latent profile analysis (see Table 5). This approach ensured robust group comparisons across profiles without influencing the latent class formation.

3.4.1. Sex Differences Across Profiles

Sex distribution significantly differed across the five temporal perspective profiles, as indicated by the BCH omnibus test (χ2 (4) = 41.94, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons showed that the high past negative profile consisted of 100% women, which was significantly different from the balanced (89.8%, p < 0.001), past negative (91%, p = 0.018), low past positive–low present hedonistic (92.7%, p = 0.023), and past positive–present hedonistic profiles (90.8%, p = 0.001). No other significant sex differences were observed between the profiles.

3.4.2. Age Differences Across Profiles

Age differed significantly between the temporal perspective profiles, according to the BCH omnibus test (χ2 (4) = 17.25, p = 0.002). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the high past negative profile (Mage = 18.46) was significantly younger than the balanced profile (Mage = 19.46, p = 0.016). There was a trend for the low past positive–low present hedonistic profile (Mage = 19.63) to be older than the high past negative profile (p = 0.057). The balanced profile was also significantly older than the past negative profile (Mage = 18.52, p = 0.003), and the past negative profile differed marginally from the low past positive-low present hedonistic profile (p = 0.050). Additionally, the past negative profile was significantly younger than the past positive–present hedonistic (Mage = 19.10 years). No other age differences were statistically significant.

3.4.3. Engagement Differences Across Profiles

Academic engagement significantly differed across the temporal perspective profiles, as indicated by the BCH omnibus test (χ2 (4) = 89.32, p < 0.001). Pairwise comparisons based on the BCH-adjusted means revealed that the high past negative profile reported significantly lower engagement levels than the balanced profile (p < 0.001), 4 (p = 0.014), and 5 (p < 0.001). The balanced profile, which showed the highest engagement, differed significantly from all the other profiles: the high past negative profile (p < 0.001), past negative profile (p < 0.001), low past positive–low present hedonistic profile (p < 0.001), and past positive–present hedonistic profile (p < 0.001). Moreover, Profile 3 had significantly lower engagement than Profiles 4 (p < 0.01) and 5 (p < 0.001). No other pairwise comparisons reached statistical significance. These results suggest that temporal perspective profiles are meaningfully associated with students’ levels of academic engagement.

4. General Discussion

The main aim of the present study was to identify 1/latent profiles of TP among French students and 2/the latent profile that is the best associated with study engagement. Based on Zimbardo and Boyd’s (2008) view, we expected that the profile best associated with study engagement is balanced. Our results highlight the existence of five profiles, including a balanced profile. As expected, our results showed that the balanced profile is the best associated with study engagement scores. To the best of our knowledge, it is the first time that a study has shown the relationships between study engagement and specific temporal orientation profiles.
About the profiles, we observed that proportion of students in the profiles is variable. More precisely, the balanced profile encompasses 33%; past positive—present hedonist 26.60%; past negative 18%; low past positive–low present hedonist with 17.50% and high past negative 4.60% of the students. In sum, our results show that around 60% of the students have no problematic profile (i.e., balanced and past positive—present hedonist). On the contrary, 22.6% of the students have TP oriented toward high past negative and high present fatalistic, and 17.5% have TP oriented toward low past positive and low present hedonistic. This raises questions about the well-being of these students.
Two types of analyses have been used to identify the BTP profile in the literature: cluster analysis and latent profile analysis. Nevertheless, the results obtained with these two analyses are inconsistent. More precisely, with cluster analysis, McKay et al. (2014) observed that adolescents (ages 12–16) with a BTP profile did not report the most adaptive outcomes. More precisely, for adolescents with a BTP and a past negative profile, the probability of being a problematic or a moderate drinker is identical, while it is smaller for those with a future profile. McKay et al. (2014) suggested that among adolescents, the future profile would be more optimal, while the BTP profile would be more optimal among young adults and adults. Nevertheless, McKay et al. (2018) constated that Australian students with a BTP profile did not differ from those with a past negative–fatalist profile on problematic alcohol use, depression, anxiety, optimism, and self-esteem. They reported lower self-esteem and optimism scores and higher depression than those with an ambivalent profile. In sum, contrary to Boniwell and Zimbardo’s (2004) view, a BTP profile identified by cluster analysis appears not to lead to the most adaptive outcomes, while a BTP based on latent profile analysis is linked with less engagement in drinking behavior (Braitman & Henson, 2015) and with higher study engagement in our study. Moreover, with a latent profile not completely balanced—but obtained in a specific context (i.e., participants tested before or during the COVID-19 pandemic)—fewer educational, social, and mental health problems were observed (Kossewska et al., 2023). A balanced profile based on latent profile analysis appears to be more theoretically adequate for Zimbardo and Boyd’s (1999) hypothesis than a balanced profile based on cluster analysis. On a large sample, a latent profile analysis appears to be more appropriate than a cluster analysis. Indeed, contrary to a cluster analysis based on a specific sample, with a latent profile analysis, the probability of belonging to each profile is calculated for each participant. The participant is assigned to the profile to which he or she is most likely to belong (Spurk et al., 2020).

4.1. Implications

Identifying the different latent profiles is useful to detect the students who most need support (i.e., students at-risk) in order to increase their study engagement—a crucial factor for academic success (Çali et al., 2024; Wang & Eccles, 2012), and well-being (Chaudhry et al., 2024; Denovan et al., 2020; Nolberto-Quispe et al., 2021). Once at-risk students are identified, it is possible to support them in different ways. For unbalanced profiles, if temporal orientations do not reveal a trait, it can be suggested to focus students on Future orientation because 1/it is an important element of BTP, and 2/it is positively associated with Past Positive (e.g., Denovan et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2013), another important dimension of BTP.
To increase future orientation, it is possible to prime them on a future-oriented mindset; they prioritize more distal goals than proximal goals (Dreves & Blackhart, 2019). Also, it is possible to improve self-efficacy. It is a malleable skill defined as the “beliefs in one’s capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to produce given attainments” (Bandura, 1997, p. 3). In academic contexts, self-efficacy is positively associated with future orientation (for a review see Kooij et al., 2018), and negatively with past negative (Zebardast et al., 2011), and present fatalistic (Taylor & Wilson, 2019; Zebardast et al., 2011). How individuals interpret their past experiences (mastery experiences), vicarious experiences, somatic/affective states (i.e., stress, fatigue, anxiety, and mood), and social persuasion (positive feedback from family, peers, and teachers) are major sources of self-efficacy (Bandura, 2001), and the most powerful is mastery experience (see meta-analysis by Byars-Winston et al., 2017). Positive feedbacks allow students to have information about their mastery of the task (Schunk & Swartz, 1993; Zumbrunn et al., 2016). Moreover, focusing on the strengths and factors that contributed to past achievement and students’ strengths increases mastery experience (Ajzen, 2002).
Finally, BTP is considered as a specific expression of the ability to manage internal states in a constructive way (Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010). This involves cultivating psychological flexibility, which refers to the capacity to act consistently with one’s values while remaining connected to the present moment, even in the face of difficult feelings, sensations, and thoughts (Hayes et al., 2006). Increasing psychological flexibility is relevant in educational settings, as it is 1/positively associated with academic self-efficacy (Martinie & Shankland, 2024; Schéle et al., 2021)—a crucial predictor for student achievement (see meta-analysis of Multon et al., 1991), 2/positively predicts study engagement (Martinie & Shankland, 2024), 3/is positively associated with past positive and negatively with past negative and present fatalistic orientations (Pyszkowska & Rönnlund, 2021). Psychological flexibility is a malleable skill that can be developed through specific interventions based on acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) principles (Hayes et al., 2006). Universities could be encouraged to lead interventions based on ACT principles among their students, as they have been shown to improve study engagement and psychological capital (Fang & Ding, 2020; Grégoire et al., 2018, 2019).

4.2. Limitations

The present study has several limitations. Our sample consisted exclusively of French students enrolled in humanities and social sciences (HSS). As such, our findings should be interpreted as specific to this population of students, which has a higher tendency to procrastinate (Martinie et al., 2022 for a study with French students enrolled in HSS; Nordby et al., 2017), which negatively impacts their study engagement (Afrashteh & Janjani, 2024). In future work, it could be interesting to compare the time perspective of these populations of students to that of other populations, such as STEM students, in order to better understand the role of temporal profiles and causes of dropout. In the same line, these results could be completed by the study of other psychological characteristics such as mood, anxiety, personality traits, or metacognitive processes, which could be particularly associated with specific profiles (Stolarski et al., 2020). We observed an effect of age on profiles that suggests that profiles could be evolving over time. Therefore, it will be necessary to consider time in establishment of the profile. Indeed, it could be relevant to test the profile structure from the same sample at several time points and examine the extent to which individuals could transition between different profiles. Furthermore, we observed differences according to sex. Nevertheless, it is worth mentioning that our population was an almost entirely female population. To generalize our results, future research should investigate other student populations and consider age and sex. Moreover, our study did not consider family socioeconomic status, while it is positively associated with study engagement (Fullarton, 2002; Tomaszewski et al., 2020). Future research should consider this factor to establish latent profiles of TP and other sociodemographic information, such as family status or employment. Finally, the association between latent profile and study engagement does not allow for any causal conclusions about the observed relationships.

4.3. Conclusions

Our results highlight five latent profiles among French students. Two are really problematic, in which students are highly focused on past negative experiences and are little oriented toward the future. Furthermore, we observed another problematic profile in which students show a low level on all orientations, and particularly on past positive and present hedonist. These three problematic profiles suggest that 40% of the students could be at-risk. Moreover, we observed two non-problematic profiles. One in which students are relatively oriented toward all temporal dimensions, including 26.60% of the students, and one balanced profile, including 33% of the students. As expected, the balanced profile was best associated with study engagement, a central factor in academic performance (Lei et al., 2018; Serrano & Andreu, 2016), and student well-being (Chaudhry et al., 2024; Denovan et al., 2020; Nolberto-Quispe et al., 2021). Therefore, identifying students’ profiles could be a means of detecting at-risk students in order to support them more specifically during their studies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.-A.M. and N.N.; methodology, M.-A.M., N.N. and Z.-A.M.; software, N.N.; validation, M.-A.M., R.S. and Z.-A.M.; formal analysis, E.D. and Z.-A.M.; investigation, Z.-A.M.; resources, N.N.; data curation, E.D. and Z.-A.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.-A.M. and Z.-A.M.; writing—review and editing, M.-A.M., N.N., R.S., E.D. and Z.-A.M.; visualization, E.D. and Z.-A.M.; supervision, M.-A.M.; project administration, M.-A.M., N.N. and Z.-A.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This research was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the university’s Ethics Committee of Poitiers and Tours (CER-TP 2024-02-05).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are publicly available on the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/nxvty/?view_only=1e68ed2df202439c84d75e9eee5a3acb (accessed on 8 September 2025).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Afrashteh, M. Y., & Janjani, P. (2024). The mediating role of goal orientation in the relationship between formative assessment with academic engagement and procrastination in medical students. BMC Medical Education, 24(1), 1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioural control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behaviour. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 665–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Fallu, J.-S., & Pagani, L. S. (2009). Student engagement and its relationship with early high school dropout. Journal of Adolescence, 32(3), 651–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2005, November 14–16). Multivariate statistical modeling with survey data (pp. 14–16). The 2005 FCSM Research Conference, Sheraton Crystal City Hotel, Arlington, VA, USA. Available online: https://www.statmodel.com/download/2005FCSM.pdf (accessed on 12 March 2025).
  5. Bakk, Z., & Vermunt, J. K. (2016). Robustness of stepwise latent class modeling with continuous distal outcomes. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 23(1), 20–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W.H. Freeman and Company. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An Agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Boniwell, I., Osin, E., Alex Linley, P., & Ivanchenko, G. V. (2010). A question of balance: Time perspective and well-being in British and Russian samples. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(1), 24–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Boniwell, I., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Balancing one’s time perspective in pursuit of optimal functioning. In P. A. Linley, & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 165–178). Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  10. Braitman, A. L., & Henson, J. M. (2015). The impact of time perspective latent profiles on college drinking: A multidimensional approach. Substance Use & Misuse, 50(5), 664–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Byars-Winston, A., Diestelmann, J., Savoy, J. N., & Hoyt, W. T. (2017). Unique effects and moderators of effects of sources on self-efficacy: A model-based meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64(6), 645–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Çali, M., Lazimi, L., & Ippoliti, B. M. L. (2024). Relationship between student engagement and academic performance. International Journal of Evaluation and Research in Education, 13(4), 2210–2217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Chaudhry, S., Tandon, A., Shinde, S., & Bhattacharya, A. (2024). Student psychological well-being in higher education: The role of internal team environment, institutional, friends and family support and academic engagement. PLoS ONE, 19(1), e0297508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Daugherty, J. R., & Brase, G. L. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 202–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. de Bilde, J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2011). Understanding the association between future time perspective and self-regulated learning through the lens of self-determination theory. Learning and Instruction, 21(3), 332–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., Macaskill, A., & Papageorgiou, K. (2020). Future time perspective, positive emotions and student engagement: A longitudinal study. Studies in Higher Education, 45(7), 1533–1546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Drake, L., Duncan, E., Sutherland, F., Abernethy, C., & Henry, C. (2008). Time perspective and correlates of wellbeing. Time & Society, 17(1), 47–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Dreves, P. A., & Blackhart, G. C. (2019). Thinking into future: How a future time perspective improves self-control. Personality and Individuals Differences, 149, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Fang, S., & Ding, D. (2020). The efficacy of group-based acceptance and commitment therapy on psychological capital and school engagement: A pilot study among Chinese adolescents. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 16(1), 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fritsch, A., & Cuervo-Lombard, C.-V. (2022). Echelle de temporalité: Validation française version courte de la Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI). Psychologie Française, 67(1), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fuentes, A., Oyanadel, C., Zimbardo, P., González-Loyola, M., Olivera-Figueroa, L. A., & Peñate, W. (2022). Mindfulness and balanced time perspective: Predictive model of psychological well-being and gender differences in college students. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 12, 306–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fullarton, S. (2002). Student engagement with school: Individual and school-level influences. Australian Council for Educational Research ACER. [Google Scholar]
  23. Grégoire, S., Chénier, C., Doucerin, M., Lachance, L., & Shankland, R. (2019). Ecological momentary assessment of stress, well-being and psychological flexibility among college and university students during Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 52(3), 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Grégoire, S., Lachance, L., Bouffard, T., & Dionne, F. (2018). The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to promote mental health and school engagement in university students: A multisite randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 49(3), 360–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Griffin, E., & Wildbur, D. (2020). The role of balanced time perspective on student well-being and mental health: A mixed-methods study. Mental Health and Prevention, 18, 200181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hallquist, M. N., & Wiley, J. F. (2018). MplusAutomation: An R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling, 25, 621–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Haririzade, F., Barzegar Bafrooei, K., & Zandevanian, A. (2023). Investigating the relationship between time perspective and academic engagement with the mediation of self-efficacy in students. Advances in Cognitive Sciences, 25(1), 46–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Henson, J. M., Carey, M. P., Carey, K. B., & Maisto, S. A. (2006). Associations among health behaviors and time perspective in young adults: Model testing with boot-strapping replication. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29(2), 127–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hong, S. (2025). The impact of future time perspective on academic achievement: Mediating roles of academic burnout and engagement. PLoS ONE, 20(1), e0316841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hong, S., & Hanafi, Z. (2024). Understanding time perspective’s influence on academic burnout and achievement in Chinese undergraduates. Scientific Reports, 14, 20430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hu, L.-t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Jankowski, K. S., Zajenkowski, M., & Stolarski, M. (2020). What are the optimal levels of time perspectives? Deviation from the Balanced Time Perspective-Revisited (DBTP-r). Psychologica Belgica, 60(1), 164–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 865–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kooij, T. A. M., Kanfer, R., Betts, M., & Rudolph, C. W. (2018). Future time perspective: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(8), 867–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kossewska, J., Tomaszek, K., & Macałka, E. (2023). Time perspective latent profile analysis and its meaning for school burnout, depression, and family acceptance in adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20, 5433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lei, H., Cui, Y., & Zhou, W. (2018). Relationships between student engagement and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Social Behavior and Personality, 46(3), 517–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Linden, A. N., Lau-Barraco, C., & Hollis, B. F. (2014). Associations between psychological distress and alcohol outcomes as mediated by time perspective orientation among college students. Mental Health and Substance Use, 7(2), 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Liu, Y., Gong, X., Shi, L., Shi, Y., Dong, B., & Tian, X. (2023). Future time perspective and study engagement among middle school students: A moderated mediation model. PsyCh Journal, 12(6), 801–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Luyckx, K., Lens, W., Smits, I., & Goossens, L. (2010). Time perspective and identity formation: Short-term longitudinal dynamics in college students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 34, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Martinie, M.-A., Potocki, A., Broc, L., & Larigauderie, P. (2022). Predictors of procrastination in first-year university students: Role of achievement goals and learning strategies. Social Psychology of Education, 25(6), 309–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Martinie, M.-A., & Shankland, R. (2024). Achievement goals, self-efficacy and psychological flexibility as antecedent of study engagement. Social Psychology of Education, 27(5), 2395–2416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. McKay, M. T., Andretta, J. R., Magee, J., & Worrell, F. C. (2014). What do temporal profiles tell us about adolescent alcohol use? Results from a large sample in the United Kingdom. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 1319–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. McKay, M. T., Worrell, F. C., Zivkovic, U., Temple, E., Mello, Z. R., Misil, B., Cole, J. C., Andretta, J. R., & Perry, J. L. (2018). A balanced time perspective: Is it an exercise in empiricism, and does it relate meaningfully to health and well-being outcomes? International Journal of Psychology, 54(6), 775–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ministère de l’Enseignement Supérieur et de la Recherche. (2020). Le BO. Bulletin officiel de l’éducation nationale. Spécial n°22 du 28 octobre 2020. CNDR Publications Administratives. Available online: https://www.enseignementsup-recherche.gouv.fr/cid154936/parcours-et-reussite-en-licence-les-resultats-de-la-session-2019.html (accessed on 28 March 2025).
  47. Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 30–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  49. Nolberto-Quispe, L., Gonzales-Macavilca, M., & Iraola-Real, I. (2021). Influence of positive and negative affect on the academic engagement of students in the initial education degree: Predictive study. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 9(5), 994–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Nordby, K., Klingsieck, K. B., & Svartdal, F. (2017). Do procrastination-friendly environments make students delay unnecessarily? Social Psychology of Education, 20(3), 491–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Peetsma, T. T. D. (2000). Future time perspective as a predictor of school investment. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 44(2), 177–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Peng, M. Y.-P., & Zhang, Z. (2022). Future time orientation and learning engagement through the lens of self-determination theory for freshman: Evidence from cross-lagged analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 760212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Posit Team. (2024). RStudio: Integrated development environment for R. [Computer software]. Posit Software, PBC. Available online: https://posit.co (accessed on 28 March 2025).
  54. Pyszkowska, A., & Rönnlund, M. (2021). Psychological flexibility and self-compassion as predictors of well-being: Mediating role of balances time perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 671746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Salmela-Aro, K., & Upadaya, K. (2012). The schoolwork engagement inventory. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28(1), 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Schaufeli, W. B., Martínez, I. M., Marques-Pinto, A., Salanova, M., & Bakker, A. (2002). Burnout and engagement in university students: A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(5), 464–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Schéle, I., Olby, M., Wallin, H., & Holmquist, S. (2021). Self-efficacy, psychological flexibility, and basic needs satisfaction make difference: Recently graduated psychologists at increased or decreased risk for future health issues. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 569605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Schunk, D. H., & Swartz, C. W. (1993). Goals and progress feedback: Effects on self-efficacy and writing achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18(3), 337–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Serrano, C., & Andreu, Y. (2016). Perceived emotional intelligence, subjective well-being, perceived-stress, engagement and academic achievement of adolescents. Revista de Psicodidactica, 21(2), 357–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Sinatra, G. M., Heddy, B. C., & Lombardi, D. (2015). The challenges of defining and measuring student engagement in science. Educational Psychologist, 50(1), 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sircova, A., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Osin, E., Milfont, T. L., Fieulaine, N., Kislali-Erginbilgic, A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2015). Time perspective profiles of cultures. In M. Stolarski, N. Fieulaine, & W. van Beek (Eds.), Time perspective theory; Review, research and application: Essays in honor of Philip G. Zimbardo (pp. 169–187). Springer International Publishing/Springer Nature. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kinderman, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Spurk, D., Hirschi, A., Wang, M., Valero, D., & Kauffeld, S. (2020). Latent profile analysis: A review and “how to” guide of its application within vocational behavior research. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 120, 103445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Stolarski, M. (2016). Not restricted by their personality: Balanced time perspective moderates well-established relationships between personality traits and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 100, 140–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Stolarski, M., Bitner, J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2011). Time perspective, emotional intelligence and discounting of delayed awards. Time & Society 20, 346–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2018). Putting time in a wider perspective: The past, the present, and the future of time perspective theory. In V. Zeigler-Hill, & T. Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences (pp. 592–628). SAGE. [Google Scholar]
  68. Stolarski, M., Zajenkowski, M., Jankowski, K. S., & Szymaniak, K. (2020). Deviation from the balanced time perspective: A systematic review of empirical relationships with psychological variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 156, 109772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Taylor, J., & Wilson, J. C. (2019). Using our understanding of time to increase self-efficacy towards goal achievement. Heliyon, 5, e02116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Tomaszewski, W., Xiang, N., & Western, M. (2020). Student engagement as a mediator of the effects of socio-economic status on academic performance among secondary school students in Australia. British Educational Research Journal, 46(3), 610–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Wang, M. T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Social support matters: Longitudinal effects of social support on three dimensions of school engagement from middle to high school. Child Development, 83(3), 877–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Xu, B., Chen, N.-S., & Chen, G. (2020). Effects of teacher role on student engagement in WeChat-based online discussion learning. Computers & Education, 157, 103956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Zaffran, J., & Aigle, M. (2020). Qui décroche de l’université? Mise en perspective nationale et analyse d’une enquête en région Aquitaine. Revue de l’OFCE, 167(3), 5–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zebardast, A., Besharat, M. A., & Hghighatgoo, M. (2011). The relationship between self-efficacy and time perspective in students. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 935–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Zhang, J. W., Howell, R. T., & Stolarski, M. (2013). Comparing three methods to measure a balanced time perspective: The relationship between balanced time perspective and subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14, 169–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The time paradox: The new psychology of time that will change your life. Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  78. Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2015). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. In M. Stolarski, N. Fieulaine, & W. van Beek (Eds.), Time perspective theory; Review, research and application: Essays in honor of Philip G. Zimbardo (pp. 17–55). Springer International Publishing/Springer Nature. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zumbrunn, S., Marrs, S., & Mewborn, C. (2016). Toward a better understanding of student perceptions of writing feedback: A mixed methods study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29(2), 349–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Mean scores of the five ZTPI dimensions (PP: past positive; PN: past negative; PH: present hedonistic; PF: present fatalistic; F: future) for the five temporal perspective profiles.
Figure 1. Mean scores of the five ZTPI dimensions (PP: past positive; PN: past negative; PH: present hedonistic; PF: present fatalistic; F: future) for the five temporal perspective profiles.
Ejihpe 15 00191 g001
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, internal consistencies, and intercorrelations between ZTPI dimensions and academic engagement.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, internal consistencies, and intercorrelations between ZTPI dimensions and academic engagement.
MSD123456789
1. Past Positive9.582.95(ω = 0.75)
2. Past Negative9.183.36−0.289 ***(ω = 0.76)
3. Present Hedonist9.312.630.236 ***0.041(ω = 0.66)
4. Present Fatalistic6.642.55−0.110 *0.299 ***0.059(r = 0.44)
5. Future9.752.730.236 ***−0.245 ***0.056−0.157 ***(ω = 0.70)
6. Engagement29.66.980.195 ***−0.272 ***0.139 **−0.238 ***0.524 *** = 0.87)
7. Devotion11.52.720.147 **−0.225 ***0.099 *−0.211 ***0.410 ***0.884 ***(ω = 0.88)
8. Vigor8.542.530.189 ***−0.282 ***0.166 ***−0.189 ***0.465 ***0.867 ***0.632 ***(r = 0.62)
9. Absorption9.632.650.183 ***−0.216 ***0.107 *−0.231 ***0.514 ***0.899 ***0.698 ***0.681 ***(r = 0.43)
Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; ω = McDonald’s Omega; r = Pearson’s r (for bivariate correlation); * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Goodness of fit statistics for model comparison (standardized scores).
Table 2. Goodness of fit statistics for model comparison (standardized scores).
ModelLog-LikelihoodBest Log ReplicatedParametersScalingAICBICSSABICEntropyVLMR-LRT (p)ALMR (p)BLRT (p)
1 Class−2877.691100.8625775.385816.495784.76----
2 Class−2754.281161.3545540.575606.355555.570.670.0300.0320.00
3 Class−2717.291221.5475478.595569.045499.220.720.4030.4090.00
4 Class−2663.321281.0865382.655497.775408.910.770.0000.0000.00
5 Class−2644.061341.1405356.115495.905388.000.780.2140.2210.00
6 Class−2626.471401.2615332.955497.405370.460.770.4890.4970.00
7 Class−2606.660461.2915305.335494.465348.470.800.3670.3730.00
8 Class−2591.330521.3305286.665500.465335.430.810.5240.5290.00
Note. AIC, Akaike Information Criteria; BIC, Bayesian Information Criteria; SSABIC, Sample Size-Adjusted Bayesian Information Criteria; VLMR, Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin likelihood ratio test; A-LMR, Lo–Mendell–Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test; BLRT, parametric bootstrapped likelihood ratio test.
Table 3. Distribution of temporal perspective dimensions by latent profile and percentage of students.
Table 3. Distribution of temporal perspective dimensions by latent profile and percentage of students.
Profile 1Profile 2Profile 3Profile 4Profile 5
Past Positive−1.6940.625−0.5−0.7660.403
Past Negative1.372−0.971.023−0.170.314
Present Hedonist−0.7590.0950.24−0.8220.398
Present Fatalistic0.306−0.350.599−0.2220.101
Future−1.2720.423−0.566−0.1340.2
N211498279120
Pourcentage4.60%33%18%17.50%26.60%
Note. Values represent standardized means (z-scores) of each ZTPI dimension across the five latent profiles. Profile 1 = high past negative profile; Profile 2 = balanced profile; Profile 3 = past negative profile; Profile 4 = low past positive—low present hedonistic profile; Profile 5 = past positive–present hedonistic profile.
Table 4. Estimated parameters for the 5-profile model.
Table 4. Estimated parameters for the 5-profile model.
b (s.e.)S.E.Est./SEp
Profile 1 (4.6%)
SPP−1.690.136−12.491<0.001
SPN1.370.11711.755<0.001
SPH−0.760.470−1.6140.107
SPF0.310.3170.9650.334
SF−1.270.256−4.967<0.001
Profile 2 (33%)
SPP0.630.06010.328<0.001
SPN−0.970.048−20.429<0.001
SPH0.100.0731.3000.194
SPF−0.350.069−5.106<0.001
SF0.420.0656.556<0.001
Profile 3 (18%)
SPP−0.500.226−2.2110.027
SPN1.020.1307.847<0.001
SPH0.240.1122.1480.032
SPF0.600.1853.2440.001
SF−0.570.130−4.343<0.001
Profile 4 (17.5%)
SPP−0.770.116−6.612<0.001
SPN−0.170.100−1.7080.088
SPH−0.820.114−7.186<0.001
SPF−0.220.098−2.2610.024
SF−0.130.112−1.2010.230
Profile 5 (26.6%)
SPP0.400.0954.235<0.001
SPN0.310.0893.534<0.001
SPH0.400.0934.270<0.001
SPF0.100.1080.9290.353
SF0.200.1111.7960.073
Note. N = 451; b: unstandardized coefficient; S.E.: standard error; Profile 1 = high past negative profile; Profile 2 = balanced profile; Profile 3 = past negative profile; Profile 4 = low past positive—low present hedonistic profile; Profile 5 = past positive–present hedonistic profile.
Table 5. Characteristics of the mixture regression profiles on covariates.
Table 5. Characteristics of the mixture regression profiles on covariates.
Variable(N = 451)MSDProfile 1Profile 2Profile 3Profile 4Profile 5Global TestSummary of Significance Tests
% Female91.35%100.00%89.8%91.0%92.7%90.8%χ2 (4) = 41.94, p < 0.0011 > 2 = 3 = 4 = 5
Age19.22.7118.4619.4618.5219.6319.10χ2 (4) = 17.25, p = 0.0021 = 4 = 5; 3 < 4 = 5 = 2; 2 > 1 = 3
Engagement29.66.98−0.730.46−0.60−0.140.08χ2 (4) = 89.32, p < 0.0011 = 3 < 5 = 4 > 2
Note. Raw scores were used for the descriptive statistics of the engagement and age variables. Percentages of females per profile are reported for each profile. Multiple comparisons are based on post hoc tests following the latent profile analysis with covariates. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; p = probability value. Profile 1 = high past negative profile; Profile 2 = balanced profile; Profile 3 = past negative profile; Profile 4 = low past positive—low present hedonistic profile; Profile 5 = past positive–present hedonistic profile.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mathieu, Z.-A.; Dujardin, E.; Noiret, N.; Shankland, R.; Martinie, M.-A. Time Perspective Profile and Study Engagement. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2025, 15, 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15100191

AMA Style

Mathieu Z-A, Dujardin E, Noiret N, Shankland R, Martinie M-A. Time Perspective Profile and Study Engagement. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2025; 15(10):191. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15100191

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mathieu, Zara-Anna, Emilie Dujardin, Nicolas Noiret, Rébecca Shankland, and Marie-Amélie Martinie. 2025. "Time Perspective Profile and Study Engagement" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 15, no. 10: 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15100191

APA Style

Mathieu, Z.-A., Dujardin, E., Noiret, N., Shankland, R., & Martinie, M.-A. (2025). Time Perspective Profile and Study Engagement. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 15(10), 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe15100191

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop