Computer-Based Prognostic Task Measurements as Indicators of Uncertainty Acceptance
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Awareness (a more complete orientation in the situation before decision-making) was positively associated with rationality and intolerance for uncertainty.
- The prognosis justifiability as the reliability of the possible outcome derived from the information available, positively correlated with the level of intelligence and negatively—with the risk-readiness and venturesome-ness.
- The trend, reflecting the tendency to react to momentary changes in a situation when giving a prognosis, was positively associated with impulsiveness and unproductive coping, hypervigilance, manifested as a tendency to make impulsive decisions in an uncertain situation.
- There were stable correlational patterns of awareness, justifiability, and trend, which indicate strategies for uncertainty control were associated with differences in personal and cognitive traits involved in decision-making regulation.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Methods and Outcome Measures
2.2.1. Prognostic Task
2.2.2. Psychodiagnostic Methods
- Personal Decision-Making Factors Questionnaire (LFR-21) developed by Kornilova [30]. The questionnaire consisted of 21 yes-or-no statements divided into two scales: (1) rationality, an inclination for a broader orientation in the decision situation, and (2) personal risk-readiness, willingness and ability to make and perform choices in uncertain situations.
- New questionnaire of tolerance/intolerance for uncertainty (NTN) developed by Kornilova [30]. The questionnaire consisted of 33 statements, evaluated with a seven-point Likert scale (from “completely disagree” to “completely agree”). The questionnaire provided three measurements: (1) tolerance for uncertainty, a personal acceptance of novelty, complexity, inconsistency problem-solving and decision-making conditions; willingness to act in new and unusual ways; (2) intolerance for uncertainty the desire for clarity, orderliness, avoidance of uncertainty, reliance on rules and principles; a tendency to see opinions, values and methods of action as either right or wrong; (3) interpersonal intolerance for uncertainty as a desire for control in interpersonal relationships, a desire for clarity and discomfort in relationships where this clarity is absent.
- Eysencks’ Impulsiveness Scale (seventh version, I7) [31] in Russian short adaptation by Kornilova and Dolnikova [32]. Consisted of 28 yes-or-no questions and three sub-scales: (1) Impulsiveness, a decrease in self-control and a tendency to act under the spur of the moment; (2) Venturesomeness which manifested in the search for strong emotions, thrills; (3) Empathy, reflecting the ability to empathize with others and feel their emotions.
- Russian version of Epstein’s Faith in Intuition scale from the Rational Experiential Inventory (REI) [33,34] with two measurements: (1) experiential (intuitive) ability—The ability to report one’s intuitive impressions and feelings, and (2) experiential (intuitive) engagement as a willingness to make decisions depending on intuitions and feelings.
- Melbourne decision making questionnaire (MDMQ), a Russian adaptation by Kornilova [35,36]. The Russian version retained a 22-statement structure with four scales: (1) vigilance—A productive uncertainty coping strategy, the desire to carefully consider possible alternatives for the decision-making; and three unproductive copings: (2) buck-passing—The desire to abandon independent decision-making; (3) procrastination—The desire to delay decision-making; and (4) hypervigilance—The tendency to decide impulsively, the desire to get rid of the uncertain situation without intellectual orientation in it.
- Brief screening test (BST), the Russian modification of The Wonderlic Personnel Test. The test consisted of 50 questions. For the Russian version, participants had 15 min to complete as many questions as possible to assess their level of general cognitive abilities [37].
2.3. Statistical Methods
3. Results
3.1. Internal Consistency of the Prognostic Task Performance Indicators
3.2. The Relationship between Psychological Measurements and the Prognostic Task Performance Characteristics
3.3. Identification of Uncertainty Control Strategies in the Prognostic Task Solving
3.4. Personality Specifics of the Participants from Different Clusters
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
- The results indicated that the characteristics of a prediction-based situation (ratio of available and missing information and justifiability of a possible choice) could be considered as individually stable subjective guidelines when making decisions in prognostic tasks. At the same time, the absence of significant relationships with the measured dispositional characteristics left open the question of their contribution to the regulation of human prognostic activity and would require further research.
- The level of general cognitive abilities was associated with the tendency to make more informed prognoses based on more information available.
- Two strategies for decision-making in prognostic tasks were identified, differing in the reliance on the available information. The first strategy involved more indicators to rely on, yet it was less successful than the second strategy, characterized by sustainable reliance only on the awareness parameter. The first strategy was related to the tendency to give earlier and less informed prognoses.
- Intolerance for uncertainty was associated with a more detailed orientation in the task, manifesting in the tendency to take more parameters into the account, yet, without a full understanding of their real contribution to the final result, which could lead to an underestimation of the role of significant parameters, and made the prognosis more risky and less justified. These findings allowed us to look from a new perspective on the connection of intolerance for uncertainty and various irrational beliefs. Intolerance for uncertainty could manifest in the tendency to search for patterns in random events and the uncritical adoption of such patterns as the basis for decision-making.
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. On the psychology of prediction. Psychol. Rev. 1973, 80, 237–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Slovic, P.; Tversky, A. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, J.S.B.T. Beliefs and expectations as causes of judgmental bias. In Judgmental Forecasting, 1st ed.; Wright, G., Ayton, P., Eds.; John Wiley: Chichester, UK, 1987; pp. 31–47. [Google Scholar]
- Hogarth, R.M.; Makridakis, S. Forecasting and planning: An evaluation. Manag. Sci. 1981, 27, 115–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gigerenzer, G. Simply Rational: Decision Making in the Real World. Evolution and Cognition, 1st ed.; Oxford University: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence, M.; Edmundson, R.; O’Connor, M. An examination of the accuracy of judgmental extrapolation of time series. Int. J. Forecast. 1985, 1, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodwin, P.; Fildes, R. Judgmental forecasts of timeseries affected by special events: Does providing a statistical forecast improve accuracy? J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 1999, 12, 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Remus, W.; O’Connor, M.; Griggs, K. Does feedback improve the accuracy of recurrent judgmental forecasts? Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1996, 66, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitecotton, S.M. The effects of experience and a decision aid on the slope, scatter, and bias of earnings forecasts. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1996, 66, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersson, P.; Edman, J.; Ekman, M. Predicting the World Cup 2002 in soccer: Performance and confidence of experts and non-experts. Int. J. Forecast. 2005, 21, 565–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, G.; Rowe, G.; Bolger, F.; Gammack, J. Coherence, calibration, and expertise in judgmental probability forecasting. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1994, 57, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawrence, M.; Goodwin, P.; O’Connor, M.; Önkal, D. Judgmental forecasting: A review of progress over the last 25 years. Int. J. Forecast. 2006, 22, 493–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, G.; Ayton, P. Immediate and short-term judgmental forecasting: Personologism, situationism or interactionism? Pers. Individ. Differ. 1988, 9, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellers, B.; Stone, E.; Atanasov, P.; Rohrbaugh, N.; Metz, S.E.; Ungar, L.; Bishop, M.M.; Horowitz, M.; Merkle, E.; Tetlock, P. The psychology of intelligence analysis: Drivers of prediction accuracy in world politics. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2015, 21, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Chumakova, M.A.; Kornilov, S.A. Intelligence and successful prognostic strategies in Iowa gambling task (IGT). Psychol. J. High. Sch. Econ. 2018, 15, 10–21, (In Russian, Abstract In English). [Google Scholar]
- Frenkel-Brunswik, E. Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable. J. Pers. 1949, 18, 108–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furnham, A.; Marks, J. Tolerance of ambiguity: A review of the recent literature. Psychology 2013, 4, 717–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zermatten, A.; Van der Linden, M.; D’acremont, M.; Jermann, F.; Bechara, A. Impulsivity and Decision Making. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2005, 193, 647–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Epstein, S. Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making, 1st ed.; Plessner, H., Betsch, C., Betsch, T., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2008; pp. 23–37. [Google Scholar]
- Kornilov, S.A.; Chumakova, M.A. Individual differences in attitudes towards uncertainty: Evidence for multiple latent profiles. Psychol. Russ. 2013, 6, 94–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janis, I.L.; Mann, L. Decision Making: A Psychological Analysis of Conflict, Choice, and Commitment, 1st ed.; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence, M.; O’Connor, M.; Edmundson, B. A field study of sales forecasting accuracy and processes. Eur. J. Oper. Res. 2000, 122, 151–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Babad, E.; Hills, M.; O’Driscoll, M. Factors influencing wishful thinking and predictions of election outcomes. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 13, 461–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, A.H.; Winkler, R.L. Can weather forecasters formulate reliable probability forecasts of precipitation and temperature? Natl. Weather Dig. 1977, 2, 2–9. [Google Scholar]
- Regush, L.A. Psihologija Prognozirovanija: Uspehi v Poznanii Budushhego (Prognosis Psychology: Successes in Knowing the Future), 1st ed.; Rech: Saint-Petersburg, Russia, 2003. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Azarov, V.N. Problemy diagnostiki impul’sivnosti kak faktora individual’nyh razlichij v zarubezhnoj psihologii (Problems of diagnostics of impulsiveness as a factor of individual differences in foreign psychology). In Jeksperimental’nye Issledovanija po Problemam Obshhej i Social’noj Psihologii i Differencial’noj Psihofiziologii (Experimental Studies on the Problems of General and Social Psychology and Differential Psychophysiology), 1st ed.; Golubeva, E.A., Ol’shanskiy, V.B., Eds.; Nauchno-Issledovatel’skij Institut Obshhej i Pedagogicheskoj Psihologii: Moscow, Russia, 1979; pp. 34–42. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Azarov, V.N. Stil’ dejstvovanija: Impul’sivnost’-upravljaemost’ (Action style: Impulsiveness-control). Vopr. Psikhol. 1982, 3, 121–127. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Hohle, S.M.; Teigen, K. Forecasting forecasts: The trend effect. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2015, 10, 416–428. [Google Scholar]
- Hohle, S.M.; Teigen, K. When probabilities change: Perceptions and implications of trends in uncertain climate forecasts. J. Risk Res. 2019, 22, 555–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Chumakova, M.A.; Kornilov, S.A.; Novikova, M.A. Psihologiya Neopredelennosti: Edinstvo Intellektual’no-Lichnostnogo Potenciala Cheloveka. (The Psychology of Uncertainty: The Unity of the Intellectual and Personal Human Potential), 1st ed.; Smysl: Moscow, Russia, 2010. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck, S.B.G.; Pearson, P.R.; Easting, G.; Allsopp, J.F. Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults. Pers. Individ. Differ 1985, 6, 613–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Dolnykova, A.A. Diagnostika impul’sivnosti i sklonnosti k risku. (Diagnostics of impulsiveness and venturesomeness). Bull. MSRU. Ser. Psychol. 1995, 3, 46–56. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Epstein, S.; Pacini, R.; Denes-Raj, V.; Heier, H. Individual Differences in Intuitive–Experiential and Analytical–Rational Thinking Styles. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 390–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kornilova, T.V.; Kornilov, S.A. Intuition, Intelligence, and Personality Traits (the Results of Trying and Testing Rational Versus Experiential Inventory by S. Epstein et al.). Psihol. Issled. 2013, 6. Available online: http://psystudy.ru (accessed on 1 July 2019). (In Russian, Abstract In English).
- Mann, L.; Burnett, P.; Radford, M.; Ford, S. The Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire: An instrument for measuring patterns for coping with decisional conflict. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 1998, 10, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornilova, T.V. Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire: A Russian Adaptation. Psihol. Issled. 2013, 6. Available online: http://psystudy.ru (accessed on 24 April 2019). (In Russian, Abstract In English).
- Buzin, V.N. Kratkij Otborochnyj Test (Brief Screening Test), 1st ed.; Smysl: Moscow, Russia, 1992. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Ross, L.; Nisbett, R.E. The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology, 2nd ed.; Pinter & Martin Ltd: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Tanner, J.; Shin, D.; Mazmanian, D. Intolerance of Uncertainty, Irrational Gambling Beliefs, and Problem Gambling. In Proceedings of the 7th Annual Conference of the Canadian Association of Cognitive and Behavioural Therapies, Ottawa, ON, Canada, 18–19 May 2017; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/324107881_Intolerance_of_Uncertainty_Irrational_Gambling_Beliefs_and_Problem_Gambling_Severity (accessed on 27 October 2019).
- Kornilova, T.V. Intellektualno-Lichnostnyi Potentsial Cheloveka v Usloviyakh Neopredelennosti i Riska. (Intellectual and Personal Human Potential in Conditions of Uncertainty and Risk), 1st ed.; Nestor-Isoriya: Saint Petersburg, Russia, 2016. (In Russian) [Google Scholar]
- Kozeleckij, J. Psihologicheskaja Teorija Reshenij (Psychological Decision Theory), 1st ed.; Progress: Moscow, Russia, 1979. [Google Scholar]
Awareness | Justifiability | Trend | |
---|---|---|---|
Cognitive abilities (BST) | 0.261 | 0.244 | |
Tolerance for uncertainty (NTN) | 0.226 |
Cluster 1 (N = 42) | Cluster 2 (N = 36) | |
---|---|---|
Awareness | 2.10 | 4.27 |
Justifiability | 4.10 | 6.52 |
Trend | 2.03 | 1.63 |
Cluster 1 | Cluster 2 | |
---|---|---|
Awareness | 0.962 | 0.958 |
Justifiability | 0.825 | 0.606 |
Trend | 0.468 | 0.405 |
2nd cluster | Awareness | Justifiability | Trend | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1st cluster | |||||||
Awareness | 0.926 ** (0.981 **) | −0.004 (−0.403 **) | |||||
Justifiability | 0.930 ** (0.981 **) | 0.273 (−0.318 **) | |||||
Trend | −0.565 ** (−0.403 **) | −0.459 ** (−0.318 **) | |||||
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Epishin, V.; Bogacheva, N. Computer-Based Prognostic Task Measurements as Indicators of Uncertainty Acceptance. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2020, 10, 206-217. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe10010016
Epishin V, Bogacheva N. Computer-Based Prognostic Task Measurements as Indicators of Uncertainty Acceptance. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2020; 10(1):206-217. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe10010016
Chicago/Turabian StyleEpishin, Vitalii, and Nataliya Bogacheva. 2020. "Computer-Based Prognostic Task Measurements as Indicators of Uncertainty Acceptance" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 10, no. 1: 206-217. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe10010016
APA StyleEpishin, V., & Bogacheva, N. (2020). Computer-Based Prognostic Task Measurements as Indicators of Uncertainty Acceptance. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 10(1), 206-217. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe10010016