How 2.5D Maps Design Improve the Wayfinding Performance and Spatial Ability of Map Users
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Experiment One: View Angle and Density of Current 2.5D Maps
2.2. Experiment Two: Design Verification
3. Results
3.1. Experiment One
3.2. Experiment Two
3.2.1. Orientation
3.2.2. Map Preference
4. Discussion
- Buildings in group A and group B maps are opaque:The participants felt that the transparency used in group B maps increased the complexity of wayfinding (p = 0.015 < 0.05). They indicated that they had to spend time observing the border between buildings and roads, and that lines made the map “messy” and hard to read.
- Buildings in group C and group D maps are transparent:The participants felt that group D maps, which included lines, were easier to understand (p = 0.031 < 0.05) and use (p = 0.016 < 0.05). They mentioned that lines emphasized key landmarks and made the maps easier to understand and use.
- Buildings in group A maps and group C maps do not include lines:The participants felt that group A maps, which were opaque, were easier to understand (p = 0.016 < 0.05) and use (p = 0.021 < 0.05), whereas the transparency in group C maps facilitated identification of landmarks (p = 0.018 < 0.05). Transparency can make irrelevant buildings fainter, and drawing all nearby buildings is not necessary. However, this could result in the map losing focus and its users becoming lost in the map.
- When lines were included into group B and group D maps, the two groups did not exhibit any significant difference.Lines can help map users use key landmarks to locate their position. Transparency can create a sense of layers in a map. After key areas are marked, other areas can be made semi-transparent to reduce obstruction while maintaining crucial information. However, if the map is too transparent, the complementary landmark information is difficult to read.
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Tolman, E.C. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 1948, 55, 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tversky, B. Structures of Mental Spaces: How People Think About Space. Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 66–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, A.H.; Petchenik, B.B. The Nature of Maps: Essays toward Understanding Maps and Mapping; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Ottosson, T. Map-Reading and Wayfinding; Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis: Göteborg, Sweden, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Maceachren, A. How Maps Work: Representation, Visualization and Design; Distributed by Longman outside N.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Caduff, D.; Timpf, S. A framework for assessing the salience of landmarks for wayfinding tasks. Cogn. Process. 2006, 7, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Neill, M. Effects of signage and floor plan configuration on wayfinding accuracy. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 553–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tom, A.; Denis, M. Referring to landmark or street information in route directions: What difference does it make? In Spatial Information Theory. Foundations of Geographic Information Science; Kuhn, W., Worboys, M.F., Timpf, S., Eds.; COSIT 2003, Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2003; Volume 2825, pp. 362–374. [Google Scholar]
- Burnett, G.; Smith, D.; May, A. Supporting the navigation task: Characteristics of “good” landmarks. In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Ergonomics Society, Turin, Italy, 7–9 November 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, R.; Winter, S. Enriching Wayfinding Instructions with Local Landmarks; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2002; pp. 243–259. [Google Scholar]
- Denis, M.; Pazzaglia, F.; Cornoldi, C.; Bertolo, L. Spatial discourse and navigation: An analysis of route directions in the city of Venice. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 1999, 13, 145–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moeser, S.D. Cognitive Mapping in a Complex Building. Environ. Behav. 1988, 20, 21–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kitchin, R.; Blades, M. The Cognition of Geographic Space; I.B. Tauris: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Casakin, H.; Barkowsky, T.; Kpippel, A.; Freksa, C. Schematic maps as wayfinding aids. In Spatial Cognition II: Integrating Abstract Theories, Empirical Studies, Formal Methods, and Practical Applications; Freksa, C., Brauer, W., Habel, C., Wender, K.F., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2000; pp. 54–71. [Google Scholar]
- Golledge, R.G. Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes; Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Seneviratne, P.N.; Morerall, J.F. Level of service on pedestrian facilities. Transp. Q. 1985, 39, 109–123. [Google Scholar]
- Ueberschaer, M.H. Choice of routes on urban networks for the journey to work. Highw. Res. Rec. 1971, 369, 228–238. [Google Scholar]
- Ito, K.; Sano, Y. Cultural Differences in the Use of Spatial Information in Wayfinding Behavior. In Proceedings of the 25th International Cartographic Conference, Paris, France, 3–8 July 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Koláčný, A. Cartographic information—A fundamental concept and term in modern cartography. Cartogr. J. 1977, 14, 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Elzakker, C.P. The Use of Maps in the Exploration of Geographic Data; Koninklijk Nederlands Aardrijkskundig Genootschap: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2004; p. 19. [Google Scholar]
- Fujimori, F.; Arikawa, M.; Morita, T. Extraction of spatial relations on SVG maps and its applications. In Proceedings of the IEICE DEWS, Kaga, Japan, 3–5 March 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bertin, J. Semiology of Graphics: Diagrams, Networks, Maps; ESRI Press: Redlands, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Tang, C.; Lin, C.; Hsu, Y. Exploratory research on reading cognition and escape-route planning using building evacuation plan diagrams. Appl. Ergon. 2008, 39, 209–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, J.; Olwal, A.; Ishii, H.; Boulanger, C. SpaceTop: Integrating 2D and spatial 3D interactions in a see-through desktop environment. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Paris, France, 27 April–2 May 2013; pp. 189–192. [Google Scholar]
- Sweany, J.B. Cognitive Demand of Engineering Information. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Colorado at Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Blair, J.P.; Schweit, K. A Study of Active Shooter Incidents, 2000–2013; Texas State University and Federal Bureau of Investigation; US Department of Justice: Washington, DC, USA, 2014.
- Blair, J.P.; Nichols, T.; Burns, D.; Curnutt, J.R. Active Shooter Events and Response; CRC Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Teizer, J.; Cheng, T.; Fang, Y. Location tracking and data visualization technology to advance construction ironworkers’ education and training in safety and productivity. Autom. Constr. 2013, 35, 53–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zheng, M.C. Influences of different underground station map designs on map-reading and wayfinding. Geoinformatica 2020, 24, 531–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Passini, R. Wayfinding in Architecture; Van Nostrand Reinhold: New York, NY, USA, 1984; Volume 4.
- You, M.-L.; Chen, C.-W.; Lee, P.-J. Effects of Map View and Landmark Presentation Styles on Usability of Wayfinding Maps. J. Des. 2006, 11, 85–99. [Google Scholar]
- Marr, D.; Vision, A. A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information; Freeman and Company: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Kealy, W.A. Map perspective and the learning of text. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 1995, 20, 457–463. [Google Scholar]
- Kiefer, P.; Giannopoulos, I.; Raubal, M. Where Am I? Investigating Map Matching during Self- Localization with Mobile Eye Tracking in an Urban Environment. Trans. GIS 2014, 18, 660–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Limberger, D.; Scheibel, W.; Trapp, M.; Döllner, J. Mixed-projection treemaps: A novel approach mixing 2d and 2.5 d treemaps. In Proceedings of the 2017 21st International Conference Information Visualisation (IV), London, UK, 11–14 July 2017; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2017; pp. 164–169. [Google Scholar]
- Zanola, S.; Fabrikant, S.I.; Çöltekin, A. The Effect of Realism on the Confidence in Spatial Data Quality in Stereoscopic 3D Displays. In Proceedings of the 24th International Cartography Conference (ICC 2009), Santiago, Chile, 15–21 November 2009; pp. 15–21. [Google Scholar]
- Liao, H.; Dong, W.; Peng, C.; Liu, H. Exploring differences of visual attention in pedestrian navigation when using 2D maps and 3D geo-browsers. Cartogr. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2017, 44, 474–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xia, N.K.; Tian, P.J. Detailed Control Planning; Tongji University Press: Shanghai, China, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Kitchin, R.M.; Jacobson, R.D. Techniques to collect and analyze the cognitive map knowledge of persons with visual impairment or blindness: Issues of validity. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 1997, 91, 360–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apelt, R. Wayfinding in the Built Environment; Tech. Rep.; Public Works Queensland: Brisbane, QLD, Australia, 2008.
Group A (Opaque and without Lines) | Group B (Transparent and without Lines) |
Group C (Opaque and with Lines) | Group D (Transparent and with Lines) |
Code | Description | H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
WD-1 | Left and right turns | All participants | 15% | 14% | 17% | 19% | 16% | 19% | 100% |
% within the group | 54% | 65% | 58% | 57% | 73% | 66% | 374% | ||
WD-2 | Intersection | All participants | 13% | 10% | 22% | 25% | 11% | 18% | 100% |
% within the group | 19% | 19% | 29% | 29% | 19% | 25% | 140% | ||
WD-3 | Block | All participants | 24% | 10% | 5% | 62% | 0% | 0% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 1% | 0% | 3% | 0% | 0% | 6% | ||
WD-4 | Main roads | All participants | 19% | 33% | 22% | 4% | 22% | 0% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 3% | 2% | 0% | 2% | 0% | 9% | ||
WD-5 | Going straight to the end | All participants | 18% | 14% | 24% | 19% | 8% | 18% | 100% |
% within the group | 9% | 9% | 11% | 8% | 5% | 9% | 50% | ||
WD-6 | Confusion | All participants | 69% | 12% | 0% | 14% | 4% | 0% | 100% |
% within the group | 10% | 2% | 0% | 2% | 1% | 0% | 15% | ||
WD-7 | Unnoticed or ignored | All participants | 71% | 14% | 0% | 10% | 0% | 5% | 100% |
% within the group | 5% | 1% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 0% | 6% | ||
total | All route planning descriptions | 33% | 15% | 13% | 22% | 9% | 8% | 100% | |
% within the group | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 600% |
Code | Description | H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M1 | Building | All participants | 24% | 27% | 22% | 8% | 14% | 5% | 100% |
% within the group | 19% | 20% | 13% | 8% | 11% | 5% | 75% | ||
M2 | Graphic memory | All participants | 17% | 20% | 24% | 14% | 12% | 14% | 100% |
% within the group | 21% | 24% | 22% | 21% | 16% | 19% | 122% | ||
M3 | Relative position | All participants | 16% | 18% | 18% | 16% | 21% | 12% | 100% |
% within the group | 23% | 24% | 19% | 29% | 32% | 19% | 145% | ||
M4 | Doubling back | All participants | 17% | 21% | 21% | 23% | 6% | 13% | 100% |
% within the group | 17% | 20% | 16% | 29% | 7% | 14% | 102% | ||
M5 | Silent rehearsal | All participants | 13% | 9% | 26% | 7% | 20% | 25% | 100% |
% within the group | 21% | 14% | 31% | 13% | 34% | 44% | 157% | ||
total | All participants | 17% | 19% | 22% | 13% | 14% | 14% | 100% | |
% within the group | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 600% |
Route Planning | Wayfinding Simulation | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Task | H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Average | H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Average |
1 | 229.7″ | 225.3″ | 190.0″ | 209.3″ | 227.0″ | 238.3″ | 219.9″ | 286.8″ | 295.2″ | 373.8″ | 160.2″ | 201.8″ | 214.3″ | 255.4″ |
2 | 232.7″ | 199.2″ | 197.2″ | 216.7″ | 215.3″ | 216.7″ | 212.9″ | 143.0″ | 118.8″ | 113.5″ | 116.8″ | 115.5″ | 143.7″ | 125.2″ |
3 | 233.3″ | 236.5″ | 227.0″ | 229.3″ | 229.0″ | 234.5″ | 231.6″ | 216.3″ | 166.3″ | 235.8″ | 243.3″ | 227.5″ | 191.2″ | 213.4″ |
4 | 224.3″ | 219.3″ | 230.8″ | 221.8″ | 220.5″ | 218.5″ | 222.6″ | 157.3″ | 168.0″ | 164.2″ | 138.2″ | 128.6″ | 136.5″ | 148.8″ |
average | 230.0″ | 220.1″ | 211.3″ | 219.3″ | 223.0″ | 227.0″ | 221.8″ | 200.9″ | 187.1″ | 221.8″ | 164.6″ | 168.4″ | 171.4″ | 185.7″ |
220.4″ | 223.1″ | 203.3″ | 168.1″ |
Code | Behavior | H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
WK-1 | Confusion and hesitation | All participants | 20.3% | 14.4% | 20.9% | 17.6% | 10.5% | 16.3% | 100% |
% within the group | 32.6% | 45.8% | 27.8% | 43.5% | 33.3% | 36.8% | 220% | ||
WK-2 | Doubling back | All participants | 23.2% | 8.5% | 28.5% | 13.4% | 11.4% | 15.0% | 100% |
% within the group | 60.0% | 43.8% | 60.9% | 53.2% | 58.3% | 54.4% | 331% | ||
WK-3 | Going off-route | All participants | 18.9% | 13.5% | 35.1% | 5.4% | 10.8% | 16.2% | 100% |
% within the group | 7.4% | 10.4% | 11.3% | 3.2% | 8.3% | 8.8% | 49% | ||
Total | All wayfinding behavior | 20.8% | 12.1% | 28.2% | 12.2% | 10.9% | 15.9% | 100% | |
% within the group | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 600% | ||
Average, high-density maps | Confusion and hesitation | 18.5% | 61% | ||||||
Doubling back | 20.1% | ||||||||
Going off-route | 22.5% | ||||||||
Average, low-density maps | Confusion and hesitation | 14.8% | 39% | ||||||
Doubling back | 13.3% | ||||||||
Going off-route | 10.8% |
H30° | H60° | H90° | L30° | L60° | L90° | Total | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Consistency between taken route and cognitive map | All participants | 16.7% | 29.2% | 25.0% | 33.3% | 41.7% | 12.5% | 26.4% |
Orientation error rate | All participants | 7.3% | 7.0% | 8.8% | 8.3% | 7.5% | 9.4% | 48.2% |
Average, high-density maps | Consistency between taken route and cognitive map | 23.6% | ||||||
Orientation error rate | 7.7% | |||||||
Average, low-density maps | Consistency between taken route and cognitive map | 29.2% | ||||||
Orientation error rate | 8.4% |
Code | Behavior | 30° | 60° | 90° | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
WK-1 | Confusion and hesitation | All participants | 37.9% | 24.8% | 37.3% | 100% |
WK-2 | Doubling back | All participants | 36.6% | 19.9% | 43.5% | 100% |
WK-3 | Going off-route | All participants | 24.3% | 24.3% | 51.4% | 100% |
Total | Wayfinding behavior | 32.9% | 23.0% | 44.0% | 100% |
30° | 60° | 90° | Total | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Consistency between taken route and cognitive map | All participants | 50.0% | 70.8% | 37.5% | 53% |
Orientation error rate | All participants | 15.6% | 14.5% | 18.1% | 16% |
Code | Description | Group A (Opaque) | Group B (Opaque with Lines) | Group C (Transparent) | Group D (Transparent with Lines) | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
WD-1 | Confusion and hesitation | All participants | 31% | 38% | 15% | 15% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 3% | 1% | 1% | 6% | ||
WD-2 | Landmark | All participants | 31% | 18% | 16% | 35% | 100% |
% within the group | 26% | 18% | 15% | 22% | 80% | ||
WD-3 | Left and right turns | All participants | 23% | 20% | 24% | 33% | 100% |
% within the group | 24% | 26% | 28% | 27% | 105% | ||
WD-4 | Intersection | All participants | 17% | 22% | 23% | 38% | 100% |
% within the group | 10% | 15% | 15% | 17% | 57% | ||
WD-5 | Alley | All participants | 29% | 29% | 25% | 17% | 100% |
% within the group | 3% | 4% | 3% | 1% | 11% | ||
WD-6 | Relative position | All participants | 27% | 18% | 16% | 39% | 100% |
% within the group | 5% | 4% | 3% | 6% | 19% | ||
WD-7 | Double back | All participants | 19% | 16% | 30% | 35% | 100% |
% within the group | 4% | 4% | 6% | 5% | 19% | ||
WD-8 | Distance | All participants | 26% | 26% | 42% | 5% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 3% | 4% | 0% | 9% | ||
WD-9 | Going straight to the end | All participants | 24% | 40% | 12% | 24% | 100% |
% within the group | 3% | 5% | 1% | 2% | 11% | ||
WD-10 | Main road | All participants | 21% | 18% | 29% | 32% | 100% |
% within the group | 3% | 3% | 5% | 4% | 15% | ||
WD-11 | Following the road | All participants | 29% | 21% | 22% | 29% | 100% |
% within the group | 16% | 14% | 14% | 12% | 56% | ||
WD-12 | Shape | All participants | 17% | 21% | 33% | 29% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 3% | 4% | 2% | 11% | ||
total | All route planning descriptions | 24% | 24% | 24% | 28% | 100% | |
% within the group | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 400% |
Group A (Opaque) | Group B (Opaque and with Lines) | Group C (Transparent) | Group D (Transparent and with Lines) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total Time | 23′31″ | 24′57″ | 25′58″ | 21′38″ |
Task1 | 04′15″ | 03′22″ | 03′32″ | 02′44″ |
Task2 | 04′34″ | 05′11″ | 05′26″ | 04′36″ |
Task3 | 03′17″ | 03′48″ | 03′11″ | 02′42″ |
Task4 | 02′58″ | 03′37″ | 03′38″ | 03′10″ |
Task5 | 04′28″ | 05′02″ | 05′39″ | 04′15″ |
Back to start | 03′59″ | 03′57″ | 04′33″ | 04′12″ |
Code | Description | Group A (Opaque) | Group B (Opaque with Lines) | Group C (Transparent) | Group D (Transparent with Lines) | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BC | Confusion and hesitation | All participants | 24% | 36% | 29% | 11% | 100% |
% within the group | 5% | 5% | 5% | 2% | 17% | ||
BB | Doubling back | All participants | 30% | 39% | 25% | 7% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 2% | 2% | 0% | 7% | ||
BTW | Looking around horizontally | All participants | 22% | 30% | 23% | 25% | 100% |
% within the group | 21% | 22% | 19% | 22% | 84% | ||
BWM | Looking at the map | All participants | 23% | 28% | 26% | 24% | 100% |
% within the group | 41% | 38% | 40% | 38% | 157% | ||
BUW | Looking up and down | All participants | 21% | 26% | 25% | 28% | 100% |
% within the group | 6% | 5% | 6% | 7% | 24% | ||
BRM | Spinning the map | All participants | 21% | 22% | 30% | 28% | 100% |
% within the group | 11% | 9% | 13% | 13% | 45% | ||
BN | Confirming the current location | All participants | 22% | 26% | 24% | 29% | 100% |
% within the group | 9% | 8% | 8% | 10% | 35% | ||
BR | Spinning the body | All participants | 18% | 33% | 25% | 24% | 100% |
% within the group | 4% | 5% | 4% | 4% | 17% | ||
BL | Becoming lost | All participants | 33% | 33% | 25% | 8% | 100% |
% within the group | 1% | 1% | 0% | 0% | 2% | ||
BM | Keeping track | All participants | 11% | 47% | 20% | 22% | 100% |
% within the group | 2% | 6% | 3% | 3% | 14% | ||
total | All wayfinding performances | 22% | 32% | 25% | 21% | 100% | |
% within the group | 100% | 100% | 100% | 100% | 400% |
Group A (Opaque) | Group B (Opaque and with Lines) |
Group C (Transparent) | Group D (Transparent and with Lines) |
Task | Recognition Points | Group A (Opaque) | Group B (Opaque with Lines) | Group C (Transparent) | Group D (Transparent with Lines) | Average AAS |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | start | 22.7 | 9.4 | 5.5 | 3.1 | 10.2 |
2 | start | 8.6 | 25.0 | 14.8 | 32.8 | 20.3 |
1 | 11.7 | 27.3 | 14.1 | 33.6 | 21.7 | |
3 | start | 44.5 | 71.9 | 54.7 | 39.1 | 52.5 |
2 | 49.2 | 62.5 | 82.0 | 46.9 | 60.2 | |
4 | start | 29.7 | 27.3 | 25.0 | 15.6 | 24.4 |
3 | 40.6 | 8.6 | 22.7 | 18.0 | 22.5 | |
5 | start | 33.6 | 10.2 | 7.0 | 3.9 | 13.7 |
4 | 19.5 | 7.8 | 8.6 | 12.5 | 12.1 | |
end | 5 | 6.3 | 18.0 | 10.2 | 11.7 | 11.5 |
Average AAS | 26.6 | 26.8 | 24.5 | 21.7 | 24.9 |
Group A | Group B | Group C | Group D | Percentage | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
B > A > D > C | 3 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 13% |
B > D > A > C | 1 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 3% |
C > D > A > B | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 | 6% |
C > D > B > A | 1 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 9% |
D > B > A > C | 0 | 0 | 0 | 2 | 6% |
D > B > C > A | 1 | 2 | 0 | 2 | 16% |
D > C > A > B | 0 | 0 | 2 | 1 | 9% |
D > C > B > A | 1 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 38% |
total | 8 | 8 | 8 | 8 | 100% |
No. | Topic | Group A (Opaque) and Group B (Opaque with Lines) | Group C (Transparent) and Group D (Transparent with Lines) | Group A (Opaque) and Group C (Transparent) | Group B (Opaque with Lines) and Group D (Transparent with Lines) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | I prefer planimetric maps over the experiment maps | 1.000 | 0.512 | 0.287 | 0.387 |
2 | I think that the experiment maps were useful for orientation | 0.657 | 0.334 | 0.246 | 0.278 |
3 | I think that the experiment maps were easy to understand | 0.582 | 0.031 * | 0.016 * | 0.875 |
4 | I think that the experiment maps facilitate wayfinding | 0.306 | 0.219 | 0.116 | 0.849 |
5 | I think that the overall layout of this map is crowded | 0.256 | 0.268 | 0.082 | 0.590 |
6 | I think that the buildings on the map increased the complexity of wayfinding | 0.678 | 0.387 | 0.678 | 0.387 |
7 | I think that the buildings on this map were useful for orientation | 0.849 | 0.512 | 0.152 | 0.126 |
8 | I think that the transparency in this map increased the complexity of wayfinding | 0.015 * | 0.758 | 0.577 | 0.063 |
9 | I think that the transparency in this map enabled easier identification of landmarks | 0.167 | 0.140 | 0.018 * | 0.876 |
10 | I think that this map is easy to use | 0.362 | 0.016 * | 0.021 * | 0.349 |
11 | I think that spatial transformation between this map and the actual environment is easy | 0.908 | 0.219 | 0.082 | 0.066 |
Comparison Item | Ranking | Value |
---|---|---|
Expressions of confusion and hesitation during route planning | B > A > C = D | 38% > 31% > 15% = 15% |
Wayfinding time | C > B > A > D | 25:58 > 24:57 > 23:31 > 21:38 |
Average frequency of ineffective wayfinding behavior: confusion/hesitation and becoming lost | B > A > C > D | 36% > 29% > 27% > 10% |
AAS | B > A > C > D | 26.8 > 26.6 > 24.5 > 21.7 |
Cognitive map: intersection errors | C > A > B = D | 15 > 11 > 3 = 3 |
Consistency between the taken and planned route | D > C > A > B | 88% > 79% > 77% > 71% |
Consistency between taken route and cognitive map | A > D > B > C | 92% > 90% > 88% > 83% |
Evaluation after map use | D > A > C > B | 42 > 34 > 25 > 19 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zheng, M.-C.; Hsu, Y.-W. How 2.5D Maps Design Improve the Wayfinding Performance and Spatial Ability of Map Users. Informatics 2021, 8, 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/informatics8040088
Zheng M-C, Hsu Y-W. How 2.5D Maps Design Improve the Wayfinding Performance and Spatial Ability of Map Users. Informatics. 2021; 8(4):88. https://doi.org/10.3390/informatics8040088
Chicago/Turabian StyleZheng, Meng-Cong, and Yi-Wen Hsu. 2021. "How 2.5D Maps Design Improve the Wayfinding Performance and Spatial Ability of Map Users" Informatics 8, no. 4: 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/informatics8040088
APA StyleZheng, M. -C., & Hsu, Y. -W. (2021). How 2.5D Maps Design Improve the Wayfinding Performance and Spatial Ability of Map Users. Informatics, 8(4), 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/informatics8040088