Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Factors in Early Vocabulary Acquisition
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Sociodemographic Variables Related to Language Acquisition
1.2. Pre-Linguistic Factors
1.2.1. First Signs of Understanding
1.2.2. Imitation
1.2.3. Gestures and Actions
1.3. Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Which of the Sociodemographic or Pre-Linguistic Variable(s) Studied Explain Early Vocabulary Acquisition?
3.2. Which of the Sociodemographic or Pre-Linguistic Variable(s) Studied Discriminate Children with a High Level of Vocabulary from Those with a Low Level of Vocabulary?
4. Discussion
4.1. Initial Vocabulary
4.2. Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Predictors of Early Vocabulary
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Model | Predictors | Coefficient (95% CI) | β | p | AdjR2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Imitation | 1.31 (1.18 to 1.45) | 0.677 | <0.001 | 0.456 |
2 | Imitation | 0.94 (0.82 to 1.07) | 0.486 | <0.001 | 0.622 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.07 (0.06 to 0.08) | 0.450 | <0.001 | ||
3 | Imitation | 0.90 (0.78 to 1.02) | 0.464 | <0.001 | 0.637 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.05 (0.04 to 0.06) | 0.327 | <0.001 | ||
Age | 0.11 (0.06 to 0.16) | 0.183 | <0.001 | ||
4 | Imitation | 0.87 (0.74 to 0.99) | 0.477 | <0.001 | 0.645 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.04 (0.03 to 0,05) | 0.266 | <0.001 | ||
Age | 0.08 (0.03 to 0.13) | 0.142 | 0.001 | ||
Phrases | 0.03 (0.01 to 0.05) | 0.139 | 0.001 | ||
5 | Imitation | 0.86 (0.74 to 0.99) | 0.445 | <0.001 | 0.648 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.04 (0.03 to 0.05) | 0.270 | <0.001 | ||
Age | 0.09 (0.04 to 0.14) | 0.154 | 0.001 | ||
Phrases | 0.03 (0.01 to 0.04) | 0.129 | 0.003 | ||
Birth order | −0.14 (−0.27 to −0.01) | −0.063 | 0.032 | ||
6 | Imitation | 0.85 (0.73 to 0.98) | 0.445 | <0.001 | 0.651 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.04 (0.03 to 0.05) | 0.245 | <0.001 | ||
Age | 0.1 (0.05 to 0.15) | 0.167 | <0.001 | ||
Phrases | 0.03 (0.01 to 0.04) | 0.140 | 0.002 | ||
Birth order | −0.15 (−0.28 to −0.02) | −0.081 | 0.021 | ||
Birth weight | 0.19 (0.02 to 0.36) | 0.063 | 0.031 |
References
- Bergelson, E.; Swingley, D. At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 3253–3258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pruden, S.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hennon, E.A. The Birth of Words: Ten-Month-Olds Learn Words Through Perceptual Salience. Child Dev. 2006, 77, 266–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hollich, G.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M.I. What Does It Take to Learn a Word? Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2000, 65, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levine, D.; Strother-Garcia, K.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. Language Development in the First Year of Life. Otol. Neurotol. 2016, 37, e56–e62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lederberg, A.R.; Schick, B.; Spencer, P.E. Language and literacy development of deaf and hard-of-hearing children: Successes and challenges. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 15–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anastasi, A. Differential Psychology, 3th ed.; MacMiilan: New York, NY, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Zambrana, I.M.; Pons, F.; Eadie, P.A.; Ystrom, E. Trajectories of language delay from age 3 to 5: Persistence, recovery and late onset. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2014, 49, 304–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Etchell, A.; Adhikari, A.; Weinberg, L.S.; Choo, A.L.; Garnett, E.O.; Chow, H.M.; Chang, S.E. A systematic literature review of sex differences in childhood language and brain development. Neuropsychologia 2018, 114, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norbury, C.F.; Gooch, D.C.; Wray, C.; Baird, G.; Charman, T.; Simonoff, E.; Vamvakas, G.; Pickles, A. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 57, 1247–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Huttenlocher, J.; Haight, W.; Bryk, A.; Seltzer, M.; Lyons, T. Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 236–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galsworthy, M.J.; Dionne, G.; Dale, P.S.; Plomin, R. Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Dev. Sci. 2000, 3, 206–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenson, L.; Marchman, V.A.; Thal, D.J.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Bates, E. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.; Paul H. Brooks: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Berglund, E.; Eriksson, M.; Westerlund, M. Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month-old children. Scand. J. Psychol. 2005, 46, 485–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenson, L.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Bates, E.; Thal, D.J.; Pethick, S.J.; Tomasello, M.; Mervis, C.B.; Stiles, J. Variability in Early Communicative Development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994, 59, i. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenson, L.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Donna, T.; Elisabeth, B.; Hartung, J.P.; Pethick, S.J.; Reilly, J.; Thal, D.J.; Bates, E.; et al. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual; Singular Publishing Group: San Diego, CA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Eriksson, M.; Marschik, P.B.; Tulviste, T.; Almgren, M.; Pérez-Pereira, M.; Wehberg, S.; Marjanovič-Umek, L.; Gayraud, F.; Kovačević, M.; Gallego, C. Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 30, 326–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovačević, M.; Cepanec, M.; Kuvač Kraljević, J. Sex differences in lexical and grammatical development in Croatian. In Proceedings from the First European Network Meeting on the Communicative Development Inventories; Marten, E., Ed.; Gävle University Press: Gävle, Švedska, 2007; pp. 5–15. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson-Maldonado, D.; Thal, D.J.; Marchman, V.; Newton, T.; Fenson, L.; Conboy, B. McArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas. User’s Guide and Technical Manual; Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Berglund, E.; Eriksson, M. Communicative Development in Swedish Children 16-28 months old: The Swedish Early Communicative Development Inventory—Words and Sentences. Scand. J. Psychol. 2000, 41, 133–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosch, L.; Agut Quijano, T.; Busquets Ferrer, L. Prematuridad y dificultades del lenguaje. In Dificultades del Habla, el Lenguaje y la Comunicación en los Trastornos del Neuro-Desarrollo; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Igualada, A., Eds.; UOC: Barcelona, Spain, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- McKean, C.; Wraith, D.; Eadie, P.A.; Cook, F.; Mensah, F.; Reilly, S. Subgroups in language trajectories from 4 to 11 years: The nature and predictors of stable, improving and decreasing language trajectory groups. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 10, 1081–1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resches, M.; Pérez-Pereira, M.; Cruz Guerrero, R.; Fernández Prieto, M. Risk for language delay in healthy preterm and full-term children. In Atypical Language Development in Romance Languages; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E.M., Buil-Legaz, L., López-Penadés, R., Sánchez-Azanza, V.A., Adrover-Roig, D., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; pp. 57–74. [Google Scholar]
- Adams-Chapman, I.; Bann, C.; Carter, S.L.; Stoll, B.J. Language outcomes among ELBW infants in early childhood. Early Hum. Dev. 2015, 91, 373–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pérez-Pereira, M.; Fernández, P.; Gómez-Taibo, M.L.; Resches, M. Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30months. Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Robertson, C.M.T.; Watt, M.-J.; Dinu, I.A. Outcomes for the Extremely Premature Infant: What Is New? and Where Are We Going? Pediatr. Neurol. 2009, 40, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hooper, S.R.; Ashley, T.A.; Roberts, J.E.; Zeisel, S.A.; Poe, M.D. The relationship of otitis media in early childhood to attention dimensions during the early elementary school years. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, J.E.; Wallace, I.; Henderson, D.E. LANGUAGE and otitis media. In Otitis Media in Young Children: Medical, Developmental, and Educational Considerations; Roberts, J.E., Wallace, I.F., Henderson, F.W., Eds.; Paul H Brookes Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1997; pp. 133–161. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, J.E.; Rosenfeld, R.M.; Zeisel, S.A. Otitis media and speech and language: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Pediatrics 2004, 113, e238–e248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rosenfeld, R.M.; Shin, J.J.; Schwartz, S.R.; Coggins, R.; Gagnon, L.; Hackell, J.M.; Hoelting, D.; Hunter, L.; Kummer, A.W.; Payne, S.C.; et al. Clinical Practice Guideline: Otitis Media with Effusion (Update). Otolaryngol. Neck Surg. 2016, 154, S1–S41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff-Ginsberg, E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1998, 19, 603–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pine, J.M. Variation in Vocabulary Development as a Function of Birth Order. Child Dev. 1995, 66, 272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oshima-Takane, Y.; Goodz, E.; Derevensky, J.L. Birth Order Effects on Early Language Development: Do Secondborn Children Learn from Overheard Speech? Child Dev. 1996, 67, 621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Auza-Benavides, A.; Peñaloza, C.; Murata, C. The influence of maternal education on the linguistic abilities of monolingual Spanish-speaking children with and without Specific Language Impairment. Atyp. Lang. Dev. Roman. Lang. 2019, 93–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conti-Ramsden, G.; Durkin, K. What factors influence language impairment? Considering resilience as well as risk. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2016, 67, 293–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilkerson, J.; Richards, J.A.; Warren, S.F.; Montgomery, J.K.; Greenwood, C.R.; Kimbrough Oller, D.; Hansen, J.H.L.; Paul, T.D. Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2017, 26, 248–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Adamson, L.B.; Bakeman, R.; Owen, M.T.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Pace, A.; Yust, P.K.S.; Suma, K. The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 1071–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Romeo, R.R.; Leonard, J.A.; Robinson, S.T.; West, M.R.; Mackey, A.P.; Rowe, M.L.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Beyond the 30-Million-Word Gap: Children’s Conversational Exposure Is Associated With Language-Related Brain Function. Psychol. Sci. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sultana, N.; Wong, L.L.N.; Purdy, S.C. Natural Language Input: Maternal Education, Socioeconomic Deprivation, and Language Outcomes in Typically Developing Children. Lang. Speech Heart Serv. Sch. 2020, 51, 1049–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sperry, D.E.; Sperry, L.L.; Miller, P.J. Language Does Matter: But There is More to Language Than Vocabulary and Directed Speech. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 993–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sperry, D.E.; Sperry, L.L.; Miller, P.J. Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 1303–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golinkoff, R.M.; Hoff, E.; Rowe, M.L.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. Language Matters: Denying the Existence of the 30-Million-Word Gap Has Serious Consequences. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 985–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lowry, L. Closing the 30 million word gap: It’s about quality, not just quantity. Hanen Early Lang. Progr. 2016, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
- Maria-Mengel, M.R.S.; Linhares, M.B.M. Risk factors for infant developmental problems. Rev. Lat. Am. Enferm. 2007, 15, 837–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dollaghan, C.; Campbell, T.F.; Paradise, J.L.; Feldman, H.M.; Janosky, J.E.; Pitcairn, D.N.; Kurs-Lasky, M. Maternal education and measures of early speech and language. J. Speech Lang. Heart Res. 1999, 42, 1432–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feldman, H.M.; Dollaghan, C.A.; Campbell, T.F.; Kurs-Lasky, M.; Janosky, J.E.; Paradise, J.L. Measurement Properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at Ages One and Two Years. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 310–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson-Maldonado, D.; Barcenas Acosta, R.P. Assessing early communicative abilities in Spanish-speaking children from low-SES families. Zero Three 2006, 27, 22–27. [Google Scholar]
- Papoušek, M.A.; Papoušek, H.B. Early verbalizations as precursors of language development. In Infant Development: Perspectives From German-Speaking Countries; Taylor and Francis: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 299–328. [Google Scholar]
- Serra, M.; Serrat, E.; Solé Planas, R.; Bel Leal, A.; Aparici, M. La Adquisición del Lenguaje; Ariel: Barcelona, Spain, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- DeCasper, A.; Fifer, W. Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 1980, 208, 1174–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hepper, P.G.; Scott, D.; Shahidullah, S. Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 1993, 11, 147–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehler, J.; Christophe, A. Maturation and learning of language in the first year of life. In The Cognitive Neurosciences: A Handbook for the Field; Gazzaniga, M.S., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 943–954. [Google Scholar]
- Saffran, J.R.; Aslin, R.N.; Newport, E.L. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants. Science 1996, 274, 1926–1928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Saffran, J.R.; Newport, E.L.; Aslin, R.N. Word Segmentation: The Role of Distributional Cues. J. Mem. Lang. 1996, 35, 606–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mampe, B.; Friederici, A.D.; Christophe, A.; Wermke, K. Newborns’ Cry Melody Is Shaped by Their Native Language. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, 1994–1997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whalen, D.H.; Levitt, A.G.; Wang, Q. Intonational differences between the reduplicative babbling of French- and English-learning infants. J. Child Lang. 1991, 18, 501–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sanz-Torrent, M.; Andreu, L. La adquisición del léxico. In La Adquisición del Lenguaje; Pearson Educación: Madrid, Spain, 2018; pp. 90–114. [Google Scholar]
- Dosman, C.F.; Andrews, D.; Goulden, K.J. Evidence-based milestone ages as a framework for developmental surveillance. Paediatr. Child Health 2012, 17, 561–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rossetti, L. The Rossetti Infant-Toddler Language Scale: A Measure of Communication and Interzaction; LinguiSystems: East Moline, IL, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Sax, N.; Weston, E. Language Development Milestones; University of Alberta: Alberta, Canada, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Gertner, Y.; Fisher, C.; Eisengart, J. Learning Words and Rules. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 684–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R.M. The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D.V.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T. CATALISE consortium CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. 1. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fish, M.; Pinkerman, B. Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian children: Normative development and individual differences, infancy to preschool. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 23, 539–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watt, N.; Wetherby, A.M.; Shumway, S. Prelinguistic predictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. J. Speech Lang. Heart Res. 2006, 49, 1224–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wetherby, A.M.; Prizant, B.M. Communication and Symbolic Behaviour Scales–Infant/Toddler Checklist; Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Zwaigenbaum, L.; Bryson, S.; Rogers, T.; Roberts, W.; Brian, J.; Szatmari, P. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2005, 23, 143–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luyster, R.; Lopez, K.; Lord, C. Characterizing communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). J. Child Lang. 2007, 34, 623–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Charman, T.; Drew, A.; Baird, C.; Baird, G. Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). J. Child Lang. 2003, 30, 213–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meltzoff, A.N. Imitation and other minds: The “Like Me” hypothesis. In Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science; Hurley, S., Chater, N., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2005; Volume 2, pp. 55–77. [Google Scholar]
- Saville-Troike, M. Private speech: Evidence for second language learning strategies during the ‘silent’ period. J. Child Lang. 1988, 15, 567–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Snow, C.E. The uses of imitation. J. Child Lang. 1981, 8, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ervin-Tripp, S. Imitation and structural change in children’s language. In New Directions in the Study of Child Language; Lennenberg, E.H., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Rood, L. Braine Children’s imitations of syntactic constructions as a measure of linguistic competence. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1970, 10, 430–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M.D.; Chomsky, N.; Fitch, T. The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science 2002, 298, 1569–1579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramer, A.L.H. Syntactic styles in emerging language. J. Child Lang. 1976, 3, 49–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodgon, M.M.; Kurdek, L.A. Vocal and Gestural Imitation in 8-, 14-, and 20-Month-Old Children. J. Genet. Psychol. 1977, 131, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bloom, L.; Hood, L.; Lightbown, P. Imitation in language development: If, when, and why. Cogn. Psychol. 1974, 6, 380–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moerk, E.L. Pragmatic and Semantic Aspects of Early Language Development; University Park Press: Baltimore, MD, USA; London, UK; Tokyo, Japan, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Masur, E.F.; Olson, J. Mothers’ and infants’ responses to their partners’ spontaneous action and vocal/verbal imitation. Infant Behav. Dev. 2008, 31, 704–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Feeley, K.; Jones, E. Strategies to address challenging behaviour in young children with Down syndrome. Down Syndr. Res. Pract. 2008, 12, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Yoder, P.J.; Layton, T.L. Speech following sign language training in autistic children with minimal verbal language. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1988, 18, 217–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, V.; Mirenda, P.; Zaidman-Zait, A. Predictors of Expressive Vocabulary Growth in Children With Autism. J. Speech Lang. Heart Res. 2007, 50, 149–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Over, H.; Gattis, M. Verbal imitation is based on intention understanding. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 25, 46–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bannard, C.; Klinger, J.; Tomasello, M. How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material? Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 2344–2356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Branigan, H.P.; Pickering, M.J.; Cleland, A.A. Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition 2000, 75, B13–B25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garrod, S.; Anderson, A. Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition 1987, 27, 181–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Garrod, S.; Clark, A. The development of dialogue co-ordination skills in schoolchildren. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1993, 8, 101–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garrod, S.; Doherty, G. Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition 1994, 53, 181–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matthews, D.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 749–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bates, E. Language in Context; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Iverson, J.M.; Goldin-Meadow, S. Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 367–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gullberg, M.; de Bot, K.; Volterra, V. Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development. Gesture 2008, 8, 149–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Esteve Gibert, N.; Igualada, A.; Prieto, P. El gest com a facilitador i precursor del desenvolupament del llenguatge. Leng. Soc. Commun. 2019, 17, 26–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McGillion, M.; Herbert, J.S.; Pine, J.M.; Vihman, M.M.; DePaolis, R.; Keren-Portnoy, T.; Matthews, D. What Paves the Way to Conventional Language? The Predictive Value of Babble, Pointing, and Socioeconomic Status. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, K. Young Minds in Social Worlds: Experience, Meaning, and Memory; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Cadime, I.; Silva, C.; Ribeiro, I.; Viana, F.L. Early lexical development: Do day care attendance and maternal education matter? First Lang. 2018, 38, 503–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lillard, A.S.; Lerner, M.D.; Hopkins, E.J.; Dore, R.A.; Smith, E.D.; Palmquist, C.M. The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Quinn, S.; Donnelly, S.; Kidd, E. The relationship between symbolic play and language acquisition: A meta-analytic review. Dev. Rev. 2018, 49, 121–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M.; Mervis, C.B. The instrument is great, but measuring comprehension is still a problem. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994, 59, 174–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serrat, E.; Sanz-Torrent, M.; Badia, I.; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E.; Olmo, R.; Lara Díaz, M.F.; Andreu, L.; Serra, M. The relationship between lexical learning and grammatical development. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2010, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Serrat-Sellabona, E.; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E.; Sanz-Torrent, M.; Andreu, L.; Amadó, A.; Badia, I.; Serra, M. Inventaris del Desenvolupament D’habilitats Comunicatives MacArthur-Bates en Català. Guia d’ús i Manual Tècnic; UOC: Barcelona, Spain, in press.
- Can, D.D.; Ginsbrug-Block, M.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. A long-term predictive validity study: Can the CDI Short Form be used to predict language and early literacy skills four years later? J. Child Lang. 2013, 40, 821–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez-Pereira, M.; Resches, M. Concurrent and predictive validity of the Galician CDI. J. Child Lang. 2011, 38, 121–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchman, V.A.; Martínez-Sussmann, C. Concurrent Validity of Caregiver/Parent Report Measures of Language for Children Who Are Learning Both English and Spanish. J. Speech Lang. Heart Res. 2002, 45, 983–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thordardottir, E.T.; Weismer, S.E.; Evans, J.L. Continuity in lexical and morphological development in Icelandic and English-speaking 2-year-olds. First Lang. 2002, 22, 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloom, L. One Word at a Time: The Use of Single-Word Utterances before Syntax; Mouton: The Hague, The Netherlands, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, K. Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1973, 21, 1–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dromi, E. Esarly Lexical Development; C.U.P.: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Serra, M.; Sanz-Torrent, M. Las 400 palabras: Datos para una perspectiva constructivista de la interfaz léxico-sintáctica. Annu. Psicol./UB J. Psychol. 2004, 35, 235–255. [Google Scholar]
- Goldfield, B.A.; Reznick, J.S. Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. J. Child Lang. 1990, 17, 171–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bronfenbrenner, U. The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1979; ISBN 0-674-22457-4. [Google Scholar]
- Duranti, A.; Ochs, E.; Schieffelin, B.B. The Handbook of Language Socialization; Wiley Blackwell: West Sussex, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Tomblin, J.B. The effect of birth order on the occurrence of developmental language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 1990, 25, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martínez-Nadal, S.; Bosch, L. Cognitive and learning outcomes in late preterm infants at school age: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gould, J.; Ashton-Smith, J. Missed diagnosis or misdiagnosis? Girls and women on the autism spectrum. Good Autism Pract. 2011, 12, 34–41. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehouse, A.J.O. Is There a Sex Ratio Difference in the Familial Aggregation of Specific Language Impairment? A Meta-Analysis. J. Speech Lang. Heart Res. 2010, 53, 1015–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valera-Pozo, M.; Adrover-Roig, D.; Pérez-Castelló, J.A.; Sanchez-Azanza, V.A.; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E. Behavioral, emotional and school adjustment in adolescents with and without developmental language disorder (DLD) is related to family involvement. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cartmill, E.A.; Armstrong, B.F.; Gleitman, L.R.; Goldin-Meadow, S.; Medina, T.N.; Trueswell, J.C. Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 11278–11283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ferjan Ramírez, N.; Lytle, S.R.; Kuhl, P.K. Parent coaching increases conversational turns and advances infant language development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 3484–3491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2405–2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Klinger, J.; Mayor, J.; Bannard, C. Children’s Faithfulness in Imitating Language Use Varies Cross-Culturally, Contingent on Prior Experience. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 820–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M. The social bases of language acquisition. Soc. Dev. 1992, 1, 67–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masur, E.F. Infants’ verbal imitation and their language development: Controversies, techniques and consequences. In Methods for Studying Language Production; Menn, L., Bernstein-Ratner, N., Eds.; LEA: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; pp. 27–43. [Google Scholar]
- Ogura, T. A longitudinal study of the relationship between early language development and play development. J. Child Lang. 1991, 18, 273–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lyytinen, P.; Poikkeus, A.-M.; Laakso, M.-L. Language and Symbolic Play in Toddlers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1997, 21, 289–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCune, L. A normative study of representational play in the transition to language. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 31, 198–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, D.V.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T. CATALISE-2 consortium Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKean, C.; Reilly, S.; Bavin, E.L.; Bretherton, L.; Cini, E.; Conway, L.; Cook, F.; Eadie, P.A.; Prior, M.; Wake, M.; et al. Language outcomes at 7 years: Early predictors and co-occurring difficulties. Pediatrics 2017, 139, e20161684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bauer, S.M.; Jones, E.A. Requesting and Verbal Imitation Intervention for Infants with Down syndrome: Generalization, Intelligibility, and Problem Solving. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 2015, 27, 37–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ingersoll, B.; Schreibman, L. Teaching reciprocal imitation skills to young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on language, pretend play, and joint attention. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006, 36, 487–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sussman, F.; Weitzman, E. Take out the Toys: Building Early Toy Play for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Other Social Communication Challenges; The Hanen Centre: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2014. [Google Scholar]
Personal and Sociodemographic Characteristics | N = 504 |
---|---|
Age in months, M (SD) | 14.23 (2.5) |
Sex as % female | 51.4 |
Birth weight in kg, M (SD) | 3.26 (0.48) |
Number of ear infections per year, M (SD) | 0.52 (1.29) |
Birth order in % of children | |
First | 56.8 |
Second | 36.8 |
Third | 5.4 |
Fourth, onwards | 1 |
Mother’s educational level, % | |
No studies | 0.2 |
Primary | 4.4 |
Secondary | 29 |
University | 66.4 |
Father’s educational level, % | |
No studies | 0.2 |
Primary | 12.3 |
Secondary | 40.9 |
University | 46.6 |
Language Precursors | N = 504 |
---|---|
First signs of understanding, % | |
0 or 1 behaviour | 0.8 |
2 behaviours | 10.6 |
3 behaviours | 88.6 |
Imitation, % | |
Not yet | 40.9 |
Sometimes | 41.9 |
Often | 17.2 |
Phrases, M (SD) | 17.93 (7.09) a |
First gestures, M (SD) | 14.73 (4.12) b |
Late gestures, M (SD) | 17.02 (9.52) c |
Predictors | Coefficient (95% CI) | β | p |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.1 (0.05 to 0.15) | 0.167 | <0.001 |
Birth weight | 0.19 (0.02 to 0.36) | 0.063 | 0.031 |
Birth order | −0.15 (−0.28 to −0.02) | −0.081 | 0.021 |
Imitation | 0.85 (0.73 to 0.98) | 0.445 | <0.001 |
Late gestures and actions | 0.04 (0.03 to 0.05) | 0.245 | <0.001 |
Phrases | 0.03 (0.01 to 0.04) | 0.140 | 0.002 |
Predictors | OR (95% CI) | p |
---|---|---|
First model | ||
Imitation | 6.889 (4.455 to 10.651) | <0.001 |
Second model | ||
Imitation | 5.348 (3.400 to 8.411) | <0.001 |
Late gestures and actions | 1.067 (1.036 to 1.099) | <0.001 |
Observed Percentile | |||
---|---|---|---|
Predicted percentile | <25 | >25 | |
<25 | 69 | 50 | |
>25 | 59 | 250 | |
Sensitivity | Specificity | Accuracy | |
53.9% | 83.3% | 74.5% |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Serrat-Sellabona, E.; Aguilar-Mediavilla, E.; Sanz-Torrent, M.; Andreu, L.; Amadó, A.; Serra, M. Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Factors in Early Vocabulary Acquisition. Children 2021, 8, 206. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030206
Serrat-Sellabona E, Aguilar-Mediavilla E, Sanz-Torrent M, Andreu L, Amadó A, Serra M. Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Factors in Early Vocabulary Acquisition. Children. 2021; 8(3):206. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030206
Chicago/Turabian StyleSerrat-Sellabona, Elisabet, Eva Aguilar-Mediavilla, Mònica Sanz-Torrent, Llorenç Andreu, Anna Amadó, and Miquel Serra. 2021. "Sociodemographic and Pre-Linguistic Factors in Early Vocabulary Acquisition" Children 8, no. 3: 206. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030206