Next Article in Journal
A New Instrument to Assess Children’s Understanding of Death: Psychometrical Properties of the EsCoMu Scale in a Sample of Spanish Children
Previous Article in Journal
Association between CCC-2 and Structural Language, Pragmatics, Social Cognition, and Executive Functions in Children with Developmental Language Disorder
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Associations between Prenatal Education, Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors in Pre-Schoolers

1
Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou 510080, China
2
School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane 4059, QLD, Australia
3
Department of Information Management, Xinhua College of Sun Yat-Sen University, Guangzhou 510080, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2021, 8(2), 124; https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020124
Submission received: 9 January 2021 / Revised: 5 February 2021 / Accepted: 5 February 2021 / Published: 9 February 2021

Abstract

:
This study aimed to investigate the association between prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors of preschoolers as well as the mediating role of breastfeeding in their associations. A cross-sectional study via a structured questionnaire was conducted with 67,578 preschool children and parents from Longhua District of Shenzhen, China. Hierarchical logistic regression models were performed to explore the associations between maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, as well as the mediating effect of breastfeeding in the associations. After controlling for potential confounders, logistic regression analysis indicated that maternal participation in prenatal education could significantly increase the prevalence of breastfeeding, and the strength of the association was enhanced with the increase frequency of prenatal education visits (ORs ranging from 1.191 to 1.899). While both maternal participation in prenatal education (ORs ranging from 0.732 to 0.798) and breastfeeding (OR = 0.773) were significantly associated with the lower presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. The crossover analysis indicated that children with both maternal prenatal education and breastfeeding had the lower risk of presence of autistics-like behaviors (OR = 0.569). Furthermore, mediation analysis illustrated that breastfeeding mediated the association between maternal participation in prenatal education and the presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, with a mediating effect of approximately 14.3%. Our findings suggest that maternal participation in prenatal education is significantly associated with a decreased risk of autistic-like behaviors in preschool children through increased breastfeeding in the mothers who attended prenatal education.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by social impairment, restrictive interests and cognitive inflexibility, with its main symptoms including social communication barriers, language barriers and repetitive stereotyped behavior [1]. Usually, ASD is manifested before the age of three, but most children with ASD are not clinically diagnosed until the age of three or later [2]. Autistic-like behaviors, an important clinical manifestation for the diagnosis of ASD, include social interaction and communication disorders [3,4], language development disorders [5], repetitive behaviors and parochial interests [6,7]. Currently, one in every 59 children in the United States is diagnosed with ASD [8], and approximately 52 million people worldwide suffer from ASD [9]. Due to the lack of a national monitoring system, there has been no large-scale epidemiological survey to address the prevalence of ASD in China to date [10]. But several regional studies reported relatively low prevalence rates of ASD. For instance, a systematic review revealed that the prevalence of ASD was 2.64% in China [11]. Another recent meta-analysis showed a pooled prevalence of ASD in China of 39.23 per 10,000 [12]. However, Constantino’s and Charman’s findings also indicate that autistic traits are continuously distributed in the general population [13].
The prior three studies found an association between earlier autistic traits and later emotional or behavioral problems in different developmental stages, i.e., from preschoolers at age 14 to 15 months to 3 years old [14], from age 2 to age 4 [15] and from preschoolers at age 5 to school-age children at age 7 as well [16]. As such, without timely intervention, autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers are a significant risk factor for the development of serious cognitive and learning deficits and lifelong disability with poor prognosis [17]. Given the high prevalence of ASD and autistic like behaviors in children, and the trajectory of impaired development, there is a need to further identify modifiable risk factors for autistic like behaviors in young children. Identifying such modifiable risk factors will help guide public health interventions aimed at reducing the prevalence of ASD and autistic like behaviors in children.
Prenatal education is a vital component of maternity care provided by public health departments, hospitals, midwives and private and community agencies for mothers and their partners [18]. Traditionally, prenatal education has occurred as a one-on-one interaction between a clinician and individual woman or a formal childbirth education class, but a group model of prenatal care, Pregnant Women School, has been widely implemented since 1995 [19,20]. Prenatal education imparts knowledge about pregnancy, childbirth, puerperium and neonatal care to pregnant women [21]. In each class, the clinicians may give a lecture on a special topic for pregnant women, such as proper maternal and infant nutritional diets or maternal weight management during pregnancy, guide pregnant women in how to breastfeed and its benefits, and so on [22]. Prenatal education can not only improve maternal self-care, management and awareness about pregnancy, but also promotes short term maternal and child health [20,23]. Unfortunately, research on the long-term effects of prenatal education is lacking. This is especially the case for improvements in the long-term physical and mental health of children caused by mothers’ changing their behaviors due to prenatal education.
There is good evidence that prenatal education can increase the rate of breastfeeding by providing appropriate knowledge and skills [24,25,26]. In accordance with these findings, Schmidt et al. found that prenatal education could increase breastfeeding initiation among low-income women, especially non-Hispanic white and black women [27]. These findings are important as there is also emerging evidence that breastfeeding is a protective factor for childhood cognitive development. For example, Horta et al. reinforced the evidence that breastfeeding was positively associated with an intelligence quotient in childhood [28]. Moreover, three recent studies reported that exclusive breastfeeding was associated with lower risk of ASD [29,30,31], and a systematic review and meta-analysis indicated that breastfeeding could significantly protect against ASD [32].
Whether prenatal education can reduce the risk of autistic-like behaviors of children, whether this could be due to increased breastfeeding and whether prenatal education and breastfeeding have combination effect on autistic-like behaviors, remains unclear. Therefore, this study aimed to resolve these issues.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample and Recruitment

Participants of this study were recruited from the Longhua Child Cohort Study (LCCS), which aimed to estimate the effect of early-life family and school risk and protective factors upon children’s psycho-behavioral development [33,34,35,36]. In October 2017, a total of 67,861 child-caregiver dyads were approached, and 67,578 child-caregiver dyads were included in our analysis after excluding those with incomplete records. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the School of Public Health at Sun Yat-sen University (ethics clearance No.: 2015–2016), and all adult participants provided their informed consent.

2.2. Data Collection

A structured questionnaire was used to collect the parents’ socio-demographic characteristics (including age, education level, marital status and family economic income), the child’s demographic information (including date of birth, gender, single child or not, etc.), parental participation in prenatal education, the mother’s breastfeeding history and the child’s current autistic-like behaviors. The investigators trained the health care doctors and kindergarten teachers of the participating child-care institutions in how to give the questionnaire to parents and guided them in filling in relevant information and questionnaires. The questionnaire had to be filled with the child’s parent or child caregiver of more than 1 year, and the child’s mother was preferred. The investigators checked the completed questionnaire and asked the parents to complete any missing items. After the data was entered into a database then 10% of the responses were randomly selected and compared against the answers in the hardcopy questionnaires, and any input errors were corrected.

2.3. Measurement of Prenatal Education, Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors

Maternal participation in prenatal classes was measured with the following two questions. “Whether did you attend the Pregnant Women School?” Response options were “Yes” or “No”. If the participants answered “Yes”, they were further asked “How many times did you attend Pregnant Women School?” Response options were categorized into once (scored 1), twice (scored 2), three times (scored 3), four times and over (scored 4). In the final data analysis, parents attending the Pregnant Women School were categorized into five groups (never, once, twice, three times and ≥ four times). Mothers who reported having breastfed were asked to report whether this involved exclusive breastfeeding (i.e., feeding their child only with breastmilk and not formula or other foods/drink) or mixed feeding (i.e., feeding their child with both breastmilk, formula and other foods/drink). Autistic-like behaviors were measured by the Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC), a scale for ASD screening and diagnosis comprised of five subscales (sensory ability, communicative ability, motor ability, language ability and self-care ability) with a total of 57 items [37]. Each item is weighted with score from 1 to 4, and the total score was 158 points. We used established scoring criteria for China, with a total score of ≥ 31 points indicating the presence of autistic-like behaviors; and a total score ≥ 62 points indicating the presence of a diagnosis of ASD [38]. In our study, a cut-off of ≥ 31 was chosen to distinguish preschool children with autistic-like behaviors.

2.4. Data Analysis

Regarding data analysis, means and standard deviations (X ± SD) were used to describe continuous variables, and proportions were used to describe categorical variables. Chi-square tests, Student’s t-tests and ANOVA were employed to test the associations between the different socio-demographic characteristics, prenatal education attendance and the prevalence of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. Binary logistic regressions were performed to investigate the associations between the dependent variable of the presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers with the independent variables (attendance at prenatal education, and breastfeeding patterns) after adjusting for potential confounders of child’s age, gender, parents’ education level, family income and parental marital status.
Moreover, we made a crossover analysis to assess combination effects of maternal prenatal education and breastfeeding on the presence of autistics-like of behaviors in pre-schoolers, and the parents attending the Pregnant Women School were then categorized into two groups (never vs. once and over). The multiplicative interaction was estimated by the interaction of odds ratio (IOR) in logistic regression models. If the 95% CI of IOR did not contain 1, the significant multiplicative interaction was considered to exist [39]. In order to evaluate additive interaction, the relative excess risk due to interaction (RERI) and the attributable proportion due to interaction (AP) were calculated [40]. In the absence of additive interaction, 95% CIs of RERI and AP were equal to 0 [39,41,42].
Furthermore, in order to explore whether breastfeeding mediated the association between maternal prenatal education attendance and autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, a series of hierarchical logistic regressions were performed after adjusting for the potential confounders. According to Baron and Kenny [43], mediation is demonstrated when the following conditions are met: (1) the independent variable (X, maternal attendances to prenatal education) is significantly related with the dependent variable (Y, autistic-like behaviors of preschoolers; the partial coefficient is denoted by c); (2) the independent variable is significantly related to the mediator variable (M, breastfeeding; the partial coefficient is denoted by a ); and (3) the mediator variable (breastfeeding) is significantly associated with the dependent variable (autistic-like behaviors of preschoolers) when the independent variable (maternal attendances to prenatal education) is controlled for (the partial coefficient is denoted by b). The indirect effect of X on Y through M (namely ORIE) was estimated as exp(ab), and the direct effect of X on Y (namely ORDE) was estimated as exp(c’) (The c’ is the effect of X on Y controlling for M). Based on the research of Vanderweele [44], we used the following formula to calculate the proportion of mediation:
OR DE × OR IE 1 OR DE × OR IE 1
All of the p-values were two-sided. Type I errors were set at 0.05. The statistical analysis was conducted with SPSS (version 23.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Description of Participants

The descriptive statistics of the participants are presented in Table 1. Of the 67,578 successfully investigated children, 54.3% were male and the average age was 4.61 years (SD = 0.88) at the time of assessment, 90.2% were breastfed, 43.0% of the mothers never participated in prenatal education during pregnancy, 9.7% for once, 18.3% for twice, 9.8% for three times and 9.2% for four times and over. Of the children, 2074 (3.1%) were categorized with autistic-like behaviors.

3.2. Association between Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Demographic Characteristics

As shown in Table 2, the results of Chi-square tests and ANOVA revealed that those who attended maternal prenatal education differed to those who did not attend maternal prenatal education on a range of demographic characteristics. Pregnant women from families with high monthly incomes participated in prenatal education more often than those from families with low monthly incomes. Women who attended more often prenatal education tended to have female infants, older maternal/paternal age in years at child’s birth, higher maternal education level and higher paternal education level. However, there was no statistically significant relationship between maternal prenatal education attendance and marital status (p > 0.05).

3.3. Association between Autistic-Like Behaviors of Preschoolers and Demographic Characteristics

Table 3 shows that being a male child, low maternal education level, low paternal education level, low monthly family income and non-married status were significantly associated with a higher probability of the presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschool children (p < 0.001).

3.4. Association between Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Breastfeeding

After adjusting for the child’s age and gender, parental education level, monthly family income and parental marital status and age at child’s birth, the results of the logistic regression analysis showed that maternal participation in prenatal education was significantly associated with an increase in breastfeeding, see Table 4.

3.5. Association of Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Breastfeeding with Autistic-Like Behaviors of Preschoolers

Table 5 displays results of the binary logistic regression analyses examining the associations between the independent variables and prevalence of autistic-like behaviors. After controlling for the aforementioned covariates, the analysis indicated that maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding (OR = 0.773, 95% CI = 0.676–0.884) were significantly associated with the less autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. Compared with those who had never participation in prenatal education, the odds ratio of exhibiting autistic-like behaviors was 0.798 (95% CI = 0.707–0.901) for once, 0.738 (95% CI = 0.648–0.841) for twice, 0.732 (95% CI = 0.615–0.871) for three times and 0.738 (95% CI = 0.681–0.883) for four times and over.
Moreover, the crossover analysis indicated that children with both maternal prenatal education and breastfeeding had a lower risk of presence of autistic-like behaviors (OR = 0.569, 95% CI = 0.478–0.677), and there was no multiplicative and additive interaction of two variables on autistic-like behaviors. See Table 6.

3.6. Mediating Effect of Breastfeeding on Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Autistic-Like Behaviors of Preschoolers

After controlling for the aforementioned confounders, the results from the binary logistic regression analysis (Table 7 and Figure 1) showed that maternal participation in prenatal education was significantly associated with decreased the probability of the presence of autistic-like behaviors (OR = 0.909, 95% CI = 0.876–0.942; Model 1 in Table 7). After breastfeeding was added into Model 1, the association between maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors became weaker, but was still significant (OR = 0.906, 95% CI = 0.873–0.939; Model 2 in Table 7). The mediation analysis indicated that breastfeeding mediated the association between maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, with its effect being approximately 14.3%.

4. Discussion

To investigate the effect of maternal prenatal education attendance on autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers and its potential pathway, we conducted a cross-sectional study with 67,578 preschool children and their mothers from Longhua New District of Shenzhen, China. After adjusting for the child’s gender, parental educational level, family income and parental marital status, we found that attending prenatal education during pregnancy and breastfeeding was significantly associated with a reduced probability of preschoolers’ autistic-like behaviors. Moreover, crossover analysis indicated that compared with children without both maternal prenatal education and breastfeeding, those with both maternal prenatal education and breastfeeding had a lower risk of the presence of autistic-like behavior, followed by ones with maternal prenatal education only and one with breastfeeding only. And, we found no significantly multiplicative and additive interaction of prenatal education and breastfeeding on autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. Furthermore, mediation analysis indicated that breastfeeding mediated in part the association between the maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, with a mediating effect being around 14.3%.

4.1. Association between Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Breastfeeding

It has been well documented that prenatal education can substantially provide health promotion content for pregnant women and lead to them gaining positive attitudes and healthier behaviors during pregnancy [24,25,26,45,46]. For example, two prior studies showed that prenatal education could effectively avoid maternal exposure to detrimental environmental factors and inappropriate behaviors [20,23]. Prenatal education has positive effect on maternal folic acid and vitamin D intake [47], parental-infant interaction and maternal psychological stress [48], etc. Further, several investigations reported that attendance at group prenatal classes could increase the rate of exclusive breastfeeding [49,50,51]. Similarly, studies by Sehhatie et al. [52] and McFadden et al. [53] revealed that prenatal breastfeeding counseling increased the rates of exclusive breastfeeding. Yan presents evidence that the lower frequency of prenatal education visits, the higher risk of no breastfeeding [51]. In line with previous studies, our findings also support a positive association between maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding. Collectively these findings indicate that maternal participation in prenatal education may increase the rate of breastfeeding.

4.2. Association between Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors

Regarding effect of breastfeeding on ASD, a multicenter study in Spain found that breastfeeding played a protective role against autistic traits [54]. A case-control study involving 861 children with ASD and 123 controls showed that children who were not breastfed for more than six months were 2.48 times more likely to have autistic spectrum disorder compared to those who were breastfed for more than six months [55]. Moreover, another case-control study found that the optimal breast-feeding practices, including the early initiation of breastfeeding, increased periods of exclusive breastfeeding and continued breastfeeding as well, could decrease the risk of ASD [56]. In line with these previous findings, we also found a significant inverse relationship between breastfeeding and the presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschool children. Taken together, these findings support the premise that breastfeeding can reduce the risk of ASD or autistic-like behaviors in children.

4.3. Association between Maternal Participation in Prenatal Education and Autistic-Like Behaviors of Preschoolers and Potential Pathways

There are few studies examining the long-term effects of maternal attendance in prenatal education upon children’s health. Our study adds to the literature by showing that maternal attendance in prenatal education was significantly and negatively associated with the presence of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. Furthermore, mediation analysis indicated that breastfeeding mediated this association, with an effect of around 14.3%. How do we explain breastfeeding mediating the association between maternal attending prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers? Breastfeeding might act as a protective factor through two mechanisms [57]. First, from a nutritional standpoint, human breast milk contains a range of nutrients, such as carbohydrates, protein, fat, vitamins, minerals, hormones and immune cells, which can support the emotional and cognitive development of infants and protects them from contagious and chronic diseases [58]. These nutrients including iron [59], insulin-like growth factor (IGF) [60,61], long-chain polyunsaturated fatty such as acids docosahexaenoic (DHA) and arachidonic (ARA) acids improve infants’ cognitive development and may protect against autistic traits [62]. Compared with bovine milk or infant formula, human milk could elevate more serum IGF in neonatal [63,64]. And the IGF could improve myelination, which can promote more effective neural impulse passage [60]. The physiological effects of IGF-1 are included stimulation of neuro cell proliferation, promotion of tissue growth and development, effects on lipid and carbohydrate metabolism, and anti-inflammatory and anti-oxidant effects as well [65]. In addition, the dietary DHA enrichment of human milk could have an effect on improving neurodevelopment only when mothers start supplementation during pregnancy [66]. Second, prenatal education could increase maternal-fetal attachment and self-efficacy related to childbirth [67,68]. As a uniquely close and sensual experience, breastfeeding creates a particular connection between mother and neonatal, which promotes mother–infant attachment [69,70]. It is known that maternal stress during pregnancy may affect motor and mental development in infants, and the effect may last to later child development [71]. Also, postpartum maternal stress is negatively associated with child poor motor, language and cognitive development [72]. The production of prolactin and oxytocin during breastfeeding not only can increase maternal-fetal bonding and skin-to-skin contact, but also confers maternal relaxation benefits [73]. Liu et al. have found that breastfeeding and active maternal-fetal bonding are in relation to lower risks of internalizing behavior problems in age six mainly by promoting neurodevelopment in early childhood [74]. Nevertheless, the exact mechanism for the associations of autistic-like behaviors, breastfeeding and prenatal education needs to be further explored.

4.4. Strength and Limitation

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to show that maternal participation in prenatal education protects against the development of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers. However, certain limitations of this study need to be noted. First, although the study sample size is very large, the study participants were all from Longhua New District of Shenzhen, China. This is a city with high levels of population mobility, which may limit the generalizability to other Chinese cities. Second, it is possible that the measures of attendance at prenatal education and engagement in breastfeeding may be influenced by recall bias. However, a couple of studies showed that mothers’ reports were important to improve the accuracy of retrospective data and may appear more reliable than medical records sometimes [75,76]. Third, while our study supports the protective effects of prenatal education against autistic-like behaviors, we did not use cut-offs to indicate the presence of ASD and so the result should not be interpreted as an indication that prenatal education prevents the development of ASD. Further examination of such processes in both clinical and sub-clinical ASD has the potential to further our understanding of the broader autism phenotype. Fourth, breastfeeding only partially mediated the relations between maternal participation in prenatal education and children’s autistic-like behaviors, and the effect is relatively small. This indicates that other possible pathways may exist and need to be further explored. Fifth, we did not ask the participants to respond to breastfeeding via pumped milk whether or not, which had different impacts than feeding baby at the breast [77]. This might disturb the true association between breastfeeding and autistic-like behaviors in our results. In addition, the exact mechanism for the associations of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, breastfeeding and prenatal education needs to be further explored. Finally, significant paths identified in mediation analysis do not establish causal effects in this cross-sectional study. Thus, further investigation in a longitudinal study is needed to confirm our findings.

5. Conclusions

In summary, our findings suggest that maternal participation in prenatal education is associated with a decreased risk of autistic-like behaviors in preschoolers, in part due to increasing breastfeeding. The findings reinforce the importance of promoting prenatal education, not only for the immediate beneficial effects on pregnant women and their infants, but also long-term benefit effects on their offspring’s health.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.C., E.S., L.-H.H. and W.-Q.C.; Data curation, L.-H.H. and W.-Q.C.; Formal analysis, J.C. and W.-Q.C.; Investigation, J.C., L.-H.H., J.-Y.C., X.-C.L., J.-H.Y. and W.-Q.C.; Methodology, J.C., E.S., L.-H.H., J.-Y.C., X.-C.L., J.-H.Y. and W.-Q.C.; Resources, W.-Q.C.; Supervision, J.C., E.S., L.-H.H. and W.-Q.C.; Validation, J.C., E.S., L.-H.H. and W.-Q.C.; Writing—original draft, J.C.; Writing—review & editing, E.S. and W.-Q.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Government of Longhua New District of Shenzhen, China, under Grant number 2013142.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of School of Public Health of Sun Yat-sen University (ethics clearance No.: 2015–2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from parents of children involved in the study, and informed assent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy concerns.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Shenzhen Longhua District Government of China for their funding and other supports. The authors are also grateful to all families who participated in this research and the doctors from Longhua Maternal and Child Healthcare Center, especially Chuan-An Wu, Xiao-Na Yin, Guo-Min Wen, Deng-Li Sun, and Dan-Xia Xian, who offered much help during the field investigation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Doernberg, E.; Hollander, E. Neurodevelopmental Disorders (ASD and ADHD): DSM-5, ICD-10, and ICD-11. CNS Spectr. 2016, 21, 295–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Huang, J.P.; Cui, S.S.; Han, Y.; Irva, H.P.; Qi, L.H.; Zhang, X. Prevalence and early signs of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) among 18–36 month-old children of Tianjin in China. Biomed. Environ. Sci. 2014, 27, 453–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Becerra-Culqui, T.A.; Lynch, F.L.; Owen-Smith, A.A.; Spitzer, J.; Croen, L.A. Parental First Concerns and Timing of Autism Spectrum Disorder Diagnosis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018, 48, 3367–3376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Neufeld, J.; Ioannou, C.; Korb, S.; Schilbach, L.; Chakrabarti, B. Spontaneous Facial Mimicry is Modulated by Joint Attention and Autistic Traits. Autism Res. Off. J. Int. Soc. Autism Res. 2016, 9, 781–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Barbaro, J.; Dissanayake, C. Developmental profiles of infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders identified prospectively in a community-based setting. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 1939–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Holloway, J.M.; Long, T.M.; Biasini, F. Relationships Between Gross Motor Skills and Social Function in Young Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Pediatric Phys. Ther. Off. Publ. Sect. Pediatrics Am. Phys. Ther. Assoc. 2018, 30, 184–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Fulceri, F.; Grossi, E.; Contaldo, A.; Narzisi, A.; Apicella, F.; Parrini, I.; Tancredi, R.; Calderoni, S.; Muratori, F. Motor Skills as Moderators of Core Symptoms in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Preliminary Data from an Exploratory Analysis with Artificial Neural Networks. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Christensen, D.L.; Braun, K.V.N.; Baio, J.; Bilder, D.; Charles, J.; Constantino, J.N.; Daniels, J.; Durkin, M.S.; Fitzgerald, R.T.; Kurzius-Spencer, M.; et al. Prevalence and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 2018, 65, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Baxter, A.J.; Brugha, T.S.; Erskine, H.E.; Scheurer, R.W.; Vos, T.; Scott, J.G. The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 601–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Li, N.; Chen, G.; Song, X.; Du, W.; Zheng, X. Prevalence of autism-caused disability among Chinese children: A national population-based survey. Epilepsy Behav. 2011, 22, 786–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Wan, Y.; Hu, Q.; Li, T.; Jiang, L.; Du, Y.; Feng, L.; Wong, J.C.; Li, C. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders among children in China: A systematic review. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry 2013, 25, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wang, F.; Lu, L.; Wang, S.B.; Zhang, L.; Ng, C.H.; Ungvari, G.S.; Cao, X.L.; Lu, J.P.; Hou, C.L.; Jia, F.J.; et al. The prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in China: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Int. J. Biol. Sci. 2018, 14, 717–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Constantino, J.N.; Charman, T. Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Reconciling the syndrome, its diverse origins, and variation in expression. Lancet Neurol. 2016, 15, 279–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Möricke, E.; Swinkels, S.H.; Beuker, K.T.; Buitelaar, J.K. Predictive value of subclinical autistic traits at age 14-15 months for behavioural and cognitive problems at age 3–5 years. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 19, 659–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Dereu, M.; Roeyers, H.; Raymaekers, R.; Meirsschaut, M.; Warreyn, P. How useful are screening instruments for toddlers to predict outcome at age 4? General development, language skills, and symptom severity in children with a false positive screen for autism spectrum disorder. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 21, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Saito, A.; Stickley, A.; Haraguchi, H.; Takahashi, H.; Ishitobi, M.; Kamio, Y. Association Between Autistic Traits in Preschool Children and Later Emotional/Behavioral Outcomes. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 3333–3346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Mughal, S.; Faizy, R.M.; Saadabadi, A. Autism Spectrum Disorder. In StatPearls; StatPearls Publishing LLC.: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  18. Gagnon, A.J.; Sandall, J. Individual or group antenatal education for childbirth or parenthood, or both. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2007, 2007, Cd002869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Likis, F.E. Prenatal education: Enduring and essential. J. Midwifery Women’s Health 2009, 54, 429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Massey, Z.; Rising, S.S.; Ickovics, J. CenteringPregnancy group prenatal care: Promoting relationship-centered care. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2006, 35, 286–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Heberlein, E.C.; Picklesimer, A.H.; Billings, D.L.; Covington-Kolb, S.; Farber, N.; Frongillo, E.A. Qualitative Comparison of Women’s Perspectives on the Functions and Benefits of Group and Individual Prenatal Care. J. Midwifery Women’s Health 2016, 61, 224–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Zhi, M.; Mao, Y.; Huang, J. Research progress of Pregnant Women School (In China). Gen. Nurs. 2014, 12, 976–978. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ustunsoz, A.; Senel, N.; Pollock, C.A. Comparison of prenatal education delivered by nurses in Ankara (Turkey) and New Orleans (USA). J. Clin. Nurs. 2011, 20, 1133–1140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Schreck, P.K.; Solem, K.; Wright, T.; Schulte, C.; Ronnisch, K.J.; Szpunar, S. Both Prenatal and Postnatal Interventions Are Needed to Improve Breastfeeding Outcomes in a Low-Income Population. Breastfeed. Med. Off. J. Acad. Breastfeed. Med. 2017, 12, 142–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lin, S.S.; Chien, L.Y.; Tai, C.J.; Lee, C.F. Effectiveness of a prenatal education programme on breastfeeding outcomes in Taiwan. J. Clin. Nurs. 2008, 17, 296–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Huang, P.; Yao, J.; Liu, X.; Luo, B. Individualized intervention to improve rates of exclusive breastfeeding: A randomised controlled trial. Medicine 2019, 98, e17822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Ahlers-Schmidt, C.R.; Okut, H.; Dowling, J. Impact of Prenatal Education on Breastfeeding Initiation Among Low-Income Women. Am. J. Health Promot. 2020, 34, 919–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Horta, B.L.; de Sousa, B.A.; de Mola, C.L. Breastfeeding and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 2018, 21, 174–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Soke, G.N.; Maenner, M.; Windham, G.; Moody, E.; Kaczaniuk, J.; DiGuiseppi, C.; Schieve, L.A. Association Between Breastfeeding Initiation and Duration and Autism Spectrum Disorder in Preschool Children Enrolled in the Study to Explore Early Development. Autism Res. Off. J. Int. Soc. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 816–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Ravi, S.; Chandrasekaran, V.; Kattimani, S.; Subramanian, M. Maternal and birth risk factors for children screening positive for autism spectrum disorders on M-CHAT-R. Asian J. Psychiatry 2016, 22, 17–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Manohar, H.; Pravallika, M.; Kandasamy, P.; Chandrasekaran, V.; Rajkumar, R.P. Role of Exclusive Breastfeeding in Conferring Protection in Children At-Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder: Results from a Sibling Case-control Study. J. Neurosci. Rural Pract. 2018, 9, 132–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Tseng, P.T.; Chen, Y.W.; Stubbs, B.; Carvalho, A.F.; Whiteley, P.; Tang, C.H.; Yang, W.C.; Chen, T.Y.; Li, D.J.; Chu, C.S.; et al. Maternal breastfeeding and autism spectrum disorder in children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr. Neurosci. 2019, 22, 354–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Yang, G.Y.; Huang, L.H.; Schmid, K.L.; Li, C.G.; Chen, J.Y.; He, G.H.; Liu, L.; Ruan, Z.L.; Chen, W.Q. Associations Between Screen Exposure in Early Life and Myopia amongst Chinese Preschoolers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. He, G.H.; Strodl, E.; Liu, L.; Ruan, Z.L.; Yin, X.N.; Wen, G.M.; Sun, D.L.; Xian, D.X.; Jiang, H.; Jing, J.; et al. Teacher’s Type D Personality and Chinese Children’s Hyperactive Behaviors: Moderation Effect of Parental Type D Personality and Mediation Effect of Teacher-Student Relationship. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. He, G.H.; Liu, L.; Strodl, E.; Ruan, Z.L.; Jiang, H.; Jing, J.; Jin, Y.; Chen, W.Q. Parental Type D Personality and Children’s Hyperactive Behaviors: The Mediating Role of Parent-Child Interactive Activities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Liu, L.; Fan, L.; Hou, X.Y.; Wu, C.A.; Yin, X.N.; Wen, G.M.; Sun, D.; Xian, D.X.; Jiang, H.; Jing, J.; et al. Family Childcare Types and Conduct Problem Behaviors in Young Children: The Mediation Role of Caregiver-Child Interaction. Front. Pediatrics 2018, 6, 217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Krug, D.A.; Arick, J.; Almond, P. Behavior checklist for identifying severely handicapped individuals with high levels of autistic behavior. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1980, 21, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Yang, X.L.; Huang, Y.Q.; Jia, M.X. Test report of autism behavior checklist in China. Chin. Ment. Health J. 1993, 7, 275–280. [Google Scholar]
  39. Rothman, K.J.; Greenland, S. Modern Epidemiology; A Wolters Kluwer Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1998; pp. 329–342. [Google Scholar]
  40. De Mutsert, R.; Jager, K.J.; Zoccali, C.; Dekker, F.W. The effect of joint exposures: Examining the presence of interaction. Kidney Int. 2009, 75, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Hosmer, D.W.; Lemeshow, S. Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology 1992, 3, 452–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Andersson, T.; Alfredsson, L.; Källberg, H.; Zdravkovic, S.; Ahlbom, A. Calculating measures of biological interaction. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 20, 575–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Vanderweele, T.J.; Vansteelandt, S. Odds ratios for mediation analysis for a dichotomous outcome. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 172, 1339–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Eluwa, G.I.; Adebajo, S.B.; Torpey, K.; Shittu, O.; Abdu-Aguye, S.; Pearlman, D.; Bawa, U.; Olorukooba, A.; Khamofu, H.; Chiegli, R. The effects of centering pregnancy on maternal and fetal outcomes in northern Nigeria; a prospective cohort analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2018, 18, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Vonderheid, S.C.; Norr, K.F.; Handler, A.S. Prenatal health promotion content and health behaviors. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2007, 29, 258–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Aktaç, S.; Sabuncular, G.; Kargin, D.; Gunes, F.E. Evaluation of Nutrition Knowledge of Pregnant Women before and after Nutrition Education according to Sociodemographic Characteristics. Ecol. Food Nutr. 2018, 57, 441–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Ip, W.Y.; Tang, C.S.; Goggins, W.B. An educational intervention to improve women’s ability to cope with childbirth. J. Clin. Nurs. 2009, 18, 2125–2135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Brumley, J.; Cain, M.A.; Stern, M.; Louis, J.M. Gestational Weight Gain and Breastfeeding Outcomes in Group Prenatal Care. J. Midwifery Women’s Health 2016, 61, 557–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Schellinger, M.M.; Abernathy, M.P.; Amerman, B.; May, C.; Foxlow, L.A.; Carter, A.L.; Barbour, K.; Luebbehusen, E.; Ayo, K.; Bastawros, D.; et al. Improved Outcomes for Hispanic Women with Gestational Diabetes Using the Centering Pregnancy(©) Group Prenatal Care Model. Matern. Child Health J. 2017, 21, 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Yan, J. The Effects of Prenatal Care Utilization on Maternal Health and Health Behaviors. Health Econ. 2017, 26, 1001–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Sehhatie, F.S.; Mirghafourvand, M.; Havizari, S. Effect of prenatal counseling on exclusive breastfeeding frequency and infant weight gain in mothers with previous unsuccessful breastfeeding: A randomized controlled clinical trial. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2020, 33, 3571–3578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. McFadden, A.; Siebelt, L.; Marshall, J.L.; Gavine, A.; Girard, L.C.; Symon, A.; MacGillivray, S. Counselling interventions to enable women to initiate and continue breastfeeding: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. Breastfeed. J. 2019, 14, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Boucher, O.; Julvez, J.; Guxens, M.; Arranz, E.; Ibarluzea, J.; Sánchez de Miguel, M.; Fernández-Somoano, A.; Tardon, A.; Rebagliato, M.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; et al. Association between breastfeeding duration and cognitive development, autistic traits and ADHD symptoms: A multicenter study in Spain. Pediatric Res. 2017, 81, 434–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Schultz, S.T.; Klonoff-Cohen, H.S.; Wingard, D.L.; Akshoomoff, N.A.; Macera, C.A.; Ji, M.; Bacher, C. Breastfeeding, infant formula supplementation, and Autistic Disorder: The results of a parent survey. Int. Breastfeed. J. 2006, 1, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Al-Farsi, Y.M.; Al-Sharbati, M.M.; Waly, M.I.; Al-Farsi, O.A.; Al-Shafaee, M.A.; Al-Khaduri, M.M.; Trivedi, M.S.; Deth, R.C. Effect of suboptimal breast-feeding on occurrence of autism: A case-control study. Nutrition 2012, 28, e27–e32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Brown Belfort, M. The Science of Breastfeeding and Brain Development. Breastfeed. Med. Off. J. Acad. Breastfeed. Med. 2017, 12, 459–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Martin, C.R.; Ling, P.R.; Blackburn, G.L. Review of Infant Feeding: Key Features of Breast Milk and Infant Formula. Nutrients 2016, 8, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Congdon, E.L.; Westerlund, A.; Algarin, C.R.; Peirano, P.D.; Gregas, M.; Lozoff, B.; Nelson, C.A. Iron deficiency in infancy is associated with altered neural correlates of recognition memory at 10 years. J. Pediatrics 2012, 160, 1027–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Steinman, G.; Mankuta, D. Breastfeeding as a possible deterrent to autism—A clinical perspective. Med. Hypotheses 2013, 81, 999–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Shafai, T.; Mustafa, M.; Hild, T.; Mulari, J.; Curtis, A. The association of early weaning and formula feeding with autism spectrum disorders. Breastfeed. Med. Off. J. Acad. Breastfeed. Med. 2014, 9, 275–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Demmelmair, H.; Koletzko, B. Lipids in human milk. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2018, 32, 57–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Díaz-Gómez, N.M.; Domenech, E.; Barroso, F. Breast-feeding and growth factors in preterm newborn infants. J. Pediatric Gastroenterol. Nutr. 1997, 24, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Steinman, G. Can the chance of having twins be modified by diet? Lancet 2006, 367, 1461–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Halmos, T.; Suba, I. [The physiological role of growth hormone and insulin-like growth factors]. Orv. Hetil. 2019, 160, 1774–1783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Agostoni, C. Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA): From the maternal-foetal dyad to the complementary feeding period. Early Hum. Dev. 2010, 86 (Suppl. S1), 3–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Serçekuş, P.; Başkale, H. Effects of antenatal education on fear of childbirth, maternal self-efficacy and parental attachment. Midwifery 2016, 34, 166–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Chang, S.; Park, S.; Chung, C. Effect of Taegyo-focused prenatal education on maternal-fetal attachment and self-efficacy related to childbirth. Taehan Kanho Hakhoe Chi 2004, 34, 1409–1415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Fergusson, D.M.; Woodward, L.J. Breast feeding and later psychosocial adjustment. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 1999, 13, 144–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Leckman, J.F.; Feldman, R.; Swain, J.E.; Eicher, V.; Thompson, N.; Mayes, L.C. Primary parental preoccupation: Circuits, genes, and the crucial role of the environment. J. Neural Transm. 2004, 111, 753–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Huizink, A.C.; Robles de Medina, P.G.; Mulder, E.J.; Visser, G.H.; Buitelaar, J.K. Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2003, 44, 810–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Oyetunji, A.; Chandra, P. Postpartum stress and infant outcome: A review of current literature. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 284, 112769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Uauy, R.; De Andraca, I. Human milk and breast feeding for optimal mental development. J. Nutr. 1995, 125, 2278s–2280s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Liu, J.; Leung, P.; Yang, A. Breastfeeding and active bonding protects against children’s internalizing behavior problems. Nutrients 2013, 6, 76–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Troude, P.; L’Hélias, L.F.; Raison-Boulley, A.M.; Castel, C.; Pichon, C.; Bouyer, J.; de La Rochebrochard, E. Perinatal factors reported by mothers: Do they agree with medical records? Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 23, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Buka, S.L.; Goldstein, J.M.; Spartos, E.; Tsuang, M.T. The retrospective measurement of prenatal and perinatal events: Accuracy of maternal recall. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 71, 417–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Felice, J.P.; Geraghty, S.R.; Quaglieri, C.W.; Yamada, R.; Wong, A.J.; Rasmussen, K.M. “Breastfeeding” but not at the breast: Mothers’ descriptions of providing pumped human milk to their infants via other containers and caregivers. Matern. Child Nutr. 2017, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Steps of mediation analysis.
Figure 1. Steps of mediation analysis.
Children 08 00124 g001
Table 1. Sample characteristics (n = 67,578).
Table 1. Sample characteristics (n = 67,578).
CharacteristicsTotal
Child age in years, mean (SD)4.61 (0.88)
Gender, n (%)
Male36,669 (54.3)
Female30,909 (45.7)
Maternal age in years at child’s birth, mean (SD)26.99 (4.18)
Maternal education level, n (%)
High school or lower29,371 (43.5)
Technical secondary school or college24,479 (36.2)
University12,425 (18.4)
Postgraduate or higher1303 (1.9)
Paternal age at child’s birth, mean (SD)29.56 (4.76)
Paternal education level, n (%)
High school or lower26,486 (39.2)
Technical secondary school or college21,810 (32.3)
University16,951 (25.1)
Postgraduate or higher2331 (3.4)
Monthly family income, n (%)
< 5000 RMB10,272 (15.2)
5000–15,000 RMB31,587 (46.7)
15,000–25,000 RMB14,459 (21.4)
≥ 25,000 RMB11,260 (16.7)
Parental marital status, n (%)
Married65,278 (96.6)
Other a2300 (3.4)
Maternal participation in prenatal education, n (%)
Never29,067 (43.0)
Once13,331 (19.7)
Twice12,361 (18.3)
Three times6591 (9.8)
≥Four times6228 (9.2)
Breastfeeding, n (%)
No6632 (9.8)
Yes60,946 (90.2)
Autistic-like behaviors of preschool children, n (%)
No65,504 (96.9)
Yes2074 (3.1)
a Including remarriage, divorce, widowed and unmarried.
Table 2. Maternal attendances to prenatal education in different socio-demographic characteristics #.
Table 2. Maternal attendances to prenatal education in different socio-demographic characteristics #.
CharacteristicsTotal
(n = 67,578)
Participation in Prenatal EducationF/χ2p Value
Never
(n = 29,067)
Once
(n = 13,331)
Twice
(n = 12,361)
Three Times
(n = 6591)
≥Four Times
(n = 6228)
Child age in years, mean (SD)67,5784.60 (0.88)4.61 (0.88)4.62 (0.87)4.59 (0.87)4.61 (0.88)1.3060.265
Gender, n (% ) 20.63<0.001
Male36,66915,958 (43.5)7207 (19.7)6732 (18.4)3420 (9.3)3352 (9.1)
Female30,90913,109 (42.4)6124 (19.8)5629 (18.2)3171 (10.3)2876 (9.3)
Maternal age in years at child’s birth, mean (SD)67,57826.43 (4.20)27.09 (4.02)27.41 (4.11)27.71 (4.14)27.76 (4.15)185.3<0.001
Maternal education level, n (% ) 1448.3< 0.001
High school or lower29,37114,613 (49.8)5512 (18.8)4822 (16.4)2352 (8.0)2072 (7.1)
Technical secondary school or college24,4799957 (40.7)5003 (20.4)4691 (19.2)2497 (10.2)2331 (9.5)
University12,4254110 (33.1)2576 (20.7)2554 (20.6)1548 (12.5)1548 (12.5)
Postgraduate or higher1303387 (29.7)240 (18.4)294 (22.6)194 (14.9)194 (14.9)
Paternal age in years at child’s birth, mean (SD)67,57829.03 (4.80)29.67 (4.58)29.96 (4.72)30.18 (4.70)30.34 (4.76)270.1< 0.001
Paternal education level, n (%) 1537.1< 0.001
High school or lower26,48613,455 (50.8)4889 (18.5)4226 (16.0)2096 (7.9)1820 (6.9)
Technical secondary school or college21,8109053 (41.5)4432 (20.3)4118 (18.9)2199 (10.1)2008 (9.2)
University16,9515851 (34.5)3571 (21.1)3491 (20.6)1978 (11.7)2060 (12.2)
Postgraduate or higher2331708 (30.4)439 (18.8)526 (22.6)318 (13.6)340 (14.6)
Monthly family income, n (%) 1200.7< 0.001
<5000 RMB10,2725415 (52.7)1743 (17.0)1558 (15.2)789 (7.7)767 (7.5)
5000–15,000 RMB31,58714,443 (45.7)6230 (19.7)5559 (17.6)2809 (8.9)2546 (8.1)
15,000–25,000 RMB14,4595341 (36.9)3066 (21.2)2921 (20.2)1662 (11.5)1469 (10.2)
≥25,000 RMB11,2603868 (34.4)2292 (20.4)2323 (20.6)1331 (11.8)1446 (12.8)
Parental marital status, n (%) 8.7820.067
Married65,27828,075 (43.0)12,897 (19.8)11,950 (18.3)6378 (9.8)5978 (9.2)
Other a2300992 (43.1)434 (18.9)411 (17.9)213 (9.3)250 (10.9)
#χ2 tests were used for categorical variables, ANOVA were used for continuous variables. a Including remarriage, divorce, widowed and unmarried.
Table 3. Autistic-like behaviors of preschool children in different socio-demographic characteristics #.
Table 3. Autistic-like behaviors of preschool children in different socio-demographic characteristics #.
CharacteristicsTotal
(n = 67,578)
Autistic-Like Behaviorst/χ2p Value
No
(n = 65,504)
Yes
(n = 2074)
Child age in years, mean (SD)67,5784.62 (0.87)4.34 (0.89)14.11< 0.001
Gender, n (% ) 64.658< 0.001
Male36,66935,364 (96.4)1305 (3.6)
Female30,90930,140 (97.5)769 (2.5)
Maternal age in years at child’s birth, mean (SD)67,57827.02(4.16)25.09(4.43)12.08< 0.001
Maternal education level, n (%) 179.307< 0.001
High school or lower29,37128,195 (96.0)1176 (4.0)
Technical secondary school or college24,47923,823 (97.3)656 (2.7)
University12,42512,198 (98.2)227 (1.8)
Postgraduate or higher13031288 (98.8)15 (1.2)
Paternal age at child’s birth, mean (SD)67,57829.59 (4.74)28.58 (5.07)9.54< 0.001
Paternal education level, n (%) 188.564< 0.001
High school or lower26,48625,390 (95.9)1096 (4.1)
Technical secondary school or college21,81021,212 (97.3)598 (2.7)
University16,95116,610 (98.0)341 (2.0)
Postgraduate or higher23312292 (98.3)39 (1.7)
Monthly family income, n (%) 189.948< 0.001
<5000 RMB10,2729806 (95.5)466 (4.5)
5000–15,000 RMB31,59730,508 (96.6)1089 (3.4)
15,000–25,000 RMB14,45914,137 (97.8)322 (2.2)
≥25,000 RMB11,26011,063 (98.3)197 (1.7)
Parental marital status, n (%) 17.917< 0.001
Married65,27863,309 (97.0)1969 (3.0)
Other a23002195 (95.4)105 (4.6)
#χ2 tests were used for categorical variables, t tests were used for continuous variables. a Including remarriage, divorce, widowed and unmarried.
Table 4. Association between maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding #.
Table 4. Association between maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding #.
Maternal Participation in Prenatal EducationBreastfeedingOR (95% CI)p Value
No
(n = 6632)
Yes
(n = 60,946)
Never (n = 29,067)3337 (11.5)25,730 (88.5)1.00
Once (n = 13,331)1325 (9.9)12,006 (90.1)1.191 (1.113–1.274)< 0.001
Twice (n = 12,361)1042 (8.4)11,319 (91.6)1.435 (1.333–1.545)< 0.001
Three times (n = 65,911)521 (7.9)6070 (92.1)1.539 (1.396–1.697)< 0.001
≥Four times (n = 6228)407 (6.5)5821 (93.5)1.899 (1.705–2.115)< 0.001
# With adjustment for child’s age, child’s gender, parental education level, parents’ age at child’s birth, monthly family income and parental marital status.
Table 5. Association of maternal participation in pregnancy school variables and breastfeeding with autistic-like behaviors of preschool children #.
Table 5. Association of maternal participation in pregnancy school variables and breastfeeding with autistic-like behaviors of preschool children #.
VariablesTotal
(n = 67,578)
Autistic-Like BehaviorsOR (95% CI)p Value
No
(n = 65,504)
Yes
(n = 2074)
Maternal participation in pregnancy school
Never29,067 (43.0)27,954 (96.2)1113 (3.8)1.00
Once13,331 (19.7)12,966 (97.3)365 (2.7)0.798 (0.707–0.901)<0.001
Twice12,361 (18.3)12,060 (97.6)301 (2.4)0.738 (0.648–0.841)<0.001
Three times6591 (9.8)6439 (97.7)152 (2.3)0.732 (0.615–0.871)<0.001
≥Four times6228 (9.2)6085 (97.7)143 (2.3)0.738 (0.681–0.883)0.001
Breastfeeding
No6632 (9.8)6375 (96.1)257 (3.9)1.00
Yes60,946 (90.2)59,129 (97.0)1817 (3.0)0.773 (0.676–0.884)<0.001
# With adjustment for child’s age, child’s gender, parental education level, parents’ age at child’s birth, monthly family income and parental marital status.
Table 6. The combination effect of maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding on autistic-like behaviors in pre-schoolers #.
Table 6. The combination effect of maternal participation in prenatal education and breastfeeding on autistic-like behaviors in pre-schoolers #.
VariablesAutistic-Like Behaviors OR (95% CI)IOR (95% CI)RERI (95% CI)AP (95% CI)
Prenatal educationBreastfeeding 1.175 (0.891, 1.594)0.176
(−0.027, 0.378)
0.309
(−0.072, 0.690)
NoNo1.000
YesNo0.664 (0.512, 0.861) *
NoYes0.729 (0.614, 0.866) **
YesYes0.569 (0.478, 0.677) **
# With adjustment for child’s age, child’s gender, parental education level, parents’ age at child’s birth, monthly family income and parental marital status. * p = 0.002; ** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Mediating effect of breastfeeding on association between maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors of preschoolers #.
Table 7. Mediating effect of breastfeeding on association between maternal participation in prenatal education and autistic-like behaviors of preschoolers #.
CharacteristicsAutistic-Like Behaviors
(Dependent Variable, Y, OR, 95% CI)
Model 1
X → Y
Model 2
X + M → Y
Proportion of
Mediation
Maternal participation in pre-natal education (Independent Variable, X)0.909 (0.876, 0.942) *0.906 (0.873, 0.939) *14.3%
Breastfeeding (Mediator, M) 0.780 (0.681, 0.892) *
# With adjustment for child’s age, child’s gender, parental education level, parents’ age at child’s birth, monthly family income and parental marital status. * p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, J.; Strodl, E.; Huang, L.-H.; Chen, J.-Y.; Liu, X.-C.; Yang, J.-H.; Chen, W.-Q. Associations between Prenatal Education, Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors in Pre-Schoolers. Children 2021, 8, 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020124

AMA Style

Chen J, Strodl E, Huang L-H, Chen J-Y, Liu X-C, Yang J-H, Chen W-Q. Associations between Prenatal Education, Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors in Pre-Schoolers. Children. 2021; 8(2):124. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020124

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Jing, Esben Strodl, Li-Hua Huang, Jing-Yi Chen, Xin-Chen Liu, Jian-Hui Yang, and Wei-Qing Chen. 2021. "Associations between Prenatal Education, Breastfeeding and Autistic-Like Behaviors in Pre-Schoolers" Children 8, no. 2: 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020124

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop