3. Results
Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics of the overall sample and according to gender for each of the variables studied at the age of 3 and 6 years, respectively.
To address the first objective—identifying which temperament dimensions remain most stable between ages three and six in each participant—a correlation study was conducted. The results (see
Table 3) show that 9 out of the 15 dimensions of child temperament exhibit a moderate correlation between the two measurements. These dimensions include the activity level, anger/frustration, discomfort, high-intensity pleasure, impulsivity, inhibitory control, low-intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity, and shyness.
To further examine the stability and change, a descriptive analysis was conducted using measures of the central tendency and mean comparison tests with the Student’s
t-test (see
Table 4) for the total sample. The results show significant differences in anger/frustration (AF), attentional focusing (AT), perceptual sensitivity (PS), and shyness (SH).
Thus, stability was found in 9 out of the 15 dimensions—however, significant changes were observed in 4 dimensions, indicating that the temperament stability is not uniform across all dimensions.
The second objective focuses on identifying the stability of temperament dimensions over time according to gender (see
Table 4). To this end, an analysis of differences in temperament dimensions by gender between ages three and six was conducted using descriptive statistics (measures of central tendency) and mean comparison tests with the Student’s
t-test. The results indicate significant differences in boys in the dimensions of anger/frustration (AF), attentional focusing (AT), and impulsivity (IM), with lower scores in AF, higher scores in AT, and lowers scores in IM. In relation to girls, significant differences were obtained in the following dimensions: attentional focusing (AT), fear (FE), perceptual sensitivity (PS), and shyness (SH), obtaining significantly higher mean scores in all dimensions except shyness (see
Table 4).
In addition, a study has been conducted to determine how different boys and girls are at age 6. The results show that the main differences are in the3 activity level (AL2), discomfort (DI2), and fear (FE2) (see
Table 5).
The third objective aims to assess whether certain temperament dimensions influence the socioemotional adjustment between the ages of three and six. To this end, a correlation analysis was conducted, which showed that there is no significant correlation between the SPECI scores and the CBQ dimensions when analyzing the full sample without distinguishing by gender (see
Table 6), except for a correlation between the internalizing dimension and FE2. This relationship was also explored by gender. In the case of boys, the results indicate that there is no significant correlation between temperament dimensions and the internalizing and externalizing factors of the SPECI. However, in girls significant correlations were identified between the externalizing factor and the dimensions of attentional focusing (AF) and smiling/laughter (SL), as well as between the internalizing factor and the dimensions of low-intensity pleasure (LP) and perceptual sensitivity (PS).
To determine whether internalizing and externalizing factors vary by gender (see
Table 7,
Table 8 and
Table 9), a mean comparison test was conducted using the Student’s
t-test. The results revealed significant differences in both factors, with higher scores in boys. Additionally, the differential item analysis of the SPECI, using the Mann–Whitney U test, showed significant differences in items 3, 4, and 5, which correspond to the categories of anxiety, the dependent-child pattern, and thought problems, respectively, in which boys scored higher.
4. Discussion
The main objective of this study was to analyze the dimensions of child temperament during the developmental stage from ages three to six, assessing their stability and variations, as well as their relationship with emotional and behavioral problems, taking into account gender differences. A series of specific objectives were established to guide the analysis of the results.
Regarding the first objective—identifying which temperament dimensions remain most stable between the ages of three and six—the results (see
Table 3) show that 9 out of the 15 dimensions of childhood temperament exhibit a moderate correlation. These dimensions include the activity level, anger/frustration, discomfort, high-intensity pleasure, impulsivity, inhibitory control, low-intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity, and shyness. These results are consistent with previous research [
9,
47,
49], which also suggest that certain temperament traits are shown to be stable across development [
13]. In this regard, there is evidence that dimensions such as the activity level, high-intensity pleasure, and impulsivity show a stability in early and middle childhood [
17], while others, such as anger/frustration, discomfort, and fear, along with self-regulation decrease significantly from early life to the preschool years []. Therefore, the temperament stability is not uniform across all dimensions, suggesting that some traits may be more strongly influenced by biological factors or socialization patterns.
Regarding the second objective—determining the changes that occur between ages three and six—the results indicate significant changes in four dimensions: anger/frustration, attentional focusing, perceptual sensitivity, and sadness. These findings are consistent with the existing literature, which suggests that these dimensions change due to socioemotional development [
49] and the maturation of executive functioning [
51,
52]. In addition, the family environment plays a key role in modulating these temperament dimensions [
47]. Specifically, Refs. [
32,
43,
47,
49] suggest that changes in the anger/frustration dimension are associated with advances in executive functions, such as the acquisition of self-regulation strategies, the development of cognitive skills, and neuroplasticity processes that allow for the better control of emotional responses. Likewise, Ref. [
47] emphasizes that a supportive family environment and appropriate parenting practices play an important role in shaping this dimension. However, other studies have shown that when children exhibit high levels of anger/frustration at an early age, this trait tends to persist over a longer period of time [
53]. With regard to attentional focusing, a faster development has been observed in children who receive cognitive stimulation and emotional support, which aligns with previous research on the development of the executive attention network [
53]. Furthermore, the literature suggests that this dimension tends to stabilize around age 4, when children begin engaging in more specific tasks that require sustained attentional control [
54]. As for perceptual sensitivity and sadness, these dimensions are shaped by the development of emotional self-regulation and are influenced by environmental and family factors. Therefore, a positive environment fosters the development of prosocial behaviors and healthy emotional adjustment [
11,
55,
56]. These findings highlight the role of environmental factors in socioemotional development and, consequently, in self-regulation, influencing how children perceive and manage stimuli [
55,
56].
Regarding the determination of stability and change in temperament dimensions from ages three to six by gender, the results indicate notable variations compared to the total sample when conducting a differential analysis. In boys, changes were observed in the dimensions of anger/frustration, attentional focusing, and impulsivity, while in girls, changes were found in attentional focusing, fear, perceptual sensitivity, and sadness. When considering the differences between boys and girls at age 6, the results show significant differences in activity levels, discomfort, and fear: boys tend to score higher in activity levels, while girls score higher in discomfort and fear. These findings are consistent with previous research, such as that of [
32,
47], which notes that boys generally have more difficulty controlling their impulses than girls, particularly in situations involving frustration or stimulation. Conversely, girls are more effective at inhibiting impulsive behavior and regulating their actions. Ref. [
32] also points out that girls at an early age tend to demonstrate a greater attentional capacity and self-regulation than boys, including skills such as waiting, planning, and controlling impulses. These dimensions are associated with greater inhibitory control, which is linked to compliance with rules as well as the development of empathy, guilt, and shame later on [
11]. On the other hand, these differences may also reflect social and cultural aspects. According to [
2,
47], certain forms of socialization promote more advanced self-regulation skills in girls from early childhood. The cultural environment and parenting practices affect these gender differences, as social expectations may reinforce the attentional capacity in girls, while boys are often socialized toward more active, physical play. This may influence their ability to sustain prolonged attention on specific tasks, as highlighted by [
57]. Attentional focusing, which tends to be higher in girls, enables them to respond to stimuli in a more controlled way and to develop self-control skills that support adaptive behavior [
58]. In relation to the dimension of impulsivity, Ref. [
59] observed that these gender differences are more pronounced in childhood but tend to decrease during adolescence. Again, these results can be explained by a combination of biological and environmental factors.
The third objective—assessing whether certain temperament dimensions predict emotional and behavioral problems with internalizing or externalizing symptoms at age 6, considering gender as a moderating variable—yielded different results depending on whether the total sample or gender-specific groups were analyzed. Thus, in the total sample, no significant relationships were generally observed between temperament dimensions and emotional or behavioral problems. This result could be explained by the nonclinical nature of the sample, which aligns with results reported by [
50]. However, when analyzing differences between boys and girls, distinct patterns emerged. In boys, no significant correlations were found between temperament dimensions and the internalizing and externalizing factors, although boys scored higher overall than girls on both factors—particularly in the categories of anxiety, the dependent-child pattern, and thought problems. These findings are consistent with those reported by [
60], suggesting that boys may be more prone to exhibiting problematic behaviors, both internalizing and externalizing, as supported by research such as [
61], which highlights the higher prevalence of behavioral and externalizing disorders in boys. In contrast, for girls, a moderate correlation was found between externalizing symptoms and the temperament dimensions of attentional focusing and smiling/laughter, as well as between internalizing symptoms and the dimensions of low-intensity pleasure and perceptual sensitivity. Previous studies have shown that girls tend to score higher in variables related to emotional awareness and other socioemotional traits, such as prosocial attitudes and empathy, which may support adaptation. On the other hand, girls also appear to express greater internalizing symptomatology, including depression and emotional distress [
62]. These findings partially confirm hypothesis H3, as the expected relationship between temperament and internalizing symptoms was identified in girls, while no clear patterns were observed in boys. Previous research has documented that high perceptual sensitivity and elevated negative affectivity may be associated with a greater vulnerability to anxiety and depression [
50].
These differences between boys and girls with respect to the expression of internalizing and externalizing problems may also be due to educational and parenting factors, as gender differences may be interpreted as social roles or expectations that affect behavior [
60]. The sociocultural environment in which boys and girls grow up has its own characteristics and values, to which children must adapt [
63].
In conclusion, this study reinforces the importance of child temperament as a key factor in socioemotional development during the ages of three to six. The findings demonstrate the stability of certain temperament dimensions in relation to gender, as well as their differential influence on emotional and behavioral problems in boys and girls. These differences not only reflect the influence of biological factors on the stability of temperament dimensions but also highlight the impact of parenting practices and the sociocultural environment on those dimensions that depend on the development of executive functions. The differences observed between boys and girls, and the specific dimensions in which these differences occur, reinforce the need to design targeted interventions tailored to temperamental and gender-specific characteristics, thus directly addressing our objectives of identifying stable and modifiable traits that predict socioemotional outcomes and informing educational and family practices to promote balanced development and minimize the occurrence of both internalizing and externalizing problems during this crucial stage of childhood development.
Moreover, this study identified specific temperament dimensions that function as protective factors. In particular, higher levels of effortful control—including attentional focusing and inhibitory control—as well as lower levels of negative affectivity dimensions, such as fear and sadness, were associated with better socioemotional adjustment, especially among girls.
This study presents several strengths that reinforce its validity and relevance. One of its main strengths lies in the longitudinal design and the differential analysis by gender. The longitudinal approach allowed the observation of the same individuals throughout their developmental period between the ages of three and six. This design provided a more comprehensive and dynamic perspective, facilitating the identification of temperament dimensions that remained stable or changed over time, while minimizing the selection bias and ensuring a greater consistency in the results. Moreover, this study highlighted how certain temperament dimensions, as well as their relationship with internalizing and externalizing problems, differ significantly when analyzed using the overall sample versus a gender-differentiated approach. This approach emphasizes the importance of studying temperament from a perspective that considers gender-specific traits, which is crucial for understanding socioemotional developmental trajectories.
On the other hand, this study also presents some limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the relatively small sample size may limit the generalizability of the findings. Future research could aim to replicate these results with larger and more representative samples.
Although the findings provide valuable insights, the representativeness of the sample is limited. The children included in this study were recruited from three early childhood education centers in Málaga with similar sociodemographic profiles. Therefore, caution is advised when generalizing the results to broader populations. Future studies should aim to replicate the findings with larger and more diverse samples and, where possible, compare outcomes with established normative data.
Another limitation concerns the exclusive use of caregiver reports to assess temperament and emotional/behavioral issues. While these reports are valuable, the inclusion of direct assessments or observational measures in future studies would provide a more objective perspective and help reduce potential response biases [
64].
Secondly, although this study analyzed the temperament stability, it did not control environmental factors such as the parenting quality or socioeconomic status, which could influence the variability of temperamental traits.
Moreover, it would be relevant to extend the follow-up beyond age six in order to assess whether changes in temperament persist into middle childhood and adolescence.
4.1. Educational Implications
The identification and understanding of child temperament are essential for families, educators, and mental health professionals, as they allow for a more accurate approach to individual needs in early childhood and their projection into adolescence. This knowledge can support the implementation of effective strategies and tools to promote social and emotional development and well-being [
8]. In addition to highlighting the stability and change in temperament dimensions, the results of this study point to differential development based on gender, emphasizing the importance of considering this variable when designing interventions aimed at improving emotionally appropriate social interactions [
65]. For example, since girls tend to show greater perceptual sensitivity, teachers may foster emotional regulation strategies in the classroom, such as mindfulness techniques, to help them manage sensory overstimulation. On the other hand, boys’ tendency to express higher levels of anger/frustration suggests the relevance of positive discipline strategies and early childhood training in conflict resolution skills.
4.2. Future Research
Future research on child temperament should explore the interaction between biological and sociocultural factors, including how parenting practices and gender expectations influence temperament stability and change. Additionally, it is important to investigate which specific strategies may help prevent internalizing and externalizing problems.