Next Article in Journal
Short-Term Effects over Time of Endotracheal Suctioning on Very Low Birth Weight Premature Infants with RDS
Previous Article in Journal
Histological Grade, Tumor Breadth, and Hypertension Predict Early Recurrence in Pediatric Sarcoma: A LASSO-Regularized Micro-Cohort Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parental Emotion Socialization and Child Adjustment in Greek Families: Supportive vs. Non-Supportive Parenting

by
Maria Markoulaki
1,
Christina Dimitrakaki
1,
Andoniki Naska
1,
Katerina Papanikolaou
2 and
Georgios Giannakopoulos
2,*
1
Department of Hygiene, Epidemiology and Medical Statistics, School of Medicine, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 115 27 Athens, Greece
2
Department of Child Psychiatry, School of Medicine, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Aghia Sophia Children’s Hospital, 115 27 Athens, Greece
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2025, 12(7), 807; https://doi.org/10.3390/children12070807
Submission received: 17 May 2025 / Revised: 11 June 2025 / Accepted: 18 June 2025 / Published: 20 June 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Mental Health)

Abstract

Background/Objectives: Parental emotion socialization is a key influence on children’s emotional regulation and psychosocial development. This study examined how supportive and non-supportive parental responses to children’s negative emotions are associated with internalizing and externalizing problems and social competence among Greek children aged 6–12 years. Methods: A non-experimental, cross-sectional correlational study using convenience sampling was conducted with 100 Greek parents who completed the Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale and the Child Behavior Checklist. Correlation and multiple regression analyses were used to examine associations between parenting responses and child outcomes, controlling for child age, child gender, and parent gender. Results: Minimization/devaluation responses were significantly associated with higher internalizing problems (β = 0.396, p = 0.009), externalizing problems (β = 0.264, p = 0.048), and total problems (β = 0.320, p = 0.012). Punitive responses significantly predicted externalizing (β = 0.383, p = 0.003) and total problems (β = 0.304, p = 0.004). Supportive strategies (e.g., emotion-focused and problem-focused responses) did not significantly predict lower problem scores but were positively correlated with social competence (e.g., problem-focused response: ρ = 0.25, p = 0.017). Parent gender predicted minimization/devaluation responses, with mothers scoring higher than fathers (β = 0.308, p = 0.006). Conclusions: Non-supportive parental responses—especially punitive and dismissive behaviors—are robustly associated with increased behavioral and emotional difficulties in children. While supportive strategies may contribute to social competence, their benefits appear diminished in the presence of negative parenting practices. These findings highlight the importance of culturally sensitive interventions tailored to Greek family dynamics. Despite limitations such as the use of self-report measures and a non-representative sample, this study contributes valuable insights into emotion socialization and child adjustment in a specific cultural context.

1. Introduction

Parental emotion socialization is a central construct in developmental psychology, reflecting the processes by which parents shape their children’s understanding, expression, and regulation of emotions [1,2,3]. Parental emotion socialization refers to the ways in which parents influence their children’s understanding, expression, and regulation of emotions through a variety of strategies, including modeling emotional responses, discussing emotions, and directly responding to children’s emotional expressions [2,4]. These socialization processes can be supportive—validating and guiding the child’s emotional experiences—or non-supportive—dismissing, punishing, or minimizing the child’s emotions. Over the past several decades, considerable attention has been given to how early family interactions contribute to the formation of emotional competence and overall psychosocial adjustment [5,6]. In this context, parental responses to children’s negative emotions—whether supportive or non-supportive—play a decisive role in influencing a child’s ability to manage stress, form healthy relationships, and navigate complex social environments [7,8,9,10]. For example, a supportive response might involve comforting a child who is upset and helping them find a solution to the problem, whereas a non-supportive response might include telling the child to stop crying or punishing them for showing distress.
Drawing on foundational theories such as attachment theory—which emphasizes the importance of early relational bonds in shaping later psychological functioning—researchers have argued that the ways in which parents respond to emotional distress provide critical models for children’s own emotion regulation strategies [11,12,13]. Supportive responses that involve acknowledging and validating a child’s emotional experience, guiding them through problem-solving, and encouraging adaptive coping tend to foster resilience and social competence [14,15,16,17]. Conversely, non-supportive responses, such as punitive or dismissive reactions, may predispose children to internalizing or externalizing difficulties by impairing their capacity to effectively manage negative emotions [18,19,20].
Despite these insights, several gaps remain in our understanding of parental emotion socialization, particularly when considering cultural and contextual factors. Much of the existing research has been conducted in North American or northern European settings, where cultural norms regarding emotional expression and family dynamics differ from those in other regions [21,22]. These studies have generally shown that supportive parenting practices, such as emotion coaching and validation, are associated with better emotional regulation and fewer behavioral problems in children, while non-supportive practices—like punishment or emotional dismissal—are linked to increased internalizing and externalizing symptoms. In Greece, traditional family values and cultural practices shape parenting styles in unique ways that may influence how emotion socialization unfolds in everyday life [23,24]. Traditional Greek families are typically characterized by close-knit family bonds, strong parental authority, and an emphasis on familial loyalty and emotional interdependence. Parenting practices often reflect a combination of protective behaviors and expectations of conformity, influenced by broader collectivistic cultural norms. Emotional expressiveness is valued within family settings, but children are also expected to demonstrate respect and self-regulation in their emotional displays. These cultural characteristics may shape the ways in which parents respond to children’s emotions, blending supportive strategies with more directive or controlling behaviors [23,24]. Recognizing these cultural nuances is essential, as they can moderate the relationship between parental behavior and children’s developmental outcomes [25,26,27].
From a lifespan developmental perspective, early experiences with emotion socialization are not isolated events; rather, they establish a foundation that influences emotional regulation and psychological well-being across different stages of life [28]. Research that integrates established theoretical frameworks with cultural considerations offers a more comprehensive understanding of how early family processes contribute to mental health outcomes over time. Focusing on a sample of parents of children aged 6 to 12 years—a critical period during which foundational emotional and social skills are rapidly developing—provides a valuable window into how early parental influences may shape a child’s trajectory toward adaptive functioning or increased vulnerability to psychological difficulties [29].
In summary, the present study is designed to address these gaps by examining the associations between parental emotion socialization and children’s psychosocial adjustment within a Greek sample. Specifically, it aims to determine whether supportive parental responses are associated with lower levels of emotional distress and behavioral challenges, whether non-supportive reactions are linked to elevated internalizing and externalizing problems, and how these associations are moderated by the unique cultural factors inherent in Greek parenting practices. By integrating theoretical models from attachment theory, emotion regulation, and cultural psychology, this research is expected to advance our theoretical understanding of emotion socialization and inform culturally sensitive interventions aimed at promoting supportive parenting practices and enhancing children’s long-term mental health outcomes.
Despite these insights, few studies have explicitly examined these dynamics within southern European or Mediterranean cultural settings, where emotional expressiveness and parental authority often co-exist. Previous findings suggest that both the attachment relationship and emotion regulation capacity may serve as key mechanisms linking parental emotion socialization with child adjustment [11,28]. However, little is known about how these pathways operate in Greek families. The current study addresses this gap by using a predictive analytic framework to examine associations between different types of parental responses and child outcomes, while also considering demographic factors such as parent and child gender.
Therefore, the primary goal of the present study was to investigate how different parental emotion socialization practices are associated with children’s psychosocial adjustment in a sample of Greek families. To guide this investigation, we posed the following research questions: (1) Are supportive parental responses (e.g., problem-focused, emotion-focused, expressive encouragement) associated with lower levels of internalizing and externalizing problems and higher levels of social competence in children? (2) Are non-supportive parental responses (e.g., punitive, minimization/devaluation) associated with higher levels of internalizing and externalizing problems? (3) Do these associations remain significant after accounting for child age, child gender, and parent gender? Based on existing literature, we formulated the following hypotheses: (1) Supportive parental responses (e.g., problem-focused, emotion-focused, expressive encouragement) would be associated with lower levels of internalizing and externalizing problems, and higher levels of social competence in children, possibly through enhanced emotion regulation. (2) Non-supportive parental responses (e.g., punitive, minimization/devaluation) would be associated with higher levels of internalizing and externalizing problems, potentially due to disruptions in the parent-child attachment dynamic or emotional invalidation. (3) These associations would remain significant even when controlling for child gender, child age, and parent gender, as examined through multiple regression analyses.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Research Design

This study employed a non-experimental, cross-sectional correlational design to examine the associations between parental emotion socialization and children’s psychosocial adjustment. Data were collected through self-report questionnaires administered to parents, allowing for the investigation of how different parental responses relate to internalizing and externalizing problems in children.

2.2. Participants

The sample comprised 100 parents of children aged between 6 and 12 years. The sample size was determined based on feasibility and in accordance with similar studies in this research area. While no formal power analysis was conducted, previous research examining emotion socialization and behavioral outcomes has used comparable sample sizes to detect medium effect sizes with sufficient statistical power. Each participating parent completed the questionnaires independently, reporting on only one child. No cases involved both parents completing measures for the same child, and no parent provided assessments for more than one child. Participants were recruited using convenience sampling from various urban areas in Greece, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. Inclusion criteria required that respondents be primary caregivers of a child within the specified age range and be proficient in Greek. Demographic characteristics, including age, gender, and other relevant background variables, are summarized in Table 1. Detailed socio-demographic information regarding parental education level, employment status, and perceived financial situation was not collected in this study. Similarly, data regarding the number of siblings and the child’s birth order were not gathered. Parents were not specifically asked about formal diagnoses (e.g., ADHD, ASD) for their children; however, based on general screening during recruitment, no major developmental concerns were reported by participating families.

2.3. Instruments

2.3.1. Parental Emotion Socialization

Parental responses to children’s negative emotions were assessed using the Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES) [30]. This instrument evaluates multiple dimensions of parental behavior by classifying responses as either supportive (e.g., emotion-focused, problem-focused) or non-supportive (e.g., punitive, dismissive, minimizing). Example items from the CCNES include supportive responses such as helping the child understand their feelings or encouraging problem-solving (e.g., comforting the child when upset), and non-supportive responses such as reacting with frustration, ignoring the emotion, or dismissing the child’s feelings as exaggerated. These illustrative items help capture the spectrum of parental behaviors the scale assesses. The CCNES has demonstrated satisfactory reliability and validity in previous research [31]. In the present study, internal consistency for the CCNES subscales was as follows: Distress/Anxiety Response (α = 0.76), Punitive Response (α = 0.77), Expressive Encouragement Response (α = 0.93), Emotion-Focused Response (α = 0.91), Problem-Focused Response (α = 0.81), and Minimization/Devaluation Response (α = 0.87), indicating acceptable to excellent reliability.

2.3.2. Children’s Psychosocial Adjustment

Children’s psychosocial functioning was measured using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [32,33]. The CBCL provides comprehensive assessments of internalizing (e.g., anxiety, depression) and externalizing (e.g., aggression, hyperactivity) problems, as well as overall social competence. Example items from the CBCL reflect a range of behavioral and emotional concerns, such as frequent crying, difficulty concentrating, disobedience at home, or challenges interacting with peers. These examples correspond to the internalizing, externalizing, and social adjustment domains measured by the instrument. The CBCL is widely recognized for its robust psychometric properties [34]. In the present study, internal consistency for the CBCL subscales was high, with Cronbach’s alpha values as follows: Internalizing Problems (α = 0.85), Externalizing Problems (α = 0.90), and Total Problems (α = 0.93), indicating excellent reliability in this sample. It should be noted that the CCNES has not been formally validated for use in Greek populations. Although it was translated and adapted with care, this represents a limitation that should be considered when interpreting the findings.

2.4. Procedure

Data collection was conducted over a two-month period. Potential participants were initially contacted through schools, community centers, and local parenting networks. Upon expressing interest, parents received comprehensive written and verbal explanations regarding the study’s purpose, procedures, and confidentiality protocols. A brief introductory session was then conducted by the first author to ensure that participants fully understood the questionnaires and to address any initial questions or concerns.
After this interaction, parents who agreed to participate provided written informed consent. In addition to completing the Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES) and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), participants also supplied demographic information and data on potential confounding factors (i.e., parent’s gender and child’s age and gender). These supplementary data were collected to enhance the robustness of subsequent analyses by allowing for the control of these variables.
Participants had the option to complete the questionnaires in a quiet setting at home or in a controlled environment provided by the research team. During the administration of the questionnaires, the first author remained available to clarify any items if needed, thereby ensuring that all questions were properly understood and reducing the likelihood of response errors.
All responses were collected anonymously, with each questionnaire being assigned a unique code to protect participant confidentiality. The completed questionnaires and additional data were then securely stored for further analysis.

2.5. Ethical Considerations

This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval were waived because the research involved minimal risk and relied solely on the anonymous and voluntary participation of adult parents who completed standardized, non-invasive self-report questionnaires. No sensitive personal data or identifying information were collected, and no interventions or experimental procedures were conducted.
Prior to participation, all respondents were provided with detailed information about the study’s purpose, procedures, and confidentiality protocols. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participation was entirely voluntary, and individuals were informed of their right to withdraw from the study at any time without any penalty. All data were collected and stored anonymously to ensure the privacy and confidentiality of participants.

2.6. Data Analysis

Descriptive statistics were computed to summarize participant demographics and scale scores. Prior to the main analyses, the distributions of all key variables were examined for normality. Variables exhibiting significant skewness were log-transformed to meet the assumptions of linear regression analyses, which were used in subsequent steps. For the correlation analyses, non-parametric Spearman’s rho was employed to assess associations between parental responses and children’s psychosocial outcomes. This approach was selected due to the ordinal nature of some response scales and the presence of residual skewness in several variables, even after transformation.
Multiple regression analyses were performed to assess the unique contributions of supportive and non-supportive parental responses in predicting internalizing and externalizing problems while controlling for potential confounding variables. Specifically, the following confounders were included in the models: child age was entered as a continuous variable, while child gender and parent gender were entered as categorical variables (with child gender coded as 0 for female and 1 for male, and parent gender coded as 0 for father and 1 for mother). Including these variables allowed us to account for their potential influence on the outcome measures.
For all regression models, standardized beta coefficients (β) were estimated and reported to facilitate comparisons of predictor effects. Statistical significance was determined at p < 0.05. Data analysis was performed using SPSS software (version 26.0).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics of the Sample

A total of 100 Greek parents participated in the study; all were the biological parents of their children. Among the parents, 82.1% were mothers, and 55.7% of the children were boys. The average age of the children was 10 years (SD = 1.7). Detailed sample characteristics are provided in Table 1.

3.2. Descriptive Measures for CBCL and CCNES Scales

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was used to assess various aspects of children’s behavior. The mean score for internalizing problems was 5.33 (SD = 5.35) and for externalizing problems was 4.05 (SD = 3.72). The overall CBCL total problems score averaged 22.55 (SD = 4.29). The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES) scores (range 1–7, with higher scores indicating more frequent use) were as follows: Distress/Anxiety Response 2.93 (SD = 0.68), Punitive Response 2.04 (SD = 0.63), Expressive Encouragement Response 5.05 (SD = 1.33), Emotion-Focused Response 5.53 (SD = 1.18), Problem-Focused Response 5.90 (SD = 0.82), and Minimization/Devaluation Response 3.07 (SD = 1.15). Table 2 provides additional descriptive details, including the observed minimum and maximum values and median scores.

3.3. Correlation Analyses

Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated between the CBCL and CCNES scales. The Distress/Anxiety Response was significantly and positively correlated with Anxiety/Depression (rho = 0.22, p = 0.047), Internalizing Problems (rho = 0.25, p = 0.021), and Social Problems (rho = 0.25, p = 0.023). The Punitive Response showed significant positive correlations with Aggressive Behavior (rho = 0.37, p = 0.001), Externalizing Problems (rho = 0.34, p = 0.001), and Total Problems (rho = 0.29, p = 0.006). The Expressive Encouragement Response correlated significantly with Activities (rho = 0.26, p = 0.017) and Overall Competence (rho = 0.27, p = 0.012). Additionally, the Emotion-Focused Response, Problem-Focused Response, and Minimization/Devaluation Response were each significantly correlated with various CBCL scales. Table 3 summarizes the complete set of Spearman correlations.

3.4. Regression Analyses

3.4.1. Regression Analyses on CCNES Dimensions

Multiple linear regression analyses were performed with the CCNES dimensions as dependent variables and child gender, child age, and parent gender as independent variables. For the dimensions of Distress/Anxiety, Punitive, Expressive Encouragement, Emotion-Focused, and Problem-Focused responses, none of the demographic variables emerged as significant predictors. In contrast, the Minimization/Devaluation Response was significantly predicted by parent gender; mothers demonstrated higher scores compared to fathers (β = 0.132, p = 0.006). These findings are detailed in Table 4.

3.4.2. Regression Analyses on CBCL Problems

Additional regressions were performed with CBCL Internalizing, Externalizing, and Total Problems as dependent variables. In these analyses, predictors included child gender, child age, parent gender, and the CCNES dimensions. Internalizing Problems were significantly predicted only by the Minimization/Devaluation Response (β = 0.135, p = 0.009). Externalizing Problems were significantly associated with both the Punitive Response (β = 0.206, p = 0.003) and the Minimization/Devaluation Response (β = 0.070, p = 0.048). Total Problems were significantly predicted by the Punitive Response (β = 0.186, p = 0.004) and the Minimization/Devaluation Response (β = 0.108, p = 0.012), with the latter showing a slightly greater standardized effect. These results are summarized in Table 5.
Overall, the findings highlight the distinct roles of supportive and non-supportive parental responses in child adjustment. Notably, punitive and minimization/devaluation behaviors emerged as the most consistent predictors of higher internalizing and externalizing symptoms, while supportive strategies correlated with social competence but did not significantly predict reduced problem behaviors. These results suggest that reducing negative parental reactions may be particularly critical in addressing behavioral and emotional difficulties in children.

4. Discussion

The present study contributes significantly to our understanding of how parental emotion socialization is associated with children’s psychosocial adjustment. The findings reveal that non-supportive parental responses—particularly punitive and minimization/devaluation behaviors—are strongly associated with increased internalizing and externalizing problems. Descriptive analyses indicated that while parents reported moderately high levels of supportive responses (e.g., emotion-focused and problem-focused) and lower scores on non-supportive dimensions (e.g., punitive responses), even these relatively lower frequencies of unsupportive reactions exert a substantial negative influence on child outcomes. This suggests that the detrimental impact of unsupportive practices may outweigh the benefits of supportive interactions, thereby highlighting a potential target for intervention.
One key interpretation of these results is that children who are frequently exposed to punitive or dismissive parental behaviors may not develop effective emotion regulation strategies. Over time, such non-supportive practices can lead to maladaptive coping patterns that predispose children to greater emotional distress and behavioral difficulties [18,19,35]. This is further supported by our regression analyses, which demonstrated that minimization/devaluation responses significantly predicted internalizing problems and, along with punitive responses, also predicted externalizing and total problems. In contrast, although supportive responses have been linked to enhanced emotional understanding and resilience [14,15,16,17], their influence may be overshadowed when even minimal non-supportive behaviors are present. This asymmetry underscores the critical need to minimize negative parental practices to promote healthier developmental outcomes.
The implications for practice are substantial. These findings advocate for intervention programs that prioritize the reduction of non-supportive parental responses [36,37]. Parent-training initiatives that incorporate emotion coaching and stress management techniques could enable caregivers to modify punitive behaviors and foster a more supportive home environment [5,38,39,40]. Notably, in the Greek cultural context—where traditional family dynamics and cultural norms strongly shape parenting practices—tailoring interventions to reflect these nuances may enhance their effectiveness and ultimately lead to improved mental health outcomes for children [25,26,27]. In the Greek context, parent-training initiatives might incorporate culturally relevant values—such as respect for authority and family cohesion—into emotion coaching strategies to enhance receptivity and engagement. Collaborations with schools and community centers could support broader implementation. Specifically, such intervention programs could integrate these familiar cultural values into evidence-based frameworks by helping parents recognize dismissive or punitive responses and practice supportive strategies aligned with Greek parenting norms. These programs could be delivered in accessible settings such as schools, pediatric clinics, and local community hubs. By tailoring approaches to local family dynamics, such initiatives would promote emotional communication within families and help reduce children’s emotional and behavioral difficulties.
These findings are consistent with previous cross-cultural research suggesting that parental emotion socialization practices are deeply influenced by cultural norms and values. For instance, studies in East Asian and Mediterranean societies—both characterized by collectivistic orientations—have found that emotional guidance and parental control often coexist, shaping emotion regulation in ways distinct from more individualistic contexts [21,23,24]. Similar to our findings in Greek families, punitive or dismissive responses in these settings are linked to adverse child outcomes, underscoring the potentially universal risk posed by non-supportive parenting. At the same time, the benefits of supportive responses may be moderated by cultural expectations around emotional restraint, hierarchical relationships, and relational harmony—factors that could attenuate their impact in more traditional family systems.
The observed associations between non-supportive parental responses and child difficulties align with the broader literature indicating that punitive or dismissive parenting practices are universal risk factors for emotional and behavioral problems [18,19,20]. Thus, while some of the results may seem expected, their replication in a culturally distinct sample contributes important cross-cultural validation to existing theoretical models.
Furthermore, our regression analyses revealed that among demographic variables, parent gender played a role in non-supportive responses, with mothers exhibiting higher levels of minimization/devaluation than fathers. This insight could guide the development of gender-sensitive training programs [41]. However, it is important to interpret this finding with caution, given the substantial imbalance in the number of mothers and fathers in our sample. The small number of participating fathers may limit the generalizability of gender-based comparisons. Nevertheless, this finding opens interesting avenues for further exploration. It may reflect gendered expectations in emotional caregiving or differences in stress exposure between mothers and fathers in Greek families. For instance, mothers may more frequently encounter children’s emotional expressions and feel pressured to manage them efficiently, sometimes leading to dismissive responses. Future studies with more balanced gender representation should examine whether these patterns hold and explore the underlying mechanisms.
Beyond practical applications, the study also enriches our theoretical understanding of emotion socialization. It supports the notion that early interactions within the family serve as a foundation for later emotional and behavioral regulation [5,6,28]. Importantly, the findings suggest that cultural context may moderate these relationships. In Greek families, for example, the interplay between traditional values and modern parenting practices might influence how parental responses affect children’s adjustment [23]. This observation invites further exploration into how cultural factors can both buffer and exacerbate the impact of parental behavior on child development.
It is important to acknowledge that the relationship between parental emotion socialization practices and children’s psychosocial adjustment is likely to be bidirectional. One plausible mechanism linking non-supportive parenting to child adjustment is the disruption of emotion regulation development, as children exposed to punitive responses may internalize maladaptive coping strategies [42]. From an attachment perspective, these parenting behaviors may also weaken the sense of emotional security needed for adaptive regulation [43,44]. Children’s emotional and behavioral characteristics, such as temperament, self-regulation skills, and even birth order, may influence how parents respond to their emotional expressions [45,46]. Furthermore, broader environmental factors, including socio-economic conditions, parental stress, and family structure, may simultaneously affect both parenting behaviors and child outcomes [47]. While the current study focused on parental responses as predictors, future longitudinal research incorporating assessments of child temperament, environmental stressors, and family dynamics is needed to better understand the complex, reciprocal processes underlying emotion socialization.
While the present study offers both practical and theoretical contributions, its findings should be interpreted with caution due to several limitations. First, the exclusive reliance on self-report measures may introduce response biases; parents might underreport negative behaviors or overestimate supportive practices, which could affect the observed associations. In addition, the CCNES has not been formally validated in Greek, and results should be interpreted in light of potential cultural or linguistic nuances that may affect response accuracy. The cross-sectional design further limits our ability to infer causality—although the correlations and regression analyses provide a snapshot of current parental practices and child outcomes, longitudinal research is needed to clarify the directionality and stability of these relationships. Moreover, the use of a non-probability sample further limits representativeness, which restricts the generalizability of our findings to the broader population of Greek families. Furthermore, detailed socio-demographic data (e.g., parental education, employment status, financial situation) and family structure information (e.g., number of siblings, birth order) were not collected, limiting our ability to explore contextual influences on parenting and child outcomes. Finally, residual confounding may be present; for example, variables such as parental age and socio-economic characteristics (e.g., educational attainment) were not adjusted for in our regression models, even though these factors could influence both parenting behaviors and children’s psychosocial outcomes. Future research should incorporate these variables to more accurately isolate the associations between parental emotion socialization and child adjustment.
Looking forward, several avenues for future research emerge. Longitudinal studies are needed to track how changes in parental behavior over time influence children’s emotional development and psychosocial adjustment. Such research could clarify whether reducing non-supportive responses leads to sustained improvements in child outcomes. Additionally, expanding the sample to include more diverse socio-economic and regional groups within Greece—and even cross-cultural comparisons—would enhance our understanding of how universal versus culturally specific these processes might be. Further research could also integrate qualitative methods to capture the nuanced experiences of parents and children, offering a richer context for interpreting quantitative findings.
Moreover, exploring potential mediating mechanisms could deepen our insight into how parental emotion socialization exerts its influence. For instance, future studies might investigate whether factors such as parental stress, child temperament, or the quality of parent–child attachment serve as mediators in the relationship between parenting practices and child outcomes [48].
In addition to psychosocial and behavioral outcomes, future research could explore the biological relevance of early emotion socialization. Emerging evidence suggests that parenting practices may influence neurobiological systems involved in emotion regulation and stress response, such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and neural circuits related to emotional processing. Investigating biological markers—such as cortisol levels or functional brain imaging—alongside parental responses and child outcomes could offer a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms linking early family interactions to long-term mental health. Integrating such measures would help clarify whether the patterns observed behaviorally also manifest at the neurobiological level [49,50,51].
In conclusion, the study underscores the critical role of parental emotion socialization in shaping children’s developmental trajectories. By replicating established associations between non-supportive parenting and child maladjustment in a Greek cultural context, this research provides important cross-cultural validation of theoretical models developed largely in Western settings. These findings highlight both the universal risk associated with punitive and dismissive parenting, and the need to tailor interventions to cultural values and family structures. As such, the study makes a valuable contribution to cross-cultural psychology and underscores the importance of culturally sensitive approaches in developmental research.

5. Conclusions

This study highlights the strong association between non-supportive parental responses—especially punitive and dismissive behaviors—and increased internalizing and externalizing problems in children. Supportive responses showed positive links to social competence but had less predictive power in the presence of negative parenting. These findings underscore the need for culturally sensitive interventions that reduce non-supportive parenting and promote emotional guidance. Despite limitations such as a cross-sectional design and self-report measures, the study contributes cross-cultural evidence on parenting and child adjustment in Greek families. Longitudinal and more diverse studies are needed to inform future interventions. While grounded in the specific cultural and familial dynamics of Greek society, these findings may offer broader relevance for other collectivistic or hierarchical cultural contexts, though further cross-cultural validation is necessary.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.M. and G.G.; methodology, M.M. and G.G.; formal analysis, M.M.; data curation, M.M.; writing—original draft, M.M. and G.G.; writing—review and editing, M.M., G.G., C.D., A.N. and K.P.; supervision, G.G.; project administration, M.M. and G.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study because it involved only the anonymous, voluntary completion of standardized questionnaires by adult participants. No identifying or sensitive personal data were collected, and the research posed minimal risk to participants. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author due to privacy reasons.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge all participants in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
CBCLChild Behavior Checklist
CCNESCoping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale

References

  1. Jin, Z.; Zhang, X.; Han, Z.R. Parental emotion socialization and child psychological adjustment among chinese urban families: Mediation through child emotion regulation and moderation through dyadic collaboration. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Eisenberg, N.; Cumberland, A.; Spinrad, T.L. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychol. Inq. 1998, 9, 241–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Alonso, A.; Cabrera, N.J.; Kerlow, M.A.; Reich, S.M. Mothers’ and fathers’ emotion socialization: Longitudinal relations with toddlers’ social competence. Infant Behav. Dev. 2025, 78, 102034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hajal, N.; Paley, B. Parental emotion and emotion regulation: A critical target of study for research and intervention to promote child emotion socialization. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 403–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Paley, B.; Hajal, N.J. Conceptualizing emotion regulation and coregulation as family-level phenomena. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2022, 25, 19–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Li, D.; Li, W.; Zhu, X. Parenting style and children emotion management skills among chinese children aged 3–6: The chain mediation effect of self-control and peer interactions. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1231920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Hong, Y.; McCormick, S.A.; Deater-Deckard, K.; Calkins, S.D.; Bell, M.A. Household chaos, parental responses to emotion, and child emotion regulation in middle childhood. Soc. Dev. 2021, 30, 786–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Perry, N.B.; Dollar, J.M.; Calkins, S.D.; Keane, S.P.; Shanahan, L. Maternal socialization of child emotion and adolescent adjustment: Indirect effects through emotion regulation. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 541–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Trumello, C.; Ballarotto, G.; Ricciardi, P.; Paciello, M.; Marino, V.; Morelli, M.; Tambelli, R.; Babore, A. Mothers and fathers of pre-school children: A study on parenting stress and child’s emotional-behavioral difficulties. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 25367–25378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bjørk, R.; Havighurst, S.; Fredriksen, E.; Bølstad, E. Up you get: Norwegian parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions. Scand. J. Psychol. 2024, 65, 1039–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cassidy, J.; Jones, J.D.; Shaver, P.R. Contributions of attachment theory and research: A framework for future research, translation, and policy. Dev. Psychopathol. 2013, 25, 1415–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bosmans, G.; Van Vlierberghe, L.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Kobak, R.; Hermans, D.; van IJzendoorn, M.H. A learning theory approach to attachment theory: Exploring clinical applications. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2022, 25, 591–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fernandes, C.; Santos, A.; Fernandes, M.; Veríssimo, M.; Santos, A. Caregivers’ responses to children’s negative emotions: Associations with preschoolers’ executive functioning. Children 2022, 9, 1075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Lathren, C.; Bluth, K.; Zvara, B. Parent self-compassion and supportive responses to child difficult emotion: An intergenerational theoretical model rooted in attachment. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 2020, 12, 368–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Prates, P.E.G.; Correa-Júnior, A.J.S.; Russo, T.; Paraizo-Horvath, C.M.S.; Teles, A.; Sonobe, H.M. Effectiveness of family coping interventions in improving problem-solving skills in the care of children and adolescent cancer survivors during and after treatment: A scoping review. Nurs. Rep. 2024, 14, 2153–2178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Frydenberg, E.; Deans, J.; Liang, R. Developing coping skills in the early years: A positive educational approach. In The Palgrave Handbook of Positive Education; Kern, M.L., Wehmeyer, M.L., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2021; pp. 369–393. [Google Scholar]
  17. Butler, N.; Quigg, Z.; Bates, R.; Jones, L.; Ashworth, E.; Gowland, S.; Jones, M. The contributing role of family, school, and peer supportive relationships in protecting the mental wellbeing of children and adolescents. School Ment. Health 2022, 14, 776–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Liu, J. Childhood externalizing behavior: Theory and implications. J. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2004, 17, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Mackler, J.S.; Kelleher, R.T.; Shanahan, L.; Calkins, S.D.; Keane, S.P.; O’Brien, M. Parenting stress, parental reactions, and externalizing behavior from ages 4 to 10. J. Marriage Fam. 2015, 77, 388–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zubizarreta, A.; Calvete, E.; Hankin, B. Punitive parenting style and psychological problems in childhood: The moderating role of warmth and temperament. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2018, 28, 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kirchner-Häusler, A.; De Leersnyder, J.; Uskul, A.K.; Mirzada, F.; Vignoles, V.L.; Rodríguez-Bailón, R.; Castillo, V.A.; Cross, S.E.; Gezici-Yalçın, M.; Harb, C.; et al. Cultural fit of emotions and subjective well-being: Replicating comparative evidence and extending it to the mediterranean region. Curr. Res. Ecol. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 5, 100171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Eichbaum, Q.; Barbeau-Meunier, C.A.; White, M.; Ravi, R.; Grant, E.; Riess, H.; Bleakley, A. Empathy across cultures-one size does not fit all: From the ego-logical to the eco-logical of relational empathy. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. Theory Pract. 2023, 28, 643–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Kadoglou, M.; Tziaka, E.; Kotsis, K.; Samakouri, M.; Serdari, A. Exploring parenting practices in greek families through the “child rearing practice report” questionnaire: A validation study. Clin. Pediatr. 2025, 64, 1016–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Vasiou, A.; Kassis, W.; Krasanaki, A.; Aksoy, D.; Favre, C.A.; Tantaros, S. Exploring parenting styles patterns and children’s socio-emotional skills. Children 2023, 10, 1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lansford, J.E. Annual research review: Cross-cultural similarities and differences in parenting. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2022, 63, 466–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. He, H.; Usami, S.; Rikimaru, Y.; Jiang, L. Cultural roots of parenting: Mothers’ parental social cognitions and practices from western us and Shanghai/China. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 565040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Acet, P.; Oliver, B.R. Determinants of mindful parenting: A cross-cultural examination of parent and child reports. Curr. Psychol. 2024, 43, 562–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Noroña-Zhou, A.N.; Tung, I. Developmental patterns of emotion regulation in toddlerhood: Examining predictors of change and long-term resilience. Infant Ment. Health J. 2021, 42, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Montroy, J.J.; Bowles, R.P.; Skibbe, L.E.; McClelland, M.M.; Morrison, F.J. The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 1744–1762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fabes, R.; Eisenberg, N.; Bernzweig, J. The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale: Description and Scoring; Unpublished scale; Department of Family Resources and Human Development, Arizona State University: Tempe, AZ, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  31. Wilhelmsen, T.; Rydland, V.; Buøen, E.S.; Drugli, M.B.; Lekhal, R. Validation of the coping with children’s negative emotions scale adapted to the early childhood education and care context. Early Educ. Dev. 2024, 36, 708–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Achenbach, T.M. The child behavior checklist and related instruments. In The Use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcomes Assessment, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 429–466. [Google Scholar]
  33. Roussos, A.; Karantanos, G.; Richardson, C.; Hartman, C.; Karajiannis, D.; Kyprianos, S.; Lazaratou, H.; Mahaira, O.; Tassi, M.; Zoubou, V. Achenbach’s child behavior checklist and teachers’ report form in a normative sample of greek children 6-12 years old. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1999, 8, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Warnick, E.M.; Bracken, M.B.; Kasl, S. Screening efficiency of the child behavior checklist and strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A systematic review. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2008, 13, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Riemens, A.; Portengen, C.; Endendijk, J. Associations between parental dispositional attributions, dismissing and coaching reactions to children’s emotions, and children’s problem behaviour moderated by child gender. Cogn. Emot. 2023, 37, 1057–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sanders, M.R. The triple p system of evidence-based parenting support: Past, present, and future directions. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 26, 880–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jeong, J.; Franchett, E.; De Oliveira, C.R.; Rehmani, K.; Yousafzai, A. Parenting interventions to promote early child development in the first three years of life: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2021, 18, e1003602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Chan, C.K.Y.; Fu, K.; Liu, S.K.Y. Incorporating emotion coaching into behavioral parent training program: Evaluation of its effectiveness. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2024, 55, 236–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Liu, S.; Ren, L. A pilot study to examine the effects of an emotion coaching parenting program for chinese parents of preschoolers. Prev. Sci. 2025, 26, 222–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Power, T.; Ramos, G.; Guerrero, Y.O.; Martinez, A.D.; Parker, L.; Lee, S. Evaluation of a program to help low-income, latina mothers help their children cope with stress. J. Prim. Prev. 2021, 42, 257–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Waters, G.M.; Tidswell, G.R.; Bryant, E.J. Mothers’ and fathers’ views on the importance of play for their children’s development: Gender differences, academic activities, and the parental role. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2022, 92, 1571–1581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Yang, Y.; Song, Q.; Doan, S.; Wang, Q. Maternal reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s socio-emotional development among european american and chinese immigrant children. Transcult. Psychiatry 2020, 57, 408–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bertie, L.-A.; Johnston, K.; Lill, S. Parental emotion socialisation of young children and the mediating role of emotion regulation. Austral. J. Psychol. 2021, 73, 293–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Boldt, L.; Goffin, K.; Kochanska, G. The significance of early parent-child attachment for emerging regulation: A longitudinal investigation of processes and mechanisms from toddler age to preadolescence. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 431–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Edler, K.; Valentino, K. Parental self-regulation and engagement in emotion socialization: A systematic review. Psychol. Bull. 2024, 150, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Hastings, P.; Grady, J.; Barrieau, L. Children’s anxious characteristics predict how their parents socialize emotions. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2018, 47, 1225–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Sager, A.A.; Goodman, S.; Jeong, J.; Bain, P.; Ahun, M. Effects of multi-component parenting and parental mental health interventions on early childhood development and parent outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2024, 8, 656–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gómez-Ortiz, O.; Ortiz-Alba, M.; Falla, D.; Romera, E.M. Are parental stress and rewards influenced by child temperament? Analysis of the moderating role of social support and gender in spanish parents. Psychosoc. Interv. 2025, 34, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Tan, P.Z.; Oppenheimer, C.W.; Ladouceur, C.D.; Butterfield, R.D.; Silk, J.S. A review of associations between parental emotion socialization behaviors and the neural substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in youth. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 516–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Callaghan, B.; Tottenham, N. The neuro-environmental loop of plasticity: A cross-species analysis of parental effects on emotion circuitry development following typical and adverse caregiving. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016, 41, 163–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Martin, R.E.; Ochsner, K.N. The neuroscience of emotion regulation development: Implications for education. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 142–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of children and parents.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics of children and parents.
CharacteristicCategoryNPercentage (%)
Child GenderBoys5455.7%
 Girls4344.3%
Nationality/EthnicityGreek100100.0%
School GradeKindergarten (5 years)22.0%
 Grade 1 (6 years)22.0%
 Grade 2 (7 years)88.0%
 Grade 3 (8 years)1111.0%
 Grade 4 (9 years)2323.0%
 Grade 5 (10 years)2323.0%
 Grade 6 (11 years)3030.0%
Parent GenderMale1717.9%
 Female7882.1%
Child Age (years)Mean (SD)10.0 (1.7) 
Table 2. Descriptive summary of child behavior problems and parental responses.
Table 2. Descriptive summary of child behavior problems and parental responses.
Scale/DimensionMinMaxMean (SD)Median (Interquartile Range)
CBCL Scales    
Anxiety/Depression0.0012.003.05 (2.95)2 (1–5)
Withdrawn/Depressed0.009.001.45 (1.95)1 (0–2)
Somatic Complaints0.009.000.95 (1.72)0 (0–1)
Internalizing Problems0.0025.005.33 (5.35)3 (1–8)
Social Problems0.0012.001.84 (2.27)1 (0–3)
Thought Problems0.008.001.18 (1.61)1 (0–2)
Attention Problems0.0013.002.49 (2.76)2 (1–3)
Rule-Breaking Behavior0.008.001.12 (1.49)1 (0–2)
Aggressive Behavior0.0012.003.27 (3.08)3 (1–5)
Externalizing Problems0.0016.004.05 (3.72)3 (1–6)
Other Problems0.008.002.22 (2.08)2 (0–3)
Total Problems0.0056.0022.55 (4.29)22 (20–25)
Activities2.7014.008.61 (2.60)8.7 (6.7–10.5)
Social Competence3.3012.508.02 (1.94)8 (6.5–9.3)
School Competence2.006.205.41 (0.78)5.8 (5–6)
CCNES Dimensions    
Distress/Anxiety Response1.505.082.93 (0.68)2.92 (2.5–3.42)
Punitive Response1.003.922.04 (0.63)1.92 (1.67–2.33)
Expressive Encouragement Response1.007.005.05 (1.33)5.33 (4.58–5.83)
Emotion-Focused Response1.007.005.53 (1.18)5.75 (5.25–6.29)
Problem-Focused Response3.177.005.90 (0.82)5.96 (5.5–6.5)
Minimization/Devaluation Response1.006.083.07 (1.15)2.96 (2.21–3.67)
Table 3. Associations between children’s behavior problems and parental emotion socialization.
Table 3. Associations between children’s behavior problems and parental emotion socialization.
CBCL/CCNES ScaleDistress/AnxietyPunitiveExpressive EncouragementEmotion-FocusedProblem-FocusedMinimization/Devaluation
Anxiety/Depression0.22 *0.11−0.090.030.070.32 **
Withdrawn/Depressed0.110.14−0.060.28 *0.23 *0.27 *
Somatic Complaints0.200.050.02−0.08−0.060.30 **
Internalizing Problems0.25 *0.15−0.070.050.060.34 **
Social Problems0.25 *0.24−0.130.010.090.32 **
Thought Problems0.160.16−0.050.120.090.21
Attention Problems0.150.020.030.160.050.06
Rule-Breaking Behavior0.110.19−0.150.020.020.08
Aggressive Behavior0.080.37 **−0.090.090.070.28 **
Externalizing Problems0.060.34 **−0.190.050.050.27 *
Other Problems0.190.21−0.080.040.040.20
Total Problems0.250.29 **−0.090.110.120.36 **
Activities−0.08−0.120.26 *−0.060.11−0.16
Social Competence−0.030.010.200.140.200.02
School Competence−0.05−0.060.100.120.00−0.02
Overall Competence−0.11−0.090.27 *0.040.25 *−0.13
Note: Spearman’s rho correlations were computed to assess the associations between CBCL and CCNES dimensions. Significance levels are indicated as * p < 0.05 and ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Regression analysis of child and parent demographics on parental emotion socialization responses.
Table 4. Regression analysis of child and parent demographics on parental emotion socialization responses.
Dependent VariablePredictorβSEStandardized Betatp
Distress/Anxiety ResponseChild Gender (Girls vs. Boys)0.0190.0240.0910.800.426
 Child Age0.0000.007−0.005−0.0430.966
 Parent Gender (Females vs. Males)0.0230.0320.0820.7110.479
Punitive ResponseChild Gender−0.0450.028−0.176−1.6230.109
 Child Age0.0030.0080.0410.3730.710
 Parent Gender0.0670.0380.1981.7940.077
Expressive Encouragement ResponseChild Gender0.0360.0370.1090.9680.336
 Child Age0.0030.0110.0330.2910.772
 Parent Gender−0.0230.049−0.053−0.4610.646
Emotion-Focused ResponseChild Gender0.0380.0310.1341.2130.229
 Child Age0.0100.0090.1241.1000.275
 Parent Gender0.0530.0420.1431.2660.209
Problem-Focused ResponseChild Gender0.0180.0160.1271.1470.255
 Child Age0.0040.0050.0840.7400.462
 Parent Gender0.0200.0210.1070.9420.349
Minimization/Devaluation ResponseChild Gender−0.0270.035−0.084−0.7920.431
 Child Age0.0090.0110.0930.8550.395
 Parent Gender0.1320.0460.3082.8500.006
Note: Regression analyses utilized logarithmic transformations of the dependent variables.
Table 5. Regression models predicting child behavioral problems from parental emotion socialization and demographic factors.
Table 5. Regression models predicting child behavioral problems from parental emotion socialization and demographic factors.
Dependent VariablePredictorβSEStandardized Betatp
Internalizing ProblemsChild Gender (Girls vs. Boys)0.1490.0870.1881.7120.091
 Child Age0.0230.0260.0980.9040.369
 Parent Gender (Females vs. Males)−0.0610.119−0.060−0.5190.606
 Distress/Anxiety Response0.0930.0660.1611.4140.162
 Punitive Response−0.0260.080−0.042−0.3270.745
 Expressive Encouragement Response0.0050.0410.0170.1280.899
 Emotion-Focused Response−0.0250.051−0.076−0.4970.621
 Problem-Focused Response0.0430.0760.0910.5670.573
 Minimization/Devaluation Response0.1350.0510.3962.6770.009
Externalizing ProblemsChild Gender (Girls vs. Boys)−0.1330.074−0.192−1.8030.076
 Child Age−0.0260.022−0.126−1.2010.234
 Parent Gender (Females vs. Males)0.0530.1010.0590.5270.600
 Distress/Anxiety Response−0.0030.056−0.006−0.0540.957
 Punitive Response0.2060.0680.3833.0410.003
 Expressive Encouragement Response−0.0230.043−0.078−0.5460.587
 Emotion-Focused Response0.0390.0430.1350.9110.365
 Problem-Focused Response0.0410.0650.1000.6400.524
 Minimization/Devaluation Response0.0700.0350.2642.0150.048
Total ProblemsChild Gender (Girls vs. Boys)−0.0110.084−0.014−0.1280.899
 Child Age−0.0130.025−0.058−0.5270.600
 Parent Gender (Females vs. Males)0.0030.1140.0030.0270.979
 Distress/Anxiety Response0.0990.0630.1811.5710.121
 Punitive Response0.1860.0630.3042.9340.004
 Expressive Encouragement Response−0.0330.039−0.116−0.8500.398
 Emotion-Focused Response0.0080.0490.0250.1650.869
 Problem-Focused Response0.0810.0730.1801.1070.272
 Minimization/Devaluation Response0.1080.0420.3202.5730.012
Note: For the regression analyses, logarithmic transformations were applied to the dependent variables.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Markoulaki, M.; Dimitrakaki, C.; Naska, A.; Papanikolaou, K.; Giannakopoulos, G. Parental Emotion Socialization and Child Adjustment in Greek Families: Supportive vs. Non-Supportive Parenting. Children 2025, 12, 807. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12070807

AMA Style

Markoulaki M, Dimitrakaki C, Naska A, Papanikolaou K, Giannakopoulos G. Parental Emotion Socialization and Child Adjustment in Greek Families: Supportive vs. Non-Supportive Parenting. Children. 2025; 12(7):807. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12070807

Chicago/Turabian Style

Markoulaki, Maria, Christina Dimitrakaki, Andoniki Naska, Katerina Papanikolaou, and Georgios Giannakopoulos. 2025. "Parental Emotion Socialization and Child Adjustment in Greek Families: Supportive vs. Non-Supportive Parenting" Children 12, no. 7: 807. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12070807

APA Style

Markoulaki, M., Dimitrakaki, C., Naska, A., Papanikolaou, K., & Giannakopoulos, G. (2025). Parental Emotion Socialization and Child Adjustment in Greek Families: Supportive vs. Non-Supportive Parenting. Children, 12(7), 807. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12070807

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop