Harnessing Daily Routines for Early Detection of Cool Executive Delays: Validating the EFORTS Questionnaire
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Demographics
2.2.2. Executive Functions and Occupational Routines Scale (EFORTS) [14]
2.2.3. Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [23] and BRIEF-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P; [18])
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Reestablishing EFORTS Daily Routines Scales: Construct (Structure) Validity
3.2. EFORTS Concurrent Validity with the BRIEF/P
3.3. EFORTS Executive Scales, Internal Reliability and Age-Related Differences: Construct (Structure) Validity
3.4. EFORTS Main Factor Cutoff Scores
4. Discussion
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Barkley, R.A. Executive Functions: What They Are, How They Work, and Why They Evolved; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; pp. 225–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, C.; Sousa-Gomes, V.; Fávero, M.; Oliveira-Lopes, S.; Merendeiro, C.S.; Oliveira, J.; Moreira, D. Assessment of preschool-age executive functions: A systematic review. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2022, 29, 1374–1391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frisch, C.; Rosenblum, S.; Tirosh, E. Parental Occupational Executive Training: Feasibility and Parental Perceptions. OTJR Occup. Ther. J. Res. 2020, 40, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Distefano, R.; Palmer, A.R.; Kalstabakken, A.W.; Hillyer, C.K.; Seiwert, M.J.; Zelazo, P.D.; Carlson, S.M.; Masten, A.S. Predictive Validity of the NIH Toolbox Executive Function Measures with Developmental Extensions from Early Childhood to Third Grade Achievement. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2023, 48, 373–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- López-Vallejo, S.; Burneo-Garcés, C.; Pérez-García, M. Development of working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Cross-sectional analysis by age intervals and gender in Ecuadorian preschoolers. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0299394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frisch, C.; Tirosh, E.; Rosenblum, S. Children with ADHD Symptomatology: Does POET Improve Their Daily Routine Management? Children 2023, 10, 1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berger, I.; Nevo, Y. Early Developmental Cues for Diagnosis of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Young Children. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2011, 17, 170–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palmer, A.R.; Kalstabakken, A.W.; Distefano, R.; Carlson, S.M.; Putnam, S.P.; Masten, A.S. A short executive functioning questionnaire in the context of early childhood screening: Psychometric properties. Child Neuropsychol. 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barkley, R.A. The CDC and behavioral parent training for ADHD. Ment. Health Wkly. 2016, 23, 5–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P.D. Executive Function and Psychopathology: A Neurodevelopmental Perspective. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2020, 16, 431–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bater, L.R.; Jordan, S.S. Child Routines and Self-Regulation Serially Mediate Parenting Practices and Externalizing Problems in Preschool Children. Child Youth Care Forum 2017, 46, 243–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sayal, K. The Role of Parental Burden in Child Mental Health Service Use: Longitudinal Study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 43, 1328–1333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frisch, C.; Rosenblum, S. Reliability and validity of the Executive Function and Occupational Routines Scale (EFORTS). Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 2148–2157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kouhbanani, S.S.; Zarenezhad, S.; Arabi, S.M. COVID-19 Anxiety in Children and Its Relationship with Self-Related Skills of Executive Functions. Ir. J. Psychiatry 2023, 18, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gandotra, A.; Cserjesi, R.; Bizonics, R.; Kotyuk, E. Age differences in executive functions among Hungarian preschoolers. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 18, 695–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García, L.F.; Merchán, A.; Phillips-Silver, J.; González, M.T.D. Neuropsychological Development of Cool and Hot Executive Functions Between 6 and 12 Years of Age: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gioia, G.A.; Espy, K.A.; Isquith, P.K. BRIEF-P: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version; Psychological Assessment Resources (PAR): Odessa, FL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Nilsen, E.S.; Huyder, V.; McAuley, T.; Liebermann, D. Ratings of Everyday Executive Functioning (reef): A parent-report measure of preschoolers’ executive functioning skills. Psychol. Assess. 2017, 29, 50–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorell, L.B.; Lindqvist, S.; Nutley, S.B.; Bohlin, G.; Klingberg, T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shimoni, M.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Tirosh, E. Executive dysfunctions among boys with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Performance-based test and parents report. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 858–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eberhart, J.; Paes, T.M.; Ellefson, M.R.; Marcovitch, S. Executive Functions and Play; Elsevier GmbH: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Kenworthy, L.; Barton, R.M. Profiles of Everyday Executive Function in Acquired and Developmental Disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Seresin, M.R.; Roy, A.; Theveniaut, C.; Le Goff, J.; Chopin, C.; Rouger, V.; Roze, J.-C.; Flamant, C.; Muller, J.-B. Assessing of executive functions in daily life in preterm children aged 3–4 years old from the ‘Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool version’ questionnaire. Front. Pediatr. 2023, 11, 999100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Why Improving and Assessing Executive Functions Early in Life Is Critical. In Executive Function in Preschool-Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment, and Translational Research; Griffin, J.A., McCardle, P., Freund, L.S., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; pp. 11–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selman, S.B.; Dilworth-Bart, J.E. Routines and child development: A systematic review. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 2023, 16, 272–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivella, C.; Ruffini, C.; Bombonato, C.; Capodieci, A.; Frascari, A.; Marzocchi, G.M.; Mingozzi, A.; Pecini, C.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C.; et al. TeleFE: A New Tool for the Tele-Assessment of Executive Functions in Children. Appl. Sci. 2023, 13, 1728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castellanos-Ryan, N.; Parent, S.; Chaput-Langlois, S.; Rioux, C.; Jacques, S.; Simard, C.; Tremblay, R.E.; Séguin, J.R.; Zelazo, P.D. Modelling executive function across early childhood: Longitudinal invariance, development from 3.5 to 7 years and later academic performance. Cogn. Dev. 2023, 68, 101365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Age Group (Years.Months) | F(4,91804) | p | ηp2 | |||
3.0–5.11 (n = 503) | 6.0–7.11 (n = 292) | 8.0–11.11 (n = 176) | |||||
M (SD) | |||||||
Mother’s education (years) | 16.62 (2.10) | 16.76 (2.33) | 16.76 (2.33) | 1.93 | 0.145 | 0.004 | |
Father’s education (years) | 15.93 (2.98) | 16.11 (2.74) | 16.11 (2.74) | 0.37 | 0.694 | 0.001 | |
n (%) | |||||||
Gender | Boys | 231 (45.9) | 152 (52.1) | 86 (48.9) | 2.81 | 0.196 | 0.004 |
Girls | 272 (54.1) | 140 (47.9) | 90 (51.1) | ||||
Socio-economic status | Low | 17 (3.4) | 10 (3.4) | 4 (2.3) | 3.87 | 0.056 | 0.006 |
Mid | 201 (40.0) | 115 (39.4) | 58 (33.0) | ||||
High | 285 (56.7) | 167 (57.2) | 114 (64.8) |
BRIEF/P Subscale | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
EFORTS Routine | Inhibition | Emotional Control | Working Memory | Plan/Organization | GEC |
Morning/evening | −0.27 *** | −0.24 *** | −0.49 *** | −0.48 *** | −0.42 *** |
Play/leisure | −0.39 *** | −0.24 *** | −0.49 *** | −0.39 *** | −0.44 *** |
Social | −0.43 *** | −0.34 *** | −0.35 *** | −0.32 *** | −0.42 *** |
Total | −0.44 *** | −0.34 *** | −0.54 *** | −0.48 *** | −0.52 *** |
Routine | Subscale n (Item Number) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Inhibition | Working Memory | Flexibility | |
Morning/evening | 4 (6, 8, 14, 16) | 9 (1–4, 7, 9–12) | 3 (5, 13, 15) |
Play/leisure | 1 (23) | 3 (18–20) | 3 (17, 21, 22) |
Social | 3 (26, 29, 30) | 2 (25, 27) | 2 (24, 28) |
α | 0.88 | 0.9 | 0.79 |
Subscale | Age Group (Years.Month) | Age Affect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
3.0–5.11 (n = 503) | 6.0–7.11 (n = 291) | 8.0–11.11 (n = 176) | F(2,966) | ηp2 | |
M (SD) | |||||
Inhibition | 3.41 (0.60) | 3.61 (0.56) *** a | 3.76 (0.61) *** b * c | 26.21 *** | 0.05 |
Working memory | 3.82 (0.55) | 4.02 (0.50) * a | 4.17 (0.53) *** b *** c | 31.14 *** | 0.06 |
Flexibility | 3.66 (0.62) | 3.84 (0.57) *** a | 4.00 (0.62) *** b * c | 22.81 *** | 0.04 |
Routine | Age Group (Years.Months) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
3.0–5.11 (n = 503) | 6.0–7.11 (n = 292) | 8.0–11.11 (n = 176) | ||||
M (SD) | Cutoff | M (SD) | Cutoff | M (SD) | Cutoff | |
Morning/evening | 3.44 (0.66) | 2.45 | 3.67 (0.62) | 2.74 | 3.80 (0.69) | 2.76 |
Play/leisure | 3.97 (0.55) | 3.14 | 4.17 (0.50) | 3.42 | 4.33 (0.52) | 3.55 |
Social | 3.61 (0.60) | 2.71 | 3.78 (0.62) | 2.85 | 3.94 (0.60) | 3.04 |
Final | 3.67 (0.50) | 2.92 | 3.87 (0.47) | 3.16 | 4.03 (0.50) | 3.28 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Frisch, C.; Rosenblum, S. Harnessing Daily Routines for Early Detection of Cool Executive Delays: Validating the EFORTS Questionnaire. Children 2024, 11, 1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11111281
Frisch C, Rosenblum S. Harnessing Daily Routines for Early Detection of Cool Executive Delays: Validating the EFORTS Questionnaire. Children. 2024; 11(11):1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11111281
Chicago/Turabian StyleFrisch, Carmit, and Sara Rosenblum. 2024. "Harnessing Daily Routines for Early Detection of Cool Executive Delays: Validating the EFORTS Questionnaire" Children 11, no. 11: 1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11111281
APA StyleFrisch, C., & Rosenblum, S. (2024). Harnessing Daily Routines for Early Detection of Cool Executive Delays: Validating the EFORTS Questionnaire. Children, 11(11), 1281. https://doi.org/10.3390/children11111281