Cognitive Control among Primary- and Middle-School Students and Their Associations with Math Achievement
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- What are the changes in cognitive control abilities with age? We hypothesized that middle-school students would present better conflict resolution (i.e., a smaller congruency effect) in both the NST and the PST than sixth graders.
- Is there a difference between the two types of conflict tasks (i.e., NST vs. PST) and between levels of conflict (i.e., simple vs. complex), and how does this difference change with age? We hypothesized that PST would be easier than NST, resulting in a smaller congruency effect in PST compared to NST. Moreover, we hypothesized that a simple level would be easier than a complex level, resulting in a smaller congruency effect in location compared to direction, and in physical compared to numerical comparisons. However, the difference between tasks may be reduced with age.
- Most importantly, our main research question aims to investigate the association between cognitive control and math achievement, and whether this association changes when dealing with basic vs. comprehensive math achievements. We hypothesized that the domain-specific cognitive control task (i.e., NST) would predict basic performance of math achievement (i.e., math fluency). In addition, we hypothesized that both the domain-general (i.e., PST) and domain-specific (i.e., NST) cognitive control factors would predict math performance as manifested in the comprehensive math achievement task (i.e., curriculum test).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Tasks
2.3.1. Computerized Tasks
Numerical Stroop Task (NST)
Perceptual Stroop Task (PST)
2.3.2. Math Achievement Measures
Math Fluency
Curriculum-Based Math Test
2.3.3. Non-Verbal Intelligence
2.4. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Cognitive Control by Task Type and Age Group
3.2. Correlations between the Cognitive Control Tasks
3.3. Math Achievement Measures and Their Associations with Cognitive Control Tasks
3.3.1. Math Fluency Test
3.3.2. Curriculum-Based Math Test
3.3.3. Correlations and Regressions
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- OECD. PISA 2018 Results (Volume III): What School Life Means for Students’ Lives; PISA; OECD: Paris, France, 2019; ISBN 978-92-64-97042-7. [Google Scholar]
- Skagerlund, K.; Forsblad, M.; Slovic, P.; Västfjäll, D. The Affect Heuristic and Risk Perception—Stability Across Elicitation Methods and Individual Cognitive Abilities. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reyna, V.F.; Nelson, W.L.; Han, P.K.; Dieckmann, N.F. How Numeracy Influences Risk Comprehension and Medical Decision Making. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 943–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szűcs, D.; Goswami, U. Developmental Dyscalculia: Fresh Perspectives. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2013, 2, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gliksman, Y.; Berebbi, S.; Hershman, R.; Henik, A. BGU-MF: Ben-Gurion University Math Fluency Test. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2022, 36, 293–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Price, G.R.; Mazzocco, M.M.M.; Ansari, D. Why Mental Arithmetic Counts: Brain Activation during Single Digit Arithmetic Predicts High School Math Scores. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vanbinst, K.; De Smedt, B. Individual Differences in Children’s Mathematics Achievement. In Progress in Brain Research; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2016; Volume 227, pp. 105–130. ISBN 978-0-444-63698-0. [Google Scholar]
- Kucian, K.; Von Aster, M. Developmental Dyscalculia. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2015, 174, 337–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gliksman, Y.; Berebbi, S.; Henik, A. Math Fluency during Primary School. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gunderson, E.A.; Park, D.; Maloney, E.A.; Beilock, S.L.; Levine, S.C. Reciprocal Relations among Motivational Frameworks, Math Anxiety, and Math Achievement in Early Elementary School. J. Cogn. Dev. 2018, 19, 21–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyons, I.M.; Price, G.R.; Vaessen, A.; Blomert, L.; Ansari, D. Numerical Predictors of Arithmetic Success in Grades 1–6. Dev. Sci. 2014, 17, 714–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Halberda, J.; Feigenson, L. Developmental Change in the Acuity of the “Number Sense”: The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-Year-Olds and Adults. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1457–1465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Libertus, M.E.; Feigenson, L.; Halberda, J. Is Approximate Number Precision a Stable Predictor of Math Ability? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 25, 126–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holloway, I.D.; Ansari, D. Domain-specific and Domain-general Changes in Children’s Development of Number Comparison. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, 644–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, R.S.; Ramani, G.B. Playing Linear Number Board Games—But Not Circular Ones—Improves Low-Income Preschoolers’ Numerical Understanding. J. Educ. Psychol. 2009, 101, 545–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sasanguie, D.; De Smedt, B.; Defever, E.; Reynvoet, B. Association between Basic Numerical Abilities and Mathematics Achievement. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 30, 344–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gliksman, Y.; Henik, A. Enumeration and Alertness in Developmental Dyscalculia. J. Cogn. 2019, 2, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schleifer, P.; Landerl, K. Subitizing and Counting in Typical and Atypical Development. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 280–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heine, A.; Tamm, S.; De Smedt, B.; Schneider, M.; Thaler, V.; Torbeyns, J.; Stern, E.; Verschaffel, L.; Jacobs, A. The Numerical Stroop Effect in Primary School Children: A Comparison of Low, Normal, and High Achievers. Child Neuropsychol. 2010, 16, 461–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rubinsten, O.; Henik, A. Automatic Activation of Internal Magnitudes: A Study of Developmental Dyscalculia. Neuropsychology 2005, 19, 641–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allen, K.; Higgins, S.; Adams, J. The Relationship between Visuospatial Working Memory and Mathematical Performance in School-Aged Children: A Systematic Review. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 509–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friso-van Den Bos, I.; Van Der Ven, S.H.G.; Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van Luit, J.E.H. Working Memory and Mathematics in Primary School Children: A Meta-Analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 2013, 10, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silverman, S.; Ashkenazi, S. The Unique Role of Spatial Working Memory for Mathematics Performance. J. Numer. Cogn. 2022, 8, 226–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ashkenazi, S.; Rosenberg-Lee, M.; Metcalfe, A.W.S.; Swigart, A.G.; Menon, V. Visuo–Spatial Working Memory Is an Important Source of Domain-General Vulnerability in the Development of Arithmetic Cognition. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 2305–2317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cragg, L.; Gilmore, C. Skills Underlying Mathematics: The Role of Executive Function in the Development of Mathematics Proficiency. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2014, 3, 63–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mulder, H.; Verhagen, J.; Van Der Ven, S.H.G.; Slot, P.L.; Leseman, P.P.M. Early Executive Function at Age Two Predicts Emergent Mathematics and Literacy at Age Five. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Purpura, D.J.; Schmitt, S.A.; Ganley, C.M. Foundations of Mathematics and Literacy: The Role of Executive Functioning Components. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 153, 15–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, P. Assessment and Development of Executive Function (EF) During Childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Guy, S.C.; Kenworthy, L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000, 6, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crone, E.A.; Steinbeis, N. Neural Perspectives on Cognitive Control Development during Childhood and Adolescence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2017, 21, 205–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crone, E.A. Executive Functions in Adolescence: Inferences from Brain and Behavior. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 825–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spiegel, J.A.; Goodrich, J.M.; Morris, B.M.; Osborne, C.M.; Lonigan, C.J. Relations between Executive Functions and Academic Outcomes in Elementary School Children: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 147, 329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biederman, J.; Monuteaux, M.C.; Doyle, A.E.; Seidman, L.J.; Wilens, T.E.; Ferrero, F.; Morgan, C.L.; Faraone, S.V. Impact of Executive Function Deficits and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) on Academic Outcomes in Children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2004, 72, 757–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bull, R.; Espy, K.A.; Wiebe, S.A. Short-Term Memory, Working Memory, and Executive Functioning in Preschoolers: Longitudinal Predictors of Mathematical Achievement at Age 7 Years. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2008, 33, 205–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diamantopoulou, S.; Rydell, A.-M.; Thorell, L.B.; Bohlin, G. Impact of Executive Functioning and Symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder on Children’s Peer Relations and School Performance. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2007, 32, 521–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laski, E.V.; Dulaney, A. When Prior Knowledge Interferes, Inhibitory Control Matters for Learning: The Case of Numerical Magnitude Representations. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 1035–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szũcs, D.; Soltész, F.; Jármi, É.; Csépe, V. The Speed of Magnitude Processing and Executive Functions in Controlled and Automatic Number Comparison in Children: An Electro-Encephalography Study. Behav. Brain Funct. 2007, 3, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soltész, F.; Goswami, U.; White, S.; Szűcs, D. Executive Function Effects and Numerical Development in Children: Behavioural and ERP Evidence from a Numerical Stroop Paradigm. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2011, 21, 662–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P.D.; Carlson, S.M. The Neurodevelopment of Executive Function Skills: Implications for Academic Achievement Gaps. Psychol. Neurosci. 2020, 13, 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubinsten, O.; Henik, A.; Berger, A.; Shahar-Shalev, S. The Development of Internal Representations of Magnitude and Their Association with Arabic Numerals. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2002, 81, 74–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsal, Y.; Shalev, L.; Mevorach, C. The Diversity of Attention Deficits in ADHD: The Prevalence of Four Cognitive Factors in ADHD Versus Controls. J. Learn. Disabil. 2005, 38, 142–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henik, A.; Tzelgov, J. Is Three Greater than Five: The Relation between Physical and Semantic Size in Comparison Tasks. Mem. Cognit. 1982, 10, 389–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Girelli, L.; Lucangeli, D.; Butterworth, B. The Development of Automaticity in Accessing Number Magnitude. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2000, 76, 104–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gliksman, Y.; Naparstek, S.; Ifergane, G.; Henik, A. Visual and Imagery Magnitude Comparisons Are Affected Following Left Parietal Lesion. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ashkenazi, S.; Henik, A.; Ifergane, G.; Shelef, I. Basic Numerical Processing in Left Intraparietal Sulcus (IPS) Acalculia. Cortex 2008, 44, 439–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shalev, L.; Kolodny, T.; Shalev, N.; Mevorach, C. Attention functioning among adolescents with multiple learning, attentional, behavioral, and emotional difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. 2016, 49, 582–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campbell, J.I.D.; Xue, Q. Cognitive Arithmetic across Cultures. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2001, 130, 299–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilmore, C.K.; McCarthy, S.E.; Spelke, E.S. Non-Symbolic Arithmetic Abilities and Mathematics Achievement in the First Year of Formal Schooling. Cognition 2010, 115, 394–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goffin, C.; Ansari, D. Beyond Magnitude: Judging Ordinality of Symbolic Number Is Unrelated to Magnitude Comparison and Independently Relates to Individual Differences in Arithmetic. Cognition 2016, 150, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ansari, D. Longitudinal predictors of the development of number skills, arithmetic and other aspects of mathematics—Have we made progress? In Proceedings of the MCLS (Mathematical Cognition Learning Society) Annual Meeting, Antwerp, Belgium, 1–3 June 2022. [Google Scholar]
- McGrew, K.S.; Schrank, F.A.; Woodcock, R.W. Technical Manual, Woodcock-Johnson III Normative Update; Riverside Publishing: Meadows, IL, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Belacchi, C.; Scalisi, T.G.; Cannoni, E.; Cornoldi, C. Matrici Progressive di Raven Forma Colore (CPM-47). In Manuale d’uso e Standardizzazione Italiana; Special Organizations: Florence, Italy, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, J.; Court, J.H. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales; Assess Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Bruyer, R.; Brysbaert, M. Combining Speed and Accuracy in Cognitive Psychology: Is the Inverse Efficiency Score (IES) a Better Dependent Variable than the Mean Reaction Time (RT) and the Percentage Of Errors (PE)? Psychol. Belg. 2011, 51, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldfarb, L.; Henik, A. Evidence for Task Conflict in the Stroop Effect. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2007, 33, 1170–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions: Individual Differences as a Window on Cognitive Structure. Cortex 2017, 86, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognit. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Segal, D.; Mashal, N.; Shalev, L. Semantic Conflicts Are Resolved Differently by Adults with and without ADHD. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 47, 416–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bull, R.; Lee, K. Executive Functioning and Mathematics Achievement. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014, 8, 36–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubin, A.; Vidal, J.; Lanoë, C.; Houdé, O.; Borst, G. Inhibitory Control Is Needed for the Resolution of Arithmetic Word Problems: A Developmental Negative Priming Study. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 105, 701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Askenazi, S.; Henik, A. Attentional Networks in Developmental Dyscalculia. Behav. Brain Funct. 2010, 6, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ashkenazi, S.; Adi, A. The Role of Domain General Factors (Spatial, Executive Function, Working Memory, Attention and Fine Motor Skills) in Numerical Processing in Early Childhood. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 23759–23771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofkens, T.L.; Whittaker, J.; Pianta, R.C.; Vitiello, V.; Ruzek, E.; Ansari, A. Pathways of Mathematics Achievement in Preschool: Examining Executive Function and Task Orientation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2022, 81, 101432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, H.J.; Brunsdon, V.E.A.; Bradford, E.E.F. The Developmental Trajectories of Executive Function from Adolescence to Old Age. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. Towards an Anatomical and Functional Model of Number Processing. Math. Cogn. 1995, 1, 83–120. [Google Scholar]
- González-Gómez, B.; Núñez-Peña, M.I.; Colomé, À. Math Anxiety and the Shifting Function: An Event-related Potential Study of Arithmetic Task Switching. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2023, 57, 1848–1869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pellegrino, J.W. Teaching, Learning and Assessing 21st Century Skills; OECD: Paris, France, 2017; pp. 223–251. [Google Scholar]
- Kanive, R.; Nelson, P.M.; Burns, M.K.; Ysseldyke, J. Comparison of the Effects of Computer-Based Practice and Conceptual Understanding Interventions on Mathematics Fact Retention and Generalization. J. Educ. Res. 2014, 107, 83–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kucian, K.; Grond, U.; Rotzer, S.; Henzi, B.; Schönmann, C.; Plangger, F.; Gälli, M.; Martin, E.; Von Aster, M. Mental Number Line Training in Children with Developmental Dyscalculia. NeuroImage 2011, 57, 782–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spaniol, M.M.; Shalev, L.; Kossyvaki, L.; Mevorach, C. Attention Training in Autism as a Potential Approach to Improving Academic Performance: A School-Based Pilot Study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018, 48, 592–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gliksman, Y.; Swartz, I.; Naparstek, S. Math fluency, verbal fluency and executive functions in young and old adults. Unpublished work. 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, P.; Kolodny, T.; Tsafrir, S.; Cohen, G.; Shalev, L. Near and Far Transfer Effects of Computerized Progressive Attention Training (CPAT) Versus Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) Practice Among Adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2023, 27, 757–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Primary Students | Middle Students | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Math Fluency | Math Curriculum | Math Fluency | Math Curriculum | |
Raven Z-score | 0.22 | 0.49 ** | 0.14 | 0.40 * |
PST | ||||
Location-congruency effect | −0.27 * | −0.27 * | −0.25 * | −0.31 * |
Direction-congruency effect | −0.16 | −0.18 | −0.14 | 0.11 |
NST | ||||
Physical-congruency effect | 0.03 | −0.22 | −0.03 | −0.21 |
Numerical-congruency effect | −0.33 * | −0.10 | −0.25 * | −0.13 |
Math Fluency | Curriculum-Based Test | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | Estimates | CI | p | Estimates | CI | p |
(Intercept) | 62.58 | 60.33–64.83 | <0.001 | −0.00 | 0.17–0.17 | 0.995 |
Location Congruency Effect | −3.21 | −5.51–−0.90 | 0.007 | −0.21 | −0.37 –0.04 | 0.015 |
Numerical Congruency Effect | −3.95 | −6.23–−1.67 | 0.001 | −0.08 | 0.24–0.09 | 0.378 |
Z Raven Score | 1.21 | −1.13–3.54 | 0.307 | 0.40 | 0.23–0.57 | <0.001 |
Observations | 122 | 106 | ||||
R2/R2 adjusted | 0.166/0.144 | 0.249/0.227 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Farhi, M.; Gliksman, Y.; Shalev, L. Cognitive Control among Primary- and Middle-School Students and Their Associations with Math Achievement. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14020159
Farhi M, Gliksman Y, Shalev L. Cognitive Control among Primary- and Middle-School Students and Their Associations with Math Achievement. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(2):159. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14020159
Chicago/Turabian StyleFarhi, Moran, Yarden Gliksman, and Lilach Shalev. 2024. "Cognitive Control among Primary- and Middle-School Students and Their Associations with Math Achievement" Education Sciences 14, no. 2: 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14020159
APA StyleFarhi, M., Gliksman, Y., & Shalev, L. (2024). Cognitive Control among Primary- and Middle-School Students and Their Associations with Math Achievement. Education Sciences, 14(2), 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14020159