Deal Me in: Playing Cards in the Home to Learn Math
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Home Mathematics Environment and Children’s Achievement
1.2. Variability within Low-SES Households
1.3. Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Child Assessments
2.2.2. Math Card Games
2.2.3. Caregiver-Child Interactions
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses
3.2. Did Children Learn from the Intervention? (RQ1)
3.3. How Do Parents and Children Use Math Talk during Gameplay? (RQ2)
3.4. Does Caregiver and Child Math Talk Relate to Learning from the Games? (RQ3)
4. Discussion
4.1. Learning from Playing Card Games at Home
4.2. Variability in Mathematical Talk during Gameplay
4.3. Relations between Mathematical Talk and Learning Shape Names
4.4. Remaining Questions and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Duncan, G.J.; Dowsett, C.J.; Claessens, A.; Magnuson, K.; Huston, A.C.; Klebanov, P.; Pagani, L.S.; Feinstein, L.; Engel, M.; Brooks-Gunn, J.; et al. School Readiness and Later Achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1428–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hinojosa, T.; Rapaport, A.; Jaciw, A.; LiCalsi, C.; Zacamy, J. Exploring the Foundations of the Future STEM Workforce: K-12 Indicators of Postsecondary STEM Success. (REL 2016-122); Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Regional Educational Laboratory Southwest: Washington, DC, USA, 2016. Available online: http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/edlabs (accessed on 30 November 2021).
- Eason, S.H.; Scalise, N.R.; Berkowitz, T.; Ramani, G.B.; Levine, S.C. Reviewing the Family Math Literature: Recommendations for Practice, Policy, and Research. 2020. Available online: https://education-first.com/wp-content/uploads/2020/06/FamilyMathReview_WhitePaper.pdf (accessed on 1 September 2021).
- Jordan, N.C.; Levine, S.C. Socioeconomic variation, number competence, and mathematics learning difficulties in young children. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2009, 15, 60–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Starkey, P.; Klein, A. Sociocultural influences on young children’s mathematical knowledge. In Contemporary Perspectives on Mathematics in Early Childhood Education; Saracho, O., Spodek, B., Eds.; Information Age Publishing: Charlotte, NC, USA, 2008; pp. 253–276. [Google Scholar]
- Verdine, B.N.; Irwin, C.M.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. Contributions of executive function and spatial skills to preschool mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 126, 37–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Daucourt, M.C.; Napoli, A.R.; Quinn, J.M.; Wood, S.G.; Hart, S.A. The home math environment and math achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 147, 565–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliott, L.; Bachman, H.J. How Do Parents Foster Young Children’s Math Skills? Child Dev. Perspect. 2018, 12, 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mutaf-Yildiz, B.; Sasanguie, D.; De Smedt, B.; Reynvoet, B. Probing the Relationship Between Home Numeracy and Children’s Mathematical Skills: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hornburg, C.B.; Borriello, G.A.; Kung, M.; Lin, J.; Litkowski, E.; Cosso, J.; Ellis, A.; King, Y.; Zippert, E.; Cabrera, N.J.; et al. Next directions in measurement of the home mathematics environment: An international and interdisciplinary perspective. J. Numer. Cogn. 2021, 7, 195–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benavides-Varela, S.; Butterworth, B.; Francesca, A.G.; Lucangeli, D.; Semenza, C. Numerical activities and information learned at home link to the exact numeracy skills in 5–6 years-old children. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mutaf-Yildiz, B.; Sasanguie, D.; De Smedt, B.; Reynvoet, B. Frequency of Home Numeracy Activities Is Differentially Related to Basic Number Processing and Calculation Skills in Kindergarteners. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Niklas, F.; Schneider, W. Casting the die before the die is cast: The importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool children. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2014, 29, 327–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung, S.K.; McBride, C. Effectiveness of parent-child number board game playing in promoting Chinese kindergarteners’ numeracy skills and mathematics interest. Early Educ. Dev. 2017, 28, 572–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sonnenschein, S.; Metzger, S.R.; Thompson, J.A. Low-income parents’ socialization of their preschoolers’ early reading and math skills. Res. Hum. Dev. 2016, 13, 207–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramani, G.B.; Rowe, M.L.; Eason, S.H.; Leech, K.A. Math talk during parent-child interactions in Head Start families. Cog. Dev. 2015, 35, 15–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramani, G.B.; Scalise, N.R. It’s more than just fun and games: Play-based mathematics activities for Head Start families. Ear. Child. Res. Quart. 2020, 50, 78–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandermaas-Peeler, M.; Ferretti, L.; Loving, S. Playing The Ladybug Game: Parent guidance of young children’s numeracy activities. Early Child Dev. Care 2011, 182, 1289–1307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zippert, E.L.; Daubert, E.N.; Scalise, N.R.; Noreen, G.D.; Ramani, G.B. “Tap space number three”: Promoting math talk during parent-child tablet play. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 55, 1605–1614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clements, D.H.; Sarama, J. Effects of a preschool mathematics curriculum: Summative research on the Building Blocks Project. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2007, 38, 136–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, R.S. Improving the Numerical Understanding of Children From Low-Income Families. Child Dev. Perspect. 2009, 3, 118–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scalise, N.R.; Daubert, E.N.; Ramani, G.B. When one size does not fit all: A latent profile analysis of low-income preschoolers’ math skills. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 209, 105156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Q.; Lei, P.-W.; DiPerna, J.; Morgan, P.L.; Reid, E.E. Identifying Differences in Early Mathematical Skills Among Children in Head Start. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2014, 13, 1403–1423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Son, S.-H.C.; Hur, J.H. Parental Math Talk During Home Cooking and Math Skills in Head Start Children: The Role of Task Management Talk. J. Res. Child. Educ. 2020, 34, 406–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Susperreguy, M.I.; Lira, C.J.; Xu, C.; LeFevre, J.-A.; Vega, H.B.; Pando, E.V.B.; Contreras, M.O. Home Learning Environments of Children in Mexico in Relation to Socioeconomic Status. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramani, G.B.; Siegler, R.S. Promoting Broad and Stable Improvements in Low-Income Children’s Numerical Knowledge Through Playing Number Board Games. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 375–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scalise, N.R.; Daubert, E.D.; Ramani, G.B. Narrowing the early mathematics gap: A play-based intervention to promote low-income preschoolers’ number skills. J. Num. Cog. 2017, 3, 655–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scalise, N.R.; Daubert, E.N.; Ramani, G.B. Benefits of Playing Numerical Card Games on Head Start Children’s Mathematical Skills. J. Exp. Educ. 2020, 88, 200–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zelazo, P.D. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 2006, 1, 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casey, B.M.; Lombardi, C.M.; Thomson, D.; Nguyen, H.N.; Paz, M.; Theriault, C.A.; Dearing, E. Maternal Support of Children’s Early Numerical Concept Learning Predicts Preschool and First-Grade Math Achievement. Child Dev. 2016, 89, 156–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliott, L.; Braham, E.J.; Libertus, M.E. Understanding sources of individual variability in parents’ number talk with young children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 159, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunderson, E.A.; Levine, S.C. Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 1021–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wynn, K. Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition 1990, 36, 155–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacWhinney, B. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk, 3rd ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). Principles and Standards for School Mathematics; NCTM: Reston, VA, USA, 2000; Available online: https://www.nctm.org/Standards-and-Positions/Principles-and-Standards/Geometry/ (accessed on 30 November 2021).
- Honoré, N.; Noel, M.-P. Improving Preschoolers’ Arithmetic through Number Magnitude Training: The Impact of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Training. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0166685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laski, E.; Siegler, R.S. Learning from number board games: You learn what you encode. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 853–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whyte, J.C.; Bull, R. Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 588–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Smedt, B.; Noël, M.-P.; Gilmore, C.; Ansari, D. How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children’s mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2013, 2, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fazio, L.; Bailey, D.H.; Thompson, C.; Siegler, R.S. Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 123, 53–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mussolin, C.; Nys, J.; Leybaert, J.; Content, A. How approximate and exact number skills are related to each other across development: A review. Dev. Rev. 2016, 39, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scalise, N.R.; Ramani, G.B. Symbolic Magnitude Understanding Predicts Preschoolers’ Later Addition Skills. J. Cogn. Dev. 2021, 22, 185–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, M.D.; Beeres, K.; Coban, L.; Merz, S.; Schmidt, S.S.; Stricker, J.; De Smedt, B. Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, M.; Merz, S.; Stricker, J.; De Smedt, B.; Torbeyns, J.; Verschaffel, L.; Luwel, K. Associations of Number Line Estimation With Mathematical Competence: A Meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1467–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, R.S. Magnitude knowledge: The common core of numerical development. Dev. Sci. 2016, 19, 341–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Silver, A.M.; Elliott, L.; Libertus, M.E. Parental math input is not uniformly beneficial for young children: The moderating role of inhibitory control. J. Educ. Psychol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Napoli, A.R.; Purpura, D.J. The home literacy and numeracy environment in preschool: Cross-domain relations of parent–child practices and child outcomes. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 166, 581–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Skwarchuk, S.-L. How Do Parents Support Preschoolers’ Numeracy Learning Experiences at Home? Day Care Early Educ. 2009, 37, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Susperreguy, M.I.; Davis-Kean, P.E. Maternal Math Talk in the Home and Math Skills in Preschool Children. Early Educ. Dev. 2016, 27, 841–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandermaas-Peeler, M.; Boomgarden, E.; Finn, L.; Pittard, C. Parental support of numeracy during a cooking activity with four-year-olds. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2012, 20, 78–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saxe, G.B.; Guberman, S.R.; Gearhart, M.; Gelman, R.; Massey, C.M.; Rogoff, B. Social Processes in Early Number Development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1987, 52, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bjorklund, D.F.; Hubertz, M.J.; Reubens, A.C. Young children’s arithmetic strategies in social context: How parents contribute to children’s strategy development while playing games. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2004, 28, 347–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levine, S.C.; Suriyakham, L.W.; Rowe, M.L.; Huttenlocher, J.; Gunderson, E.A. What counts in the development of young children’s number knowledge? Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 1309–1319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Susperreguy, M.I. Math talk between children and mothers and its connection to math-related practices in the home setting. In Socializing Children through Language; Davis-Kean, P.E., Tang, S., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 81–109. [Google Scholar]
- Hendrix, N.M.; Hojnoski, R.L.; Missall, K.N. Shared Book Reading to Promote Math Talk in Parent–Child Dyads in Low-Income Families. Top. Early Child. Spéc. Educ. 2019, 39, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuchirko, Y.A.; Coskun, L.Z.; Duch, H.; Castaner, M.M.; Gennetian, L.A. Light-Touch Design Enhancements Can Boost Parent Engagement in Math Activities. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2021, 128, 106133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung, S.K.; McBride-Chang, C. Evaluation of a Parent Training Program for Promoting Filipino Young Children’s Number Sense with Number Card Games. Child Stud. Asia-Pac. Context 2015, 5, 39–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berkowitz, T.; Schaeffer, M.W.; Maloney, E.A.; Peterson, L.; Gregor, C.; Levine, S.C.; Beilock, S.L. Math at home adds up to achievement in school. Science 2015, 350, 196–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- High, P.C.; LaGasse, L.; Becker, S.; Ahlgren, I.; Gardner, A. Literacy Promotion in Primary Care Pediatrics: Can We Make a Difference? Pediatrics 2000, 105, 927–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bailey, D.; Duncan, G.J.; Odgers, C.; Yu, W. Persistence and Fadeout in the Impacts of Child and Adolescent Interventions. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2017, 10, 7–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Arnold, D.H.; Fisher, P.H.; Doctoroff, G.L.; Dobbs, J. Accelerating math development in Head Start classrooms. J. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 94, 762–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klibanoff, R.S.; Levine, S.C.; Huttenlocher, J.; Vasilyeva, M.; Hedges, L.V. Preschool children’s mathematical knowledge: The effect of teacher “math talk”. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 42, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Completed Consent (n = 86) | Received Materials (n = 70) | Completed Study (n = 50) | |
---|---|---|---|
Child age (in months) | 60.5 (4.7) 1 | 60.4 (4.9) | 60.1 (4.4) |
% Female | 47 | 49 | 56 |
% Hispanic/Latinx | 35 | 37 | 38 |
% African American/Black | 30 | 27 | 26 |
% Caucasian/White | 44 | 51 | 52 |
% Asian/Pacific Islander | 9 | 7 | 6 |
% Biracial/Multiracial | 14 | 13 | 16 |
Code | Definition | Examples |
---|---|---|
Counting and cardinality | Player counts, asks other player to count, discusses counting strategies, or discusses the cardinal values of sets. | One, two, three… (C) Let’s count it! (A) Count the dots. (A) You have seven cards. (A) How many cards do I have? (A) |
Number identification | Player identifies a written numeral or asks other player to identify a numeral. Includes references to the same cards or the same number. | What is your card? (A) What number is this? (A) I have a 5. (C) We have the same number! (A) |
Magnitude comparison | Player compares two quantities, makes a statement about relative quantity, or asks other player to make a comparison of two quantities. This includes statements of non-equivalence and talking about which number is closest to a target number. | Which is more, 3 or 5? (A) Whose card is higher? (A) I have more cards. (C) My card is more! (C) |
Shape identification | Player identifies a shape or asks other player to identify a shape (i.e., circle, triangle, square, rectangle, pentagon, hexagon, octagon). | What shape is this? (A) Do you have a rectangle? (A) I have a square. (C) |
Color identification | Player identifies a color or asks other player to identify a color. | What color is this? (A) My card is red. (C) Do you have green? (A) |
Matching features | Player identifies or asks other player to compare features of the cards, such as if both cards have the same color or shape. Focus on comparing features or the logic of finding a matching feature. | Is it the same shape? (A) Is it the same color? (A) I don’t have any rectangles. (C) You could play a red one. (A) Do you have any green cards? (A) |
Magnitude Comparison | Shape and Color | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Pretest M (SD) | Posttest M (SD) | Pretest M (SD) | Posttest M (SD) | |
Participants who received materials (n = 70) | ||||
Number knowledge | ||||
Counting | 0.76 (0.32) | 0.72 (0.34) | 0.76 (0.31) | 0.78 (0.32) |
Cardinality | 5.17 (1.52) | 4.52 (1.06) | 4.21 (1.37) | 5.24 (1.42) |
Number identification | 0.81 (0.29) | 0.76 (0.35) | 0.84 (0.25) | 0.86 (0.27) |
Symbolic magnitude | 0.82 (0.21) | 0.78 (0.31) | 0.80 (0.23) | 0.83 (0.21) |
Number line estimation | 0.74 (0.10) | 0.73 (0.09) | 0.75 (0.08) | 0.72 (0.09) |
Shape knowledge | ||||
Shape naming | 0.64 (0.20) | 0.60 (0.29) | 0.70 (0.17) | 0.84 (0.19) |
Shape finding | 0.62 (0.25) | 0.56 (0.29) | 0.61 (0.24) | 0.76 (0.21) |
Sorting by shape and color | 1.78 (0.42) | 1.64 (0.80) | 1.65 (0.54) | 1.82 (0.67) |
Participants who completed the study (n = 50) | ||||
Number knowledge | ||||
Counting | 0.76 (0.31) | 0.78 (0.30) | 0.77 (0.29) | 0.79 (0.31) |
Cardinality | 4.44 (1.08) | 3.62 (0.82) | 3.44 (0.96) | 5.20 (1.53) |
Number identification | 0.81 (0.27) | 0.81 (0.28) | 0.83 (0.25) | 0.86 (0.25) |
Symbolic magnitude | 0.86 (0.17) | 0.83 (0.26) | 0.78 (0.25) | 0.80 (0.23) |
Number line estimation | 0.25 (0.09) | 0.28 (0.09) | 0.25 (0.09) | 0.28 (0.09) |
Shape knowledge | ||||
Shape naming | 0.66 (0.20) | 0.64 (0.26) | 0.71 (0.18) | 0.90 (0.13) |
Shape finding | 0.65 (0.24) | 0.60 (0.29) | 0.64 (0.23) | 0.76 (0.21) |
Sorting by shape and color | 1.84 (0.37) | 1.72 (0.74) | 1.68 (0.56) | 1.84 (0.75) |
Outcome | Pretest Score | Condition 1 | R2 |
---|---|---|---|
Participants who received materials (n = 70) | |||
Counting | 0.76 *** | 0.68 | 0.66 |
Cardinality | 0.66 *** | 0.01 | 0.63 |
Number identification | 0.68 *** | −0.08 | 0.37 |
Symbolic magnitude | 0.66 *** | −0.06 | 0.29 |
Number line estimation | 0.35 ** | −0.01 | 0.13 |
Shape naming | 0.69 *** | −0.21 *** | 0.42 |
Shape finding | 0.52 *** | −0.20 *** | 0.35 |
Sorting shapes and colors | 0.50 ** | −0.25 | 0.12 |
Participants who completed the study (n = 50) | |||
Counting | 0.72 *** | 0.80 | 0.58 |
Cardinality | 0.72 *** | −0.14 | 0.67 |
Number identification | 0.61 *** | −0.04 | 0.36 |
Symbolic magnitude | 0.70 *** | −0.02 | 0.39 |
Number line estimation | 0.40 * | 0.01 | 0.15 |
Shape naming | 0.66 *** | −0.22 *** | 0.55 |
Shape finding | 0.73 *** | −0.16 ** | 0.52 |
Sorting shapes and colors | 0.52 * | −0.20 | 0.12 |
Time 1 M (SD) | Time 2 M (SD) | Correlation T1-T2 | |
---|---|---|---|
Number comparison game: Caregiver talk | |||
Counting/cardinality | 37.9 (34.3) | 25.9 (41.0) | 0.68 *** |
Number identification | 45.2 (33.1) | 42.4 (29.0) | 0.44 * |
Magnitude comparison | 36.5 (25.4) | 28.0 (24.5) | 0.50 * |
Shape identification | 0.1 (0.4) | 0.2 (0.8) | 0.99 *** |
Color identification | 0 (0) | 0.1 (0.2) | --- |
Matching features | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | --- |
Number comparison game: Child talk | |||
Counting/cardinality | 20.8 (19.8) | 16.6 (20.8) | 0.34 |
Number identification | 36.5 (23.2) | 35.0 (19.8) | 0.08 |
Magnitude comparison | 23.0 (15.5) | 22.0 (17.2) | 0.34 |
Shape identification | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | --- |
Color identification | 0.1 (0.4) | 0 (0) | --- |
Matching features | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | --- |
Shape and color matching game: Caregiver talk | |||
Counting/cardinality | 15.5 (11.9) | 16.3 (16.1) | 0.39 |
Number identification | 0 (0) | 0.4 (1.0) | --- |
Magnitude comparison | 0.1 (0.3) | 0.1 (0.2) | −0.08 |
Shape identification | 68.0 (45.2) | 60.6 (46.5) | 0.46 * |
Color identification | 57.4 (39.9) | 53.1 (45.4) | 0.41 * |
Matching features | 25.2 (20.6) | 14.3 (20.7) | 0.74 *** |
Shape and color matching game: Child talk | |||
Counting/cardinality | 9.6 (7.5) | 9.7 (10.1) | 0.54 ** |
Number identification | 0 (0) | 0.5 (1.9) | --- |
Magnitude comparison | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | --- |
Shape identification | 29.4 (20.5) | 38.5 (32.4) | 0.36 |
Color identification | 27.7 (20.7) | 36.5 (33.5) | 0.13 |
Matching features | 9.5 (9.0) | 9.1 (13.0) | 0.48 * |
Posttest Shape Naming B (SE) | Posttest Shape Matching B (SE) | |
---|---|---|
Caregiver shape identification | 0.03 (0.02) | 0.02 (0.03) |
Caregiver color identification | 0.04 (0.02) | 0.02 (0.03) |
Caregiver matching features | 0.05 (0.02) * | 0.002 (0.03) |
Child shape identification | −0.003 (0.02) | −0.002 (0.03) |
Child color identification | 0.003 (0.02) | 0.008 (0.03) |
Child matching features | 0.06 (0.02) ** | 0.05 (0.03) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Scalise, N.R.; DePascale, M.; Tavassolie, N.; McCown, C.; Ramani, G.B. Deal Me in: Playing Cards in the Home to Learn Math. Educ. Sci. 2022, 12, 190. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030190
Scalise NR, DePascale M, Tavassolie N, McCown C, Ramani GB. Deal Me in: Playing Cards in the Home to Learn Math. Education Sciences. 2022; 12(3):190. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030190
Chicago/Turabian StyleScalise, Nicole R., Mary DePascale, Nadia Tavassolie, Claire McCown, and Geetha B. Ramani. 2022. "Deal Me in: Playing Cards in the Home to Learn Math" Education Sciences 12, no. 3: 190. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030190
APA StyleScalise, N. R., DePascale, M., Tavassolie, N., McCown, C., & Ramani, G. B. (2022). Deal Me in: Playing Cards in the Home to Learn Math. Education Sciences, 12(3), 190. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030190