Preschool Children Science Mental Representations: The Sound in Space
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background and Literature Review
2.1. The Issue of Sound Comprehension
2.2. Sound Approach within Early Childhood Science Education
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. The Procedure and the Interview
3.3. Data Analysis
4. Results
4.1. First Research Question
- Sound propagates in space. Here, children seemed to recognize that sound acts independently from both its point of production and the receiver. To quote pupil 6 (P. 6), ‘It’s something that reaches our ears …. it comes from around’ (Researcher. ‘Why do you think this? How do you think it?’), ‘Because we keep hearing it wherever we go …’.
- Sound as a product of human and social activity. Answers in this category were focused on the source of the sound production. For example, P. 17. ‘When we hit something …. or on the roads from the cars …’.
- Sound as a product of vocal expression. This category included answers that recognize an unambiguous relationship between sound and voice. While in some cases other types of sounds were mentioned, the main emphasis on sound production here remains voice. For example, P. 43. ‘If we shout, the sound is generated’, (R. ‘Is there any other way for a sound to be produced?’) ‘With voices … from mouths …’, (R. ‘Only human can produce sound?), ’Yes, humans … and animals that bark let say …’.
- Sound propagates in space. Here, children seemed to attribute to sound the capability of propagation in space. For example, P. 33. ‘The sound that comes out when a hammer strikes’, (R. ‘When you say ‘it comes out’ what do you mean?’), ‘I mean that as the hammer strikes its target, a loud sound generates reaches our ears’.
- Sounds as a product of human and social activity. In these answers, children focused their thinking on the actions that are capable of sound production. For example, P. 2. ‘When two things strike, we hear the blow’, (R. ‘How do you think that? Could you give me an example?’), ‘Yes … when we clap … our hands click …’.
- Sounds attributed exclusively to voices. Here, children seemed to limit their views about sound exclusively on voices. For example, P. 21. ‘The voices of children at school’, (R. ‘Could you give me any other example?’), ‘Yes, voices at home …. or on the road’.
- Music is sound. This category included answers that referred to the existence of music everywhere in space. For example, (R. ‘Do you think that the music we listen is sound?’) P. 64. ‘Yes, it is … but it does not look like other sounds … it is somewhat special …’, (R. ‘So what is special about it?’), ‘It is produced from instruments that play all together … and individually sometimes’.
- Music is associated with sound, in an unstable manner yet. In particular, when children focused their attention on sound perception, they did not seem to distinguish sound from music. However, when they referred to the sources of production, they seemed to recognize sound and music as different entities. For example, P. 60 ‘We hear both (music and sound) … with our ears … music is like sound. That is, it is like the sound we hear … But they are made up differently … ‘, (R. ‘What do you mean they are ‘made up’ differently?’), ‘Let’s say … the music from guitars and the sound from our voices ...’.
- Music and sound totally differ. These answers record a clear distinction between ‘sound’ and ‘music’, i.e., music was understood as an element that was not related to sounds. For example, P. 51 ‘Music is ‘music’, it is not sound’, (R. ‘So, do they differ somewhere? What is different about them?’), ‘Yes, of course. The music is songs …That is, it is not done by … when a voice is heard …’.
4.2. Second Research Question
- Answers in which differences are mainly identified on the different subjective characteristics of sound. Here, children’s responses are mainly related to loudness and timbre while the distinction of sounds seems to be a constant element of their reasoning. For example, P. 46. ‘They are not all the same. Others are strong and conflict pain to our ears, and other… we do not even listen to them …. or let’s say if we are far away we do not hear well when someone speaks’, (R. ‘Could we distinguish people who speak the same loud?’), ‘Do you mean to distinguish who is it? Who is talking?’ (R. ‘Exactly to distinguish who speaks when two people speak the same loud’). ‘Yes, it is quite easy … my mom has a totally different voice from my sister Joana’.
- Answers that recognize limited differences in sounds. This category included children’s responses that pointed out differences between sounds in an instable and/or difficult manner. For example, P. 11. ‘Hmmm … it seems to me that they are not the same … that is … I do not know’, (R. ‘Do you want to explain it further? What do you think about these sounds that are not the same?’), ‘I think they are the same but sometimes they do not look alike … they differ’, (R. ‘Could you just tell me two of these sounds to make it clearer?’), ‘The loud and the quiet sound’.
- Answers in which no criteria were found for sound discrimination. Few children were not able to use criteria for sound discrimination. From the relevant discussions, it seemed that although these children recognized differences between sounds, they were not able to find indicators of sound discrimination. For example, P. 3 ‘They are the same … they do not change …’, (R. ‘I mean if we hear two different sounds are they the same?’), ‘They are the same’, (R. ‘So, does that mean that the voice of your mother and the sound that comes from TV are the same?) ‘No, it’s a totally different thing my mother voice and the sound from TV … nevertheless, the sound does not change’.
- Those that satisfactorily recognized 5–6 sounds;
- Those that satisfactorily recognized 3–4 sounds;
- Those that either satisfactorily recognized 1–2 sounds or do not answer at all.
4.3. Third Research Question
- Answers in which sound is recognized as something that can be produced by any source. This category includes responses that refer to different audio sources, often without any kind of distinction between them. Essentially, the emphasis here is on the ‘sound’ itself. For example, P. 14. ‘Sounds are produce all the time … in many ways … through voices, through objects … the music, the cars …’, (R. ‘How can a sound be produced?’), ‘Hmmm … the objects have to hit each other to get out sound …’ (R. ‘How about cars, how does the sound come out?’), ‘Hmmm … the engine knocks … even from the exhaust … something hits there too … and as the sound comes out we hear it’.
- Answers in which limited natural and artificial sound sources are recognized while the distinction between objects of sound production and sound itself is blurred. Here, we have children’s answers which refer to a limited number of audio sources and face difficulties in identifying different sources. For example, P. 28. ‘Hmmm … people’s voices …’, (R. ‘Any other way?’), ‘animals voices’, (R. ‘except of voices, is there any other way to produce a sound?’), ‘hmmm …. how else?’, (R. ‘I mean only from voices can produced sounds?’), ‘Not only …’, (R. ‘Could you please express your thoughts?’)?, ‘hmmm … let’s say when a car passes …’, (R. ’In any other way?’).
- Answers that focus exclusively on one type of voice source. In this category of responses, children focused on a single-signal type of sound source and mainly on the human voice. For example, P. 21. ‘From the voices we make’, (R. ‘In any other way?’), ‘When we talk … when we cough …’, (R. ‘Do you think of anything else … of any other way that sounds can be produced?’), ‘Oh yes, animals can also produce sounds’, (R. ‘How are they produced?’), ‘…as the bark …!’.
- Answers where children recognize that sound is propagated everywhere in space. This category of responses is quite interesting as it highlights the ability of young children to recognize sound in space. For example, P. 39. ‘Everywhere … all around …’, (R. ‘How do you know that?’), ‘Because, let’s say, in case the bell of Saint Andreas rings, it can be also heard by us that we live by the sea …. All the neighbors can hear it …. It’s like flying everywhere’, and P. 64. ‘Everywhere … when it comes out of the CD let’s say … it goes everywhere. Everywhere … like in concerts that everyone is able to listen to the music, irrespectively of the place it sits …’.
- Answers where sound is closely related to the way that is received by humans. In these responses, the receiver seems to hold a particularly important position, which leads to a kind of decentralization of children’s thinking from the sources of sound production. For example, P. 10. ‘In our ear’, (R. ‘How does this happen?’), ‘It happens… because we hear it…’ and P. 88. ‘Everywhere’, (R. ‘If we produce a sound here on the table, inside the room, will it go somewhere else?’), ‘It will go around here’, (R. ‘Could you show me the exact point it will go?’), ‘Yes … here where we are sitting …’.
- Answers that focus on sound production. In these responses, the main focus stands on sound sources. For example, P. 30. ‘They do not go anywhere …’, (R. ‘Could you explain it further to me?’), ‘They are people’s voices … they do not go somewhere … they just go along with peoples …’.
- Answers where children recognize the propagation of sound throughout space. In this category of responses, all five points were selected without any special distinction. For example, P. 77. ‘In all places … here … there … everywhere’, (R. ‘How do you know that?’), ‘Hmmm … wherever we go we will listen’.
- Answers where sound is linked with an imaginary space defined by the linear source-receiver relationship. In these responses, the presence of sound was recognized in points 2, 4 and 5. For example, P. 10. ‘There … in the ears of the rabbit the song will be heard’ and P. 70 (R. ‘Somewhere else?’), ‘Maybe here too (point 2) … I guess …’ (R. ‘Why? How do you think this?’), ‘Because if rabbit’s ear placed there, I think it will be able to hear …’.
- Answers focused solely on the sound receiver. Some children’s responses seem to focus solely on the sound receiver. For example, P. 52. ‘In rabbit’s place where he is able to hear … here and there (points 4 and 5)’ (R. ‘Could you tell me how you think this?’), ‘Hmmm … it will be where he can hear … that is in his ears’.
5. Discussion
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Order | Number | Task |
---|---|---|
1 | 1.1 | What is sound? |
2 | 1.2 | Could you tell me some kind of sounds that you know? |
3 | 2.1 | Are all sounds the same? If not, where do they differ? |
4 | 3.1 | How are sounds produced, how are they made of? |
5 | 2.2 | Behind you there are some objects made of iron, wood and glass. I am going to hit two of them and I would like you to guess which one I am hitting each time. For example, if I hit these two objects made of plastic, they make that sound. Let’s see if we can recognize the sounds without observing the objects that being hit. |
6 | 3.2. | Where do the sounds go? If we produce a sound here on the table, inside the room, will it go somewhere else? |
7 | 1.3 | Is music a kind of sound? |
8 | 3.3 | Here we can see a mouse who sings and a rabbit who wants to hear her songs. I will show you some points to tell me whether her voice could be heard there (Figure 1). |
References
- Ravanis, K. Early Childhood Science Education: State of the art and perspectives. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 2017, 16, 284–288. [Google Scholar]
- Ravanis, K. The Physical Sciences in Early Childhood Education: Theoretical frameworks, strategies and activities. J. Phys. Confer. Ser. 2021, 1796, 012092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christidou, V.; Dimitriou, A.; Barkas, N.; Papadopoulou, M.; Grammenos, S. Young noise researchers: An intervention to promote noise awareness in preschool children. J. Balt. Sci. Educ. 2015, 14, 569–585. [Google Scholar]
- Kambouri, M. Children’s misconceptions and the teaching of early years Science: A case study. J. Emerg. Sci. 2011, 2, 7–16. [Google Scholar]
- Raviv, A.; Dadon, M. Teaching Astronomy in Kindergarten: Children’s perceptions and projects. Ath. J. Educ. 2021, 8, 305–328. [Google Scholar]
- Pantidos, P. Epistemic, cognitive and semiotic significations in science teaching: The case of sound. Europ. J. Educ. Stud. 2019, 6, 210–231. [Google Scholar]
- Van der Burg, E.; Talsma, D.; Olivers, C.N.; Hickey, C.; Theeuwes, J. Early multisensory interactions affect the competition among multiple visual objects. Neuroimage 2011, 55, 1208–1218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dinh, H.Q.; Walker, N.; Hodges, L.F.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, A. Evaluating the importance of multi-sensory input on memory and the sense of presence in virtual environments. In Proceedings of IEEE Virtual Reality Conference, Houston, TX, USA, 13–17 March 1999; IEEE Ed.: Houston, TX, USA, 1999; pp. 222–228. [Google Scholar]
- Lammert-Siepmann, N.; Bestgen, A.K.; Edler, D.; Kuchinke, L.; Dickmann, F. Audiovisual communication of object-names improves the spatial accuracy of recalled object-locations in topographic maps. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0186065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Driver, R.; Squires, A.; Rushworth, P.; Wood-Robinson, V. Making Sense of Secondary School Science; Routledge: London, UK, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Lautrey, J.; Mazens, K. Is children’s naïve knowledge consistent? A comparison of the concepts of sound and heat. Learn. Instr. 2004, 14, 399–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Asoko, H.M.; Leach, J.; Scott, P.H. Classroom research as a basis for professional development of teachers: A study of students’ understanding of sound. New prospects for teacher education in Europe II. In Proceedings of the 16th Annual Conference of ATEE, Amsterdam, The Neatherlans, 2–4 September 1992; Vonk, J.C., Giesbers, J.G.I., Peeters, J.J., Wubbles, T., Eds.; Universitaire Lerarenopleiding Vrije Universiteit: Amsterdam, The Neatherlans, 1992; pp. 109–119. [Google Scholar]
- Watt, D.; Russel, T. Sound. Primary SPACE Project Research Report; University Press: Liverpool, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Hapkiewicz, A. Naïve ideas in Earth science. MSTA J. 1999, 44, 26–30. [Google Scholar]
- Eshach, H.; Schwartz, L.J. Sound stuff? Naïve materialism in middle-school students’ conceptions of sound. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2006, 28, 733–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reiner, M.; Slotta, J.D.; Chi, M.T.H.; Resnick, L.B. Naïve physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions. Cogn. Instr. 2000, 18, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hrepic, Z.; Zollman, D.; Rebello, N. Identifying students’ mental models of sound propagation: The role of conceptual blending in understanding conceptual change. Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 2010, 6, 020114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krnel, D.; Watson, R.; Glazer, S.A. The development of the concept of “matter”: A cross-age study of how children classify materials. Sci. Educ. 2003, 87, 621–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, K.; Longden, K.; McGuigan, L. SPACE Research Report: Materials; University Press: Liverpool, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Lin, C.H. Application of a model for the integration of technology in kindergarten: An empirical investigation in Taiwan. Ear. Child. Educ. J. 2012, 40, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eshach, H.; Lin, T.C.; Tsai, C.C. Misconception of sound and conceptual change: A cross sectional study on students’ materialistic thinking of sound. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2017, 5, 664–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, E.; Wallin, A. Students’ learning of a generalized theory of sound transmission from a teaching–learning sequence about sound, hearing and health. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2013, 35, 980–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Piaget, J. Causalité et opérations. In Les Explications Causales; Piaget, J., Garcia, R., Eds.; PUF: Paris, France, 1971; pp. 11–140. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, J.; Lannoy, J.D. La Projection de la Lumière et de la Chaleur. In Recherche non Publiée sur la Causalité—Manuscrit Complet; Archives Jean Piaget: Geneva, Switzerland, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Ravanis, K. Représentations des élèves de l’école maternelle: Le concept de lumière. Int. J. Ear. Child. 1999, 31, 48–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ravanis, K.; Christidou, V.; Hatzinikita, V. Enhancing conceptual change in preschool children’s representations of light: A socio-cognitive approach. Res. Sci. Educ. 2013, 43, 2257–2276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J.; Vergopoulo, T. La transmission des vibrations entre deux diapasons. In La Transmission des Mouvements; Piaget, J., Ed.; PUF: Paris, France, 1972; pp. 213–227. [Google Scholar]
- Gallegos-Cázares, L.; Flores-Camacho, F.; Calderón-Canales, E. Preschool Children’s Reasoning about Sound from an Inferential-Representational Approach. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalogiannakis, Μ.; Rekoumi, C.; Chatzipapas, C. Playing on the journey of sound. A teaching proposal for children in early childhood. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Computer Based Learning in Science CBLIS’2012, Learning Science in the Society of Computers, Barcelona, Spain, 26–29 June 2012; Pintó, R., López, V., Simarro, C., Eds.; Centre for Research in Science and Mathematics Education: Barcelona, Spain, 2012; pp. 279–285. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, M.; Trundle, K. Shrieks and shrills: Investigating sound in preschool. Sci. Child. 2014, 52, 38–43. [Google Scholar]
- Theodoraki, C.; Plakitsi, K. Analyzing activities in the course of science education, according to activity theory: The case of sound. US China Educ. Rev. A 2013, 3, 353–364. [Google Scholar]
- Larsson, J. Contextual and conceptual intersubjectivity and opportunities for emergent Science knowledge about sound. Int. J. Ear. Child. 2013, 45, 101–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Calderón-Canales, E.; Gallegos-Cázares, L.; Flores-Camacho, F. Sound representations in preschool students. J. Stud. Educ. Dev. 2019, 42, 952–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patton, M.Q. Qualitative Research and Evaluation Methods, 3rd ed.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, L.; Manion, L.; Morrison, K. Research Methods in Education, 6th ed.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: Oxford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Ravanis, K. Mental representations of light propagation time for 10- and 14-year-old students: Didactical perspectives. J. Bal. Sci. Educ. 2019, 18, 276–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ravanis, K. Precursor models of the Physical Sciences in Early Childhood Education students’ thinking. Sci. Educ. Res. Prax. 2020, 76, 24–31. [Google Scholar]
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Sound propagates in space | 13 | 14.3 |
b. Sound as a product of human and social activity | 61 | 67 |
c. Sound as a product of vocal expression | 17 | 18.7 |
Categories of Responses. | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Sound propagates in space | 16 | 17.6 |
b. Sound as a product of human and social activity | 66 | 72.5 |
c. Sound as a product of vocal expression | 9 | 9.9 |
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Music is sound | 18 | 19.8 |
b. Music is associated with sound, in an unstable manner yet. | 56 | 61.5 |
c. Music and sound totally differ | 17 | 18.7 |
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Differences based on subjective characteristics of sound | 24 | 26.4 |
b. Recognition of limited subjective characteristics of sound | 53 | 58.2 |
c. Absence of criteria for sound discrimination | 14 | 15.4 |
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Recognition of 5–6 sounds | 13 | 14.3 |
b. Recognition of 3–4 sounds | 38 | 41.7 |
c. Recognition of 1–2 sounds | 40 | 44.0 |
Categories of Responses. | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Sound produced from a variety of sources with a clear distinction between sound and sources | 11 | 12.1 |
b. Sound production from a limited number of sources without a clear distinction between sound and sources | 61 | 67 |
c. Correlation of sound mainly with voice | 19 | 20.9 |
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies (%) |
---|---|---|
a. Sound is propagated everywhere in space | 14 | 15.4 |
b. Focus on the perception of sound by humans | 62 | 68.1 |
c. Focus on sound production | 15 | 16.5 |
Categories of Responses | Frequencies | Relative Frequencies % |
---|---|---|
Sound propagates everywhere | 17 | 18.7 |
Sound between the source and the receiver | 60 | 65.9 |
Sound on the receiver | 14 | 15.4 |
Answers | a. Sufficient | f | b. Intermediate | f | c. Insufficient | f | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
First Research Question | Question 1.1. What is sound? | 5, 6, 14, 15, 31, 33, 42, 46, 61, 69, 77, 84, 91 | 13 | 2, 3, 4, 9, 10, 11, 12, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 29, 32, 34, 35, 37, 39, 40, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88, 90 | 61 | 1, 7, 8, 13, 21, 28, 30, 36, 38, 43, 48, 53, 62, 71, 75, 86, 89 | 17 |
Question 1.2. Sound that are recognized by children. | 5, 6, 12, 14, 15, 31, 33, 42, 46, 60, 61, 69, 77, 84, 90, 91 | 16 | 2, 3, 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 13, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 32, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88, 89 | 66 | 1, 7, 21, 30, 43, 48, 53, 71, 86 | 9 | |
Question 1.3. Is music a kind of sound? | 5, 6, 12, 14, 15, 26, 31, 33, 42, 46, 55, 61, 64, 69, 77, 84, 90, 91 | 18 | 2, 3, 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 16, 17, 18, 19, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 29, 32, 34, 35, 37, 39, 40, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 52, 54, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 63, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88 | 56 | 1, 7, 13, 20, 28, 30, 36, 38, 43, 48, 51, 53, 62, 71, 75, 86, 89 | 17 | |
Second Research Question | Question 2.1. Are all the sounds the same? | 2, 5, 6, 12, 14, 15, 22, 26, 31, 33, 40, 42, 46, 55, 58, 60, 61, 65, 69, 73, 77, 84, 90, 91 | 24 | 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 13, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 23, 24, 25, 27, 29, 32, 34, 35, 36, 37, 39, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 56, 57, 59, 63, 64, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88 | 53 | 1, 3, 7, 28, 30, 38, 43, 48, 53, 62, 71, 75, 86, 89 | 14 |
Question 2.2. Sound recognition. | 6, 14, 15, 31, 41, 42, 46, 61, 64, 69, 77, 84, 91 | 13 | 2, 3, 4, 5, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 17, 19, 21, 22, 24, 26, 27, 28, 32, 33, 34, 35, 37, 39, 44, 47, 49, 54, 55, 58, 60, 65, 66, 73, 81, 82, 85, 89, 90 | 38 | 1, 7, 13, 16, 18, 20, 23, 25, 29, 30, 36, 38, 40, 43, 45, 48, 50, 51, 52, 53, 56, 57, 59, 62, 63, 67, 68, 70, 71, 72, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 80, 83, 86, 87, 88 | 40 | |
Third Research Question | Question 3.1. How are sounds produced? | 6, 14, 15, 31, 42, 46, 61, 69, 77, 84, 91 | 11 | 5, 3, 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 16, 17, 18, 19, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 29, 32, 33, 34, 35, 37, 39, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 62, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88, 90 | 61 | 1, 2, 7, 13, 20, 21, 28, 30, 36, 38, 40, 43, 48, 53, 63, 71, 75, 86, 89 | 19 |
Question 3.2. Where do sounds go? | 5, 6, 14, 15, 31, 39, 42, 46, 61, 64, 69, 77, 84, 91 | 14 | 2, 3, 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 32, 33, 34, 35, 37, 40, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 52, 54, 55, 56, 57, 59, 60, 63, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88, 90 | 62 | 1, 7, 13, 30, 36, 38, 43, 48, 53, 58, 62, 71, 75, 86, 89 | 15 | |
Question 3.3. Sound in space. | 5, 6, 12, 14, 15, 31, 33, 42, 46, 55, 61, 64, 69, 77, 84, 90, 91 | 17 | 2, 4, 8, 9, 10, 11, 13, 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 29, 32, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 44, 45, 47, 49, 50, 51, 54, 56, 57, 59, 60, 63, 65, 66, 67, 68, 70, 72, 73, 74, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 87, 88, 89 | 60 | 1, 3, 7, 28, 30, 43, 48, 52, 53, 58, 62, 71, 75, 86 | 14 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ravanis, K.; Kaliampos, G.; Pantidos, P. Preschool Children Science Mental Representations: The Sound in Space. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 242. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050242
Ravanis K, Kaliampos G, Pantidos P. Preschool Children Science Mental Representations: The Sound in Space. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(5):242. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050242
Chicago/Turabian StyleRavanis, Konstantinos, George Kaliampos, and Panagiotis Pantidos. 2021. "Preschool Children Science Mental Representations: The Sound in Space" Education Sciences 11, no. 5: 242. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050242
APA StyleRavanis, K., Kaliampos, G., & Pantidos, P. (2021). Preschool Children Science Mental Representations: The Sound in Space. Education Sciences, 11(5), 242. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050242