Efficacy of the RtI Model in the Treatment of Reading Learning Disabilities
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Tier 1: Evidence-based, generalized, preventive, proactive instruction for all students in a class. The instruction is given within the timetable for the subject, and it is here where possible students who are at risk of presenting difficulties are detected [13,14]. Monitoring progress is just as important as identification, which is why it must be dynamic and be able to measure possible changes in student performance [15], therefore all students are evaluated at three time points throughout the academic year: at the beginning, in the middle, and at the end [14].
- Tier 2: A more focused intervention aimed at achieving objectives based on the student’s needs. It is taught in small homogeneous groups (three to six students) and is aimed at at-risk students who had an inadequate response to Tier 1 [16]. In the earliest educational years, this intervention is preventive in nature and the objective is to identify students at risk of presenting difficulties, consisting of a relatively brief intervention that allows students to be directed towards achievement [17]. These are typically 40-min daily sessions and progress is monitored once a month [18,19].
- Tier 3: Based on individualized, more intensive intervention, given individually or in very small homogeneous groups (one to three students). At this tier, 60-min daily sessions and weekly monitoring are advocated with previously set objectives [14].
- Phonetics: understanding the relationship and the rules of grapheme-phoneme conversion is a prerequisite for correct identification of words [20].
- Vocabulary: instruction focused on word recognition and understanding what words mean from their use is very important for school success and reading comprehension. When a student has a rich knowledge of vocabulary, they meet one of the main objectives of the first few years of education [24].
- Comprehension: the ability to understand the meaning of a text, includes different cognitive processes, such as inferences and connections with previous knowledge [18]. For this reason, prevention is a very important factor in dealing with learning disabilities, because there is a high chance of success in cases identified at the beginning [25].
2. Method
2.1. Bibliographic Search and Selection Procedure
2.2. Analysis Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Assessment Measures
3.1.1. Identification
3.1.2. Evaluation/Monitoring in RtI Model
3.2. Intervention in Different Tiers of the RtI Model
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Pediatr. Rev. 2003, 24, 147–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wajuihian, S.O.; Naidoo, K.S. Dyslexia: An overview. Afr. Vis. Eye Health 2011, 70, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vellutino, F.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Scanlon, D.M. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kavale, K. Discrepancy models in the identification of learning disability. In Identification of Learning Disabilities: Research to Practice; Bradley, R., Danielson, L., Hallahan, D.P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; pp. 369–426. [Google Scholar]
- Jiménez, J.E. Retos y prospectiva de la atención al alumnado con dificultades específicas de aprendizaje: Hacia un modelo basado en la respuesta a la intervención. In Respuestas Flexibles en Contextos Educativos Diversos; Navarro, J., Fernández, M.T., Soto, F.J., Tortosa, F., Eds.; Consejería de Educación, Formación y Empleo: Murcia, Spain, 2012; pp. 30–45. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, E.; Mellard, D.; Byrd, S. Alternative models of learning disabilities identification: Considerations and nitial conclusions. J. Learn. Disabil. 2005, 38, 569–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Soriano, M. La investigación en dificultades de aprendizaje: Un análisis documental. Rev. Neurol. 2005, 41, 550–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradley, R.; Danielson, L.; Doolittle, J. Response to intervention. J. Learn. Disabil. 2005, 38, 485–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L. Introduction to response to intervention: What, why, and how valid is it? Read. Res. Q. 2006, 41, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, D.; Mock, D.; Morgan, P.; Young, C. Responsiveness-to-intervention: Definitions, evidence, and implications for the learning disabilities construct. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2003, 18, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, L.; Vaughn, S. Responsiveness-to-Intervention: A Decade Later. J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 45, 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiménez, J.E.; Luft Baker, D.; Rodríguez, C.; Crespo, P.; Artiles, C.; Alfonso, M.; González, D.; Peake, C.; Suárez, N. Evaluación del progreso de aprendizaje en lectura dentro de un Modelo de Respuesta a la Intervención (RtI) en la Comunidad Autónoma de Canarias. Escr. Psicol. 2011, 4, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Compton, D.L.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L.S.; Bryant, J.D. Selecting at-risk readers in first grade for early intervention: A two-year longitudinal study of decision rules and procedures. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 394–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiménez, J.E.; Crespo, P. Modelo de respuesta a la intervención: Definición y principios componentes. In Modelo de Respuesta a la Intervención. Un Enfoque Preventivo para el Abordaje de las Dificultades Específicas de Aprendizaje; Jiménez, J.E., Ed.; Ediciones Pirámide: Madrid, Spain, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Crespo, P.; Jiménez, J.E.; Rodríguez, C.; González, D. El Modelo de Respuesta a la Intervención en la Comunidad Autónoma de Canarias: Nivel 2 de Intervención. Rev. Psicol. Educ. 2013, 8, 187–203. [Google Scholar]
- Arrimada, M.; Torrance, M.; Fidalgo, R. Response to Intervention Model in writing: A review of Assessment measures and instructional practices. Pap. Psicól. 2020, 41, 54–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wanzek, J.; Vaughn, S.; Scammacca, N.; Gatlin, B.; Walker, M.A.; Capin, P. Meta-analyses of the effects of tier 2 type reading interventions in grades K-3. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 28, 551–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jennings, M.; McDowell, K.D.; Carroll, J.A.; Bohn-Gettler, C.M. Applying a teacher-designed response to intervention to improve the reading among struggling third grade students. Open Commun. J. 2015, 9, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fuchs, L.S.; Fuchs, D. On the importance of a unified model of responsiveness to intervention. Child Dev. Perspect. 2009, 3, 41–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Al Otaiba, S.; Rouse, A.G.; Baker, K. Elementary grade intervention approaches to treat specific learning disabilities, including dyslexia. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 829–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gonzales, W.; Tejero, M. Libros en Mano: Phonological Awareness Intervention in Children’s Native Languages. Educ. Sci. 2018, 8, 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mehigan, G. Effects of Fluency Oriented Instruction on Motivation for Reading of Struggling Readers. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rasinski, T.V.; Yates, R.; Foerg, K.; Greene, K.; Paige, D.; Young, C.; Rupley, W. Impact of Classroom-Based Fluency Instruction on Grade One Students in an Urban Elementary School. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nousiainen, M.; Koponen, I.T. Pre-Service Teachers’ Declarative Knowledge of Wave-Particle Dualism of Electrons and Photons: Finding Lexicons by Using Network Analysis. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Batista, M.; Pestun, M.S.V. The RTI Model as a prevention strategy for learning disorder. Psicol. Esc. Educ. 2019, 23, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denton, C.A.; Fletcher, J.M.; Taylor, W.P.; Barth, A.E.; Vaughn, S. An experimental evaluation of guided reading and explicit interventions for primary-grade students at-risk for reading difficulties. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2014, 7, 268–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Greulich, L.; Al Otaiba, S.; Schatschneider, C.; Wanzek, J.; Ortiz, M.; Wagner, R.K. Understanding inadequate response to first-grade multi-tier intervention: Nomothetic and idiographic perspectives. Learn. Disabil. Q. 2014, 37, 204–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miciak, J.; Cirino, P.T.; Ahmed, Y.; Reid, E.; Vaughn, S. Executive functions and response to intervention: Identification of students struggling with reading comprehension. Learn. Disabil. Q. 2019, 42, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Al Otaiba, S.; Connor, C.M.; Folsom, J.S.; Wanzek, J.; Greulich, L.; Schatschneider, C.; Wagner, R.K. To wait in Tier 1 or intervene immediately: A randomized experiment examining first-grade response to intervention in reading. Except. Child. 2014, 81, 11–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denton, C.A.; Tolar, T.D.; Fletcher, J.M.; Barth, A.E.; Vaughn, S.; Francis, D.J. Effects of tier 3 intervention for students with persistent reading difficulties and characteristics of inadequate responders. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 105, 633–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Al Otaiba, S.; Kim, Y.S.; Wanzek, J.; Petscher, Y.; Wagner, R.K. Long-term effects of first-grade multi-tier intervention. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2014, 7, 250–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pericola, L.; Speece, D.L.; Silverman, R.; Ritchey, K.D.; Schatschneider, C.; Cooper, D.H.; Montanaro, E.; Jacobs, D. Validation of a supplemental reading intervention for first-grade children. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010, 43, 402–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Romeo, R.R.; Christodoulou, J.A.; Halverson, K.K.; Murtagh, J.; Cyr, A.B.; Schimmel, C.; Chang, P.; Hook, P.C.; Gabrieli, J.D. Socioeconomic status and reading disability: Neuroanatomy and plasticity in response to intervention. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 2297–2312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chambers, B.; Slavin, R.E.; Madden, N.A.; Abrami, P.; Logan, M.K.; Gifford, R. Small-group, computer-assisted tutoring to improve reading outcomes for struggling first and second graders. Elem. Sch. J. 2011, 111, 625–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ritchey, K.D.; Silverman, R.D.; Montanaro, E.A.; Speece, D.L.; Schatschneider, C. Effects of a tier 2 supplemental reading intervention for at-risk fourth-grade students. Except. Child. 2012, 78, 318–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tilanus, E.A.; Segers, E.; Verhoeven, L. Predicting responsiveness to a sustained reading and spelling intervention in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2019, 25, 190–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouton, B.; McConnell, J.R.; Barquero, L.A.; Gilbert, J.K.; Compton, D.L. Upside-Down Response to Intervention: A Quasi-Experimental Study. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2018, 33, 229–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castejón, L.A.; González-Pumaniega, S.; Cuestos, A. Enseñar fluidez lectora en el aula: De la investigación a la práctica. Ocnos Rev. Estud. Lect. 2019, 18, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, E.; Compton, D.L.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L.; Bouton, B. Examining the Predictive Validity of a Dinamic Assessment of Decoding to Forecast Response Tier 2 Intervention. J. Learn. Disabil. 2014, 47, 409–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Georgiou, G.K.; Savage, R.; Dunn, K.; Bowers, P.; Parrila, R. Examining the effects of Structured Word Inquiry on the reading and spelling skills of persistently poor Grade 3 readers. J. Res. Read. 2020, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Svensson, I.; Fälth, L.; Tjus, T.; Heimann, M.; Gustafson, S. Two-step tier three interventions for children in grade three with low reading fluency. J. Res. Spec. Educ. Needs 2019, 19, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Norman, E.R.; Nelosn, P.M.; Klingbeil, D.A. Profiles of reading performance after exiting Tier 2 intervention. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 57, 757–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connor, R.E.; Bocian, K.M.; Beach, K.D.; Sanchez, V.; Flynn, L.J. Special education in a 4-year Response to Intervention (RtI) environment: Characteristics of students with learning disability and grade of identification. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2013, 28, 98–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cho, E.; Capin, P.; Roberts, G.; Vaughn, S. Examining Predictive Validity of Oral Reading Fluency Slope in Upper Elementary Grades Using Quantile Regression. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 51, 565–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vaughn, S.; Capin, P.; Scammacca, N.; Roberts, G.; Cirino, P.; Fletcher, J.M. The Critical Role of Word Reading as a Predictor of Response to Intervention. J. Learn. Disabil. 2020, 53, 415–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferroni, M.V.; Barreyro, J.P.; Mena, M.; Diuk, B.G. Perfiles cognitivos de niños de nivel socioeconómico bajo con dificultades en la velocidad lectora: Análisis de los resultados de una intervención. Interdisciplinaria 2019, 36, 273–288. [Google Scholar]
- Jiménez, J.E.; Rodríguez, C.; Crespo, P.; González, D.; Artiles, C.; Alfonso, M. Implementation of Response to Intervention (RtI) Model in Spain: An example of a collaboration between Canarian universities and the department of education of the Canary Islands. Psicothema 2010, 22, 935–942. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.S.; Apel, K.; Al Otaiba, S. The relation of linguistic awareness and vocabulary to word reading and spelling for first-grade students participating in response to intervention. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2013, 44, 337–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Duff, F.J.; Hulme, C.; Grainger, K.; Hardwick, S.J.; Miles, J.N.; Snowling, M.J. Reading and language intervention for children at risk of dyslexia: A randomised controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 1234–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Martens, B.K.; Young, N.D.; Mullane, M.P.; Baxter, E.L.; Sallade, S.J.; Kellen, D.; Long, S.J.; Sullivan, W.E.; Womack, A.J.; Underberg, J. Effects of word overlap on generalized gains from a repeated readings intervention. J. Sch. Psychol. 2019, 74, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steacy, L.M.; Edwards, A.A. Modeling and Visualizing the Codevelopment of Word and Nonword Reading in Children from First Through Fourth Grade: Informing Developmental Trajectories of Children With Dyslexia. Child. Dev. 2020, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milburn, T.F.; Lonigan, C.J.; Philips, B.M. Stability of Risk Status During Preschool. J. Learn. Disabil. 2019, 52, 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgoyne, K.; Duff, F.J.; Clarke, P.J.; Buckley, S.; Snowling, M.J.; Hulme, C. Efficacy of a reading and language intervention for children with Down syndrome: A randomized controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 1044–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhou, Q.; Dufrene, B.A.; Mercer, S.; Olmi, D.J.; Tingstom, D.H. Parent-implemented reading interventions within a response to intervention framework. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 56, 1139–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkes, S.; Kazakoff, E.R.; Prescott, J.E.; Bundschuh, K.; Hook, P.E.; Wolf, R.; Hurwitz, L.B.; Macaruso, P. Measuring the impact of a blended learning model on early literacy growth. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 2020, 36, 595–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jiménez, J.E.; Gutiérrez, N. IPAL: Indicadores de Progreso de Aprendizaje en Lectura; Universidad de La Laguna: Santa Cruz de Tenerife, Spain, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Slavin, R.E.; Lake, C.; Davis, S.; Madden, N.A. Effective programs for struggling readers: A best-evidence synthesis. Educ. Res. Rev. 2011, 6, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, K.J.; Morris, D.; Fields, M. Intervention after grade 1: Serving increased numbers of struggling readers effectively. J. Lit. Res. 2005, 37, 61–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Method of Identification | Selection Criteria | Studies | |
Diagnostic report | Clinical diagnosis of some type of learning disability demonstrated by evaluations | [33] Romeo et al.; [34] Chambers et al.; [35] Ritchey et al.; [36] Tilanus et al. | |
Expert judgement of the tutor | Selection of classroom students who present more difficulties | [29] Al Otaiba et al.; [31] Al Otaiba et al.; [32] Pericola et al.; [37] Bouton et al.; [38] Castejón et al.; [39] Cho et al.; [40] Georgiou et al.; [41] Svensson et al. | |
Specific Test | Evaluated Variable | ||
AIMSweb | Score below the 40th percentile (subtest Letter Sound Fluency) | Letter sound fluency | [29] Al Otaiba et al.; [31] Al Otaiba et al. |
CBM-R 1 | Standard score below 100 words read per minute in three passages | Oral reading | [42] Van Norman et al. |
CTOPP 2 |
| Reading speed and accuracy and phonemic awareness Core reading subskills | [39] Cho et al. [33] Romeo et al. |
DIBELS 3 |
| Reading in both studies | [33] Romeo et al. [43] O’Connor et al. |
DRA 4 | Score of 1, 2 or 3 | Reading | [32] Pericola et al. |
GMRT 5 | Score of 85 or lower | Reading achievement | [28] Miciak et al.; [44] Cho et al.; [45] Vaughn et al. |
KBIT 6 | Score in or above the 16th percentile | Nonverbal cognition | [33] Romeo et al. |
MAP 7 | Score below the 25th percentile | Academic achievement | [18] Jennings et al. |
Not specified | Score in the 50th percentile for reading accuracy and score below the 30th percentile in word reading speed with different levels of phonological and spelling complexity | Reading speed and accuracy | [46] Ferroni et al. |
RAN/RAS 8 | Score below the 25th percentile | Rapid automatized naming | [33] Romeo et al. |
The Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Behavior Checklist | Score above the 75th percentile | Reading | [47] Jiménez et al. |
TOWRE 9 |
| Identification of words and decoding. Word reading fluency. Word reading fluency | [29] Al Otaiba et al.; [31] Al Otaiba et al. [26] Denton et al. [30] Denton et al. |
WASI 10 |
| Intelligence in both studies | [37] Bouton et al. [40] Georgiou et al. |
WIF 11 | Subprime lowest score | Word identification fluency | [32] Pericola et al. |
WJ 12 |
| Letter identification and decoding Letter identification and decoding Letter identification and comprehension Comprehension | [26] Denton et al.; [30] Denton et al. [27] Greulich et al. [48] Kim et al. [28] Miciak et al. |
WRAT 13 | Score less than 90 in word reading | Reading and comprehension | [40] Georgiou et al. |
YARC 14 | Lower scores in Early Word Reading and Single Word Reading | Reading and comprehension | [49] Duff et al. |
Studies | Tier | Selection Battery | Monitoring Battery | Variables and Monitoring Mode |
---|---|---|---|---|
[27] Greulich et al. | 2, 3 | WJ | AIMSweb CTOPP DIBELS KBIT TOWRE |
|
[28] Miciak et al. | 2 | GMRT and WJ | TOWRE |
|
[29] Al Otaiba et al. | 1, 2, 3 | AIMSweb TOWRE | DIBELS WJ |
|
[30] Denton et al. | 3 | TOWRE WJ | CTOPP DIBELS |
|
[31] Al Otaiba et al. | 1, 2, 3 | AIMSweb TOWRE | DIBELS WJ |
|
[32] Pericola et al. | 1, 2 | DRA WIF | CTOPP WJ WRMT |
|
[33] Romeo et al. | 2, 3 | CTOPP DIBELS KBIT RAN/RAS | TOWRE WRMT |
|
[34] Chambers et al. | 2 | Diagnostic report | WJ |
|
[35] Ritchey et al. | 2 | Diagnostic report GMRT TOSWRF | TOWRE WJ |
|
[37] Bouton et al. | 3 | Expert judgement of the tutor WASI | TOWRE WRMT |
|
[38] Castejón et al. | 1 | Expert judgement of the tutor | Collaborative Program of Fluency in Reading and Writing |
|
[39] Cho et al. | 2 | Expert judgement of the tutor CTOPP | TOWRE WJ |
|
[41] Svensson et al. | 3 | Expert judgement of the tutor | LOGOS |
|
[43] O’Connor et al. | 2 | DIBELS | WRMT 1 |
|
[44] Cho et al. | 2 | GMRT | AIMSweb |
|
[45] Vaughn et al. | 2 | GMRT | WJ |
|
[46] Ferroni et al. | 3 | Not specified | No specific |
|
[47] Jiménez et al. | 2 | Not specified | EGRA 2 No specific |
|
[48] Kim et al. | 1, 2, 3 | WJ | CTOPP |
|
[49] Duff et al. | 2, 3 | YARC | CELF |
|
[50] Martens et al. | 1 | Not specified | CBM-R |
|
[51] Steacy et al. | Not specified | Not specified | CTOPP TOWRE |
|
[52] Milburn et al. | Not specified | Not specified | CELF 3 CTOPP TERA-3 4 |
|
[53] Burgoyne et al. | 2 | Specific sample | APT 5 CELF EOWPT and ROWPT 6 TRG-2 7 YARC |
|
[54] Zhou et al. | 2, 3 | Specific criteria | AIMSweb |
|
[55] Wilkes et al. | 1 | Specific criteria | DIBELS |
|
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[38] Castejón et al. | Not specified | Class group In ordinary classroom | Tutors | 28 weeks For 7 months | Timed repeated reading of lists of words and phrases adapted to the level of the students. If the objective is achieved (the average of the class), the next list is read and if it is not, another of the same level is read. A notebook is used to practice at home in the same way and, if any student does little practice at home, the special education teacher complements the training at school. | In general, there is a reduction in reading times for short high frequency words and long low frequency words. Regarding errors and reading speed, they were reduced in relation to pre-intervention scores, where most subjects scored at risk. In general, subjects have improved in both conditions but should continue with training, but there were not statistically significant differences. |
[50] Martens et al. | Not specified | Groups of 2–4 students In ordinary classroom | Graduate students and research assistants | 30-min sessions | Fluency training with generalization and training passages extracted from AIMSweb. Training passages are created by modifying the generalization passage, retaining words, and replacing nouns and verbs with new ones. | There are significant differences between the ME (multiple exemplar) group and both the LWO (low word overlap) (X2 = 7.54, p = 0.006) and HWO group (high word overlap) (X2 = 16.17, p = 0.001). The difference between the LWO and HWO groups was not significant (X2 = 1.90, p = 0.168). |
[55] Wilkes et al. | Not specified | Individual Place not specified | Teachers | 20–60 min/week For 1 year | Reading instruction with the Core5 program, focused on phonological awareness, phonetics, structural analysis, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension. Students must complete each activity with at least 90% accuracy to advance to the next level. When students show inaccurate responses, Core5 offers simplified activities. They also have access to paper and pencil tasks to develop automaticity and language skills. | Experimental group outperformed control group and the discrepancy between both on post-test scores was more pronounced when students had lower pretest scores, but there were not statistically significant differences. |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[32] Pericola et al. | The mean fidelity across lessons was 90% (SD = 0.07) | Tier 1: small groups in 1st grade Tier 2: groups of 3 to 4 students Both in the ordinary classroom | Research assistants and graduates | 16 h For 11 weeks | Tier 1: phonetic instruction, guided reading and activities focused on written language, study of words and spelling, using books from the Houghton Mifflin Reader collection. Tier 2: Each lesson contains three parts: (a) Phonological awareness and phonetic skills to reinforce letter-to-sound relationships. (b) Visual and decodable words, vocabulary and pre-reading comprehension strategies. (c) Fluency and reading comprehension, oral reading, discussion and word recognition. | The intervention significantly affected growth in Decodable Word Fluency, F (1, 59) = 8.57, p = 0.005, and Spelling of Words, F (1, 88) = 5.85, p = 0.02. In both cases, the results favored the intervention group. |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[18] Jennings et al. | Not specified | Tier 1: 3rd grade group Tier 3: individualized instruction Both outside the ordinary classroom | Instructor | 90 min, 5 days/week For 6 weeks | Reading instruction (vocabulary and fluency) at both levels is with the Houghton Mifflin Reading Program. In Tier 3, Power Reading is used to work on reading passages, comprehension with questions, and vocabulary. In the first session, they read aloud and write the unknown words that the instructor explains. In the second session, the words are reviewed and they are divided into base and affix words. In the third and fourth sessions, they create sentences with these words. In the fifth session, they read a passage aloud and answer questions. | The intervention group obtained statistically significant scores post-intervention compared to the administration of the pre-intervention tests on fluency, vocabulary knowledge, and reading comprehension. The intervention group and the control group started with similar scores on the NWEA MAP (15.8% for the intervention group and 13.5% for the comparison group) and, although the students in the experimental group started in the lower quartile, at the end of the intervention all were reading at or above grade level. In contrast, only three of the eight in the control group read at grade level on the posttest. |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[29] Al Otaiba et al.; [31] Al Otaiba et al. | Inter-rater reliability was high (98.1%). Fidelity ratings for the tutors ranged from 0.77 to 0.98 (M = 0.89) | Tier 1: 1st grade group in the classroom Tier 2: groups of 4–7 students outside the ordinary classroom Tier 3: groups of one-three students outside the ordinary classroom | Tier 1: tutors Tier 2 and 3: qualified personnel | Tier 1: 90 min Tier 2: 30 min, two days/week Tier 3: 45 min, four days/week For 24 weeks | Tier 1: Basic reading and language arts instruction through Open Court. Tier 2: Open Court activities Imagine It! and the Florida Center for Reading Research K-3 Center that include phonological awareness and skills with letter sound, decoding, visual word instruction, and fluency. Tier 3: Early Interventions in Reading activities. Vocabulary is also worked on. | In the Al Otaiba study [29] there were not statically significant differences in pretest Reading factor scores for students in the Dynamic RTI compared to the Typical RTI groups. However, the Dynamic RTI group had statistically significantly higher posttest Reading scores than did students in Typical RTI, with a moderate effect size of 0.314. Tier 2 students in the Dynamic RTI condition obtained significantly higher reading outcomes scores compared to Tier 2 students in Typical RTI condition, who only received Tier 2 when they did not respond to Tier 1 over the first or second session. In the Al Otaiba study [31], ER (relatively easy to remediate) and SR (requiring sustained remediation) groups had statistically lower scores on TOWRE and oral reading fluency compared to the NR (no risk) group (p < 0.01). SR group also had statistically lower scores on the Letter Word Identification (p < 0.01). Compared with NR group, ER group in the Dynamic condition had higher scores on all the four reading outcomes in second grade (p ≤ 0.02). In addition, ER group had statistically higher scores than NR group (p ≤ 0.03) in all the reading outcomes (p < 0.01), except Passage Comprehension, while SR group had significantly lower scores than the NR group in the reading outcomes. |
[48] Kim et al. | Test-retest reliability estimates were 0.88 for the blending and elision tasks for 5–7 years old children | Tier 1: 3rd grade group Tier 2: groups of 5–7 students Tier 3: groups of one-three students Unspecified location | Research team | Tier 1: 90 min daily Tier 2: 30 min, two days/week Tier 3: 45 min, four days/week For 24 weeks | All three tiers use the Open Court program and instruction is based on the relationship of language awareness and vocabulary with word reading and spelling. | Tier 1 group outperformed Tier 2 and 3 groups on linguistic awareness, phonological, and orthographic awareness measures (p < 0.001) but Tier 2 group did not differ from Tier 3 students on any measure (statistical significance at p < 0.004 = 0.05/12), as shown post hoc tests. Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and orthographic awareness measures were all moderately related to end of year word reading and spelling (0.41 ≤ rs ≤ 0.63). The relation between morphological awareness, phonological awareness, vocabulary, and orthographic awareness measures were from weak (r = 0.27 between orthographic awareness and vocabulary) to moderate (r = 0.68 between morphological awareness and vocabulary). |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[53] Burgoyne et al. | Not specified | 1st grade group In school (unspecified classroom) | Teaching assistants | 40 min/day For 20 weeks | Instruction follows a prescribed program with opportunities to tailor sessions according to the needs and skills of participants. It includes a reading chapter for teaching reading and phonetics and a language chapter for learning new vocabulary. Visual supports and simple games are also used as reinforcers. | Comparing t2 (20-week intervention period) with t1 (before intervention), there were statistically significant differences on single word reading (d = 0.23; p = 0.002), letter-sound knowledge (d = 0.42; p = 0.002), phoneme blending (d = 0.54; p = 0.02), and taught expressive vocabulary (d = 0.47; p = 0.01). There were not statistically significant differences between t3 (after the second 20-week intervention period) and t1. |
[34] Chambers et al. | Not specified | Groups of 6 students in 1st and 2nd grade In the ordinary classroom | Certified teachers | 45 min, four days/week For 13 weeks | They work on phonemic awareness, skills with letters, visual words, vocabulary, fluency, and understanding through a program that combines cooperative learning, computer-assisted instruction, integrated multimedia, and mentoring and evaluates strengths and reading difficulties. | Statistically significant differences were observed, such that the first-grade treatment group outperformed in the pre-test measures of LWID (Letter-Word Identification) to the individually tutored control group (p = 0.05, η2 = 0.28) and marginally higher in the second grade (p = 0.068, η2 = 0.21). In the LWID post-test (η2 = 0.17, p = 0.05), WA (Word Attack) (η2 = 0.21, p = 0.04), and PC (Passage Comprehension) (η2 = 0.15, p = 0.05) statistically significant differences were also obtained. |
[39] Cho et al. | Fidelity reached an average of 94.04% | Unspecified 1st grade groups At school (unspecified classroom) | Graduate students | 45 min, three days/week For 14 weeks | Each session addressed: sight word reading, story words, letter sounds, phonological awareness and decoding, spelling, sentences and passage reading. For each activity, the previous session is reviewed and new information is entered. | WIF final level and growth were highly correlated with timed and untimed word reading (r = 0.68–0.89) and less highly correlated with timed and untimed word decoding (r = 0.45–0.68) and with passage comprehension (r = 0.59–0.70). WIF final level was also highly correlated with their growth during tutoring (r = 0.89). |
[44] Cho et al. | Fidelity reached an average of 90% | 5th grade groups Unspecified location | Research staff | 30 min/day For 16 weeks | Multicomponent instruction aimed at vocabulary, word reading, fluency, and reading comprehension, all working through scientific texts. To improve basic reading skills, students received phonics instruction. | Pearson correlations indicate that the TOSREC (Test of Silent Reading Efficiency and Comprehension) and GMRT (Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests) were only moderately correlated (r = 0.26, p = 0.008). Model 1 (ORF initial performance) and Model 2 (ORF slope) demonstrated a moderate correlation to the GMRT (r = 0.32, p < 0.001; r = 0.23, p = 0.019, respectively) and a slightly stronger correlation to the TOSREC (r = 0.44, p < 0.001; r = 0.37, p < 0.001, respectively). Model 3 (ORF initial performance and slope simultaneously) were also moderately correlated (r = 0.47, p = 0.02). |
[26] Denton et al. | The mean program adherence score across tutors and observations was 4 | Groups of 2–4 1st grade students Outside the regular classroom | Experienced clinical teachers or tutors | 45 min, four days/week For 23–25 weeks |
| There were statistically significant differences on Word Reading and Phonological Decoding between GR (Guided Reading) and TSI (Typical School Instruction) on Letter-Word Identification (p < 0.05); and between EX (Explicit Instruction) and TSI on both Letter-Word Identification (p < 0.05) and Word Attack (p < 0.01). Follow-up analyses revealed only a significant difference for WJ III Passage Comprehension between EX and TSI conditions (p < 0.05). For the measures of fluency (TOWRE, TPRI Passage Fluency) and for TOSREC (to measure Silent Reading Fluency and Comprehension), there were not statistically significant differences. |
[47] Jiménez et al. | Not specified | Groups of 4–6 students from kindergarten to 2nd grade Outside the ordinary classroom | Researchers | 30 min/day For 12 weeks | Intervention carried out with the PREDEA program and focused on main components of reading, reading strategies, and educational activities that are grouped into different modules (phonological awareness, alphabetical knowledge, vocabulary, oral and reading comprehension, and fluency). | Results show that progress monitoring measures were significantly different in alphabetic knowledge F (2.34, 109.81) = 42.31, p < 0.001, η² = 0.47; isolation F (2.42, 125.91) = 5.32, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.09; and phoneme segmentation task F (3, 51) = 5.36, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.24. There were not significant differences found between the progress monitoring measures in vocabulary and word naming accuracy, F (2, 39) = 0.31, p = 0.73, η2 = 0.01; F (3, 40) = 1.79, p = 0.16, η2 = 0.12, respectively. |
[28] Miciak et a | The intervention received a mean global fidelity score of 3.48 (SD = 0.55, range 2.00 to 3.82). | Groups of 4–5 4th grade students At school | Trained tutors | 30 min/day For 16 weeks | A theme is covered for two weeks and in each session three activities were carried out: (a) Development of words and concepts through knowledge of vocabulary. (b) Reading of narrative or expository texts. (c) Study of words using decoding strategies. | There was significant group-by-task interaction, F (10, 590) = 11.31, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.04, on all six Executive Functions factors (Executive Functions, BRIEF/ Metacognition, Fluency, Self-Regulated Learning, Working Memory/Planning, and Working Memory/Updating) |
[43] O’Connor et al. | The average fidelity rating for Tier 2 instruction in Grade 1 was 93%; and for Grades 2 to 4 89% | Groups of 2–3 students from 1st to 4th grade In school (unspecified classroom) | Special education and general education teachers | 25–35 min, four days/week | In 1st grade the intervention is based on Sound Partners and includes explicit instructions on letters, sounds, decoding, word identification, and reading of prayers and books. In 2nd grade, the intervention includes word study, vocabulary, reading, comprehension strategies, spelling, and sentence writing are included. In 3rd and 4th, they work on comprehension, explicit decoding, and writing of more complex words. | The analysis did not reveal significant difference on measures of vocabulary or language use (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test and Test of Oral Language Development) across cohorts (Grade 1: Cohort 1; Grade 2: Cohort 2 comparison group) (Wilks’ lambda (2, 751) = 0.58, ns). M and SD for Cohorts 1 and 2, respectively, were PPVT = 86.78 (12.03) and 87.18 (12.61); TOLD = 8.11 (2.34) and 8.12 (2.35). |
[35] Ritchey et al. | 93% (SD = 4%) of the components were delivered and there were no significant differences in total fidelity per year, F (121) = 0.05, p = 0.823 | Groups of 2–4 4th grade students At school | Research assistants | 40 min, one day/week For 12–15 weeks | The intervention focuses on the understanding of expository texts, working on:
| There were not significant differences on pre-post measures. Instead, there were statistically significant differences on post-test measures: group on ASKIT Comprehension (Assessment of Knowledge and Strategy Use for Information Text), F (1, 16) = 10.09, p = 0.006, favoring the intervention group, g = 0.5631; and group on Science Knowledge, F (1, 16) = 12.70, p < 0.0031, favoring the intervention group too, g = 0.6458. |
[36] Tilanus et al. | Not specified | 2nd year groups in a professional clinic | Specialized professionals | 45 min Phase 1: 12 sessions Phase 2: 36 sessions | In the first phase, grapheme-phoneme correspondences are taught and the basic reading level and the sublexic level are improved to strengthen the ability to combine phonemes and recognize groups of words and morphemes. The second phase focuses on declarative and procedural aspects and the reading of disyllabic and polysyllabic words. Syllabic segmentation is used, starting with phonological units. | For word decoding efficiency, the difference between Time 1 and Time 2 versus Time 2 and Time 3 was statistically significant, t (85) = −2.273, p = 0.026, d = −0.49. For pseudo-word decoding efficiency, the difference in growth was significant, in advantage for children with dyslexia during Time 1 to Time 2 t (223) = 2.016, p = 0.045, d = 0.27. For word decoding accuracy, the difference in growth was statistically significant between Time 1 and Time 2 t (215) = 2.05, p = 0.04, d = 0.28, and between Time 1 and Time 3 in advantage for children with dyslexia t (133.414) = 4.045, p = < 0.001, d = 0.70. For pseudo-word decoding accuracy, the difference in growth was statistically significant in advantage for typical readers. For word spelling, the differences in growth between Time 1 and Time 2 versus Time 2 and Time 3 within the group of children with dyslexia were significant, t (84) = −2.863, p = 0.005, d = −0.63. |
[42] Van Norman et al. | Tutors followed intervention protocols with 96% accuracy on average. | Individuals Inside the classroom | Professionals | 20 min/day | The Reading Corps literacy program is used. The intervention includes duet reading and repeated reading with comprehension strategies. | The observed differences in the proportion of students identified who did not achieve proficiency between Profile 1 (Below Average Start/Average Response) and Profile 2 (Average Start/Average Response) were statistically significant at the p < 0.05 level (χ2 (1) = 6.62, p = 0.010); the differences observed between Profile 1 and Profile 3 (Above Average Start/Fast Response) were statistically significant at the p < 0.001 level (χ2 (1) = 22.69), and the difference in the proportion of students identified as at-risk between Profile 2 and Profile 3 was statistically significant at the p < 0.001 level (χ2 (1) = 7.81). |
[45] Vaughn et al. | The intervention had a fidelity of 96% | Groups of 2–4 students At the school | Researchers | 30 min/day For 16 weeks | The intervention addresses word reading, fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension. It is organized in units of 10 lessons each, aligned with what is taught in ordinary classes and consists of three components: (a) Vocabulary: essential words are taught to understand texts. (b) Reading a passage. (c) Instruction in decoding. | Statistically significant differences were showed in the following measures: the very low word skills cluster had mean standard scores that ranged from 60.84 to 75.60; mean scores ranged from 77.00 to 88.63 for the low word skills cluster and 90.71 to 99.43 for the near-adequate word skills cluster. Mean scores were lowest on the TOWRE Sight Word assessment and highest on the WJ-III Spelling assessment in all three groups, and mean scores for each group were statistically significantly different from each other on each measure at p < 0.001. |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[49] Duff et al. | Not specified | Tier 2: groups of 2–4 students Tier 3: individualized instruction At school (unspecified classroom) | Research team | Tier 2: 30 min, two days/week Tier 3: 20 min, three days/week For 18 weeks | The RALI program is used for instruction, which includes Reading Strand (integrates training in phonological awareness and reading) and Language Strand (focuses on training vocabulary and narrative skills, using storybooks as the basis for its themes and structure). | At t2 (midtest evaluation), statistically significant but small effects were observed in letter knowledge (d = 0.33; p = 0.27) and early word reading (d = 0.13; p = 0.34). Small to moderate effects were also shown on phonemic awareness (d = 0.57; p = 0.47) and taught vocabulary (d = 0.46; p = 0.40). On the other hand, there are no significant differences between the groups in any measure of language or literacy in t3 (posttest evaluation). |
[27] Greulich et al. | Fidelity of intervention implementation was observed to be high (M = 0.89) | Tier 2: groups of 4–7 students Tier 3: groups of 1–3 students Both in 1st grade In school (unspecified classroom) | Graduate students and certified teachers | Tier 2: 30 min, two days/week Tier 3: 45 min, four days/week For 24 weeks | Tier 2: activities taken from Open Court Imagen It! and the Florida Center for Reading Research K-3 Center. Phonological awareness and skills with letter sound, decoding, visual word instruction, and fluency are worked on. Tier 3: activities based on Early Interventions in Reading. Phonological, alphabetic, and phonetic fluency and awareness are worked on. Meaning is worked on at both levels (reading aloud from decodable books written to emphasize the structure of the text. | From the multivariate analysis of variance and the Wilk’s lambda test statistic demonstrated a statistically significant multivariate main effect of membership (group of adequate and inadequate responders), F (16, 139) = 2.191 p = 0.008, with significant differences across most measures (Blending, Elision, Letter Word Identification, Word Attack, Passage Comprehension, Fluency Composite, Speece Rating, Verbal and Non-Verbal IQ, SSRS Academic, SWAN Inattention). The five main models that contain the information about the prediction of adequate and inadequate responders are: Letter Word, Fluency, Blending (R2 = 0.151) (model that explains the greatest variation); IQ-non-verbal, Fluency, Blending (R2 = 0.129); Word Attack, Fluency, Blending (R2 = 0.128); Fluency, Elision, Blending (R2 = 0.126); and Letter Word, Fluency, Elision (R2 = 0.125). |
[54] Zhou et al. | Not specified | Tier 2: groups of three-five students Outside the ordinary classroom Tier 3: individual At Home | Tier 2: researcher Tier 3: parents trained | Tier 2: 30 min, three days/week Tier 3: three days/week For 2 weeks | Tier 2: Phonics, word recognition and text reading are practiced. Tier 3: four conditions:
| There were no statistically significant differences for WCPM (words correct per minute) or EPM (errors per minute) for any of the students |
Study | Fidelity | Group of Students | Who Performs the Intervention | Duration | Description of the RtI Model | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[37] Bouton et al. | Administration on all testing and tutoring instruments have 90% or more accuracy | Individual to 1st grade students In school (unspecified classroom) | Teachers and research assistants | 30 min/day For 35 weeks | Instruction sessions about sounds of letters, visual words, decoding and reading fluency. When they reach objectives, they go on to form small groups with 45-min sessions, three days per week with the same intervention program. | One-way MANOVA was conducted to test the hypothesis that there would be one or more mean differences between student groups in terms of overall word level reading and, with a p < 0.10 criterion, a statistically significant MANOVA effect was obtained, Wilks’ Λ = 0.828; F (4,43) = 2.228; p = 0.082, with the multivariate effect size (η2) estimated at 0.172, a large effect. This implied that 17.2 percent of the variance in the canonically derived dependent variable was attributable to the difference in intervention. This indicated that upside down RTI worked better than regular RTI on regarding reading proficiency, with a 10 percent probability that this difference was due to random sampling error. |
[30] Denton et al. | The mean score for compliance with the program was 93% (SD = 0.07). The mean quality score across components and across teachers was 99% (SD = 0.02). | Groups of 2–3 2nd grade students Outside the ordinary classroom | Certified teachers | 45 min For 24–26 weeks | Responsive Reading Instruction (RRI) is used and, for those who need more instruction, Read Naturally. Each RRI lesson addresses: a) Word study: includes phonological awareness, grapheme-phoneme correspondence, word recognition, phonological decoding, structural analysis and spelling. (b) Oral reading fluency. (c) Reading comprehension. (d) Application of skills and strategies in reading. In the second half of the intervention period, additional fluency instruction is given. | The Pre- and Posttest Means by Treatment Group showed statistically significant differences on WJ Basic Reading [F = 8.13; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.56], WJ Letter-Word Identification [F = 7.90; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.44], WJ Word Attack [F = 5.78; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.65], TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency [F = 5.07; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.39] and WJ III Passage Comprehension [F = 3.95; p < 0.05; η2 = 0.34]. There were not statistically significant differences on TOWRE Phonemic Decoding Efficiency, DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency, and Gates Passage Comprehension. |
[46] Ferroni et al. | Not specified | Individual to 6th grade students At school (unspecified classroom) | Research team | 20 min, two days/week For 8 weeks | Stories are read aloud and Comprehension is supported by asking questions. Reading fluency is also worked on and activities are carried out with the lexical and sub-lexical units involved. | The analysis does not show statistically significant differences, in either of the two groups, between the pre and post-test measures. |
[40] Georgiou et al. | Not specified | Individual Outside the ordinary classroom | Research assistants | 30 min, three days/week For 10 weeks | The Simplicity intervention involves five steps: 1. Practice combining and segmenting phonemes. 2. Introduction, definition, and spelling of a new word. 3. Search for the word in books. 4. Shared reading of written texts that repeat the word. 5. Grapheme-phoneme correspondence. | Both SWI (Structured Word Inquiry) and Simplicity group were advantaged over control group when either morphological awareness or phonological awareness was included in the model (p < 0.05). At delayed post-test, the effect of condition approached significance when morphological awareness was included in the model. The effect reflected a larger mean estimate for SWI (p = 0.051) and Simplicity (p = 0.073) over control group at delayed post-test. Simplicity and SWI were advantaged over Controls across all decoding tasks when participants had stronger morphological awareness skills at pre-test (p < 0.05 in both cases). |
[33] Romeo et al. | Not specified | Groups of 3–5 students from 1st to 3rd grade At school (unspecified classroom) | Teachers | 4 h, five days/week For 6 weeks | They train in phonological processing skills through a multisensory approach (the ability to recognize patterns and create mental representations of words allows a more solid understanding of the letters and their respective sounds) of the Seeing Stars plan. | Higher socioeconomic status (SES) correlated significantly with higher scores on receptive vocabulary (PPVT-4, r = 0.37, p = 0.002), with marginally with higher nonverbal cognitive ability scores (KBIT-2, r = 0.023, p = 0.065), and with higher scores on one of the four single-word reading subtests (WRMT-3 Word Attack: r = 0.26, p = 0.036; all other reading subtests r < 0.17, p > 0.2). Consequently, was marginally correlated with higher reading-composite scores (r = 0.24, p = 0.05). SES was not correlated with scores on any subtests assessing phonological awareness, phonological memory, or rapid naming (all |r| < 0.08, all p > 0.50). |
[41] Svensson et al. | Not specified | Individual to 3rd grade students In school (unspecified classroom) | Special education teachers | 35 sessions For 14 weeks | The Omega-IS and COMPHOT programs begin. Those who show problems in decoding fluency and accuracy receive more sessions with the second program; those with grammatical and reading comprehension problems receive sessions with the first program; those who show difficulty in phonological and spelling aspects receive sessions with both programs. | There was agreement between the test scores at T1 and T2 (Cronbach’s alpha of 0.82 or above) for all variables (Word recognition, Reading fluency, Word and Non-word reading) except for reading comprehension (0.40). There were significant differences for the non-word reading test, where the group which obtained the COMPHOT intervention yielded a significantly higher gain (p < 0.05) than the combined group which had used both intervention methods. Control group outperformed the experimental group on the word recognition test both at T1 and at T4. However, the gap was less at T4 and there was a significant difference in the observed change score (CS) (p < 0.001) between the groups from T1 to T4, where the experimental group had a higher change score (8.5) than the comparison group (1.7). |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Arias-Gundín, O.; García Llamazares, A. Efficacy of the RtI Model in the Treatment of Reading Learning Disabilities. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 209. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050209
Arias-Gundín O, García Llamazares A. Efficacy of the RtI Model in the Treatment of Reading Learning Disabilities. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(5):209. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050209
Chicago/Turabian StyleArias-Gundín, Olga, and Ana García Llamazares. 2021. "Efficacy of the RtI Model in the Treatment of Reading Learning Disabilities" Education Sciences 11, no. 5: 209. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050209
APA StyleArias-Gundín, O., & García Llamazares, A. (2021). Efficacy of the RtI Model in the Treatment of Reading Learning Disabilities. Education Sciences, 11(5), 209. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050209