The Effects of L2 Proficiency on Pragmatic Comprehension and Learner Strategies
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background
3. Method
3.1. Participants
3.2. Instrument
- (a)
- Sorry, I want to buy a new copy.
- (b)
- I have spilled a cup of coffee over your magazine. Do you still want it?
- (c)
- I ruined your magazine so I’ll replace it.
- (d)
- Sorry, Sean, I ruined your magazine. I’ll buy you a new copy.
- Sean: No, don’t worry about replacing it, I read it already.
- Answer: (d)
3.3. Data Collection
3.4. Data Analysis
4. Results
4.1. Results for Research Question 1
4.2. Results for Research Question 2
4.2.1. Quantitative Analysis
4.2.2. Qualitative Analysis
5. Discussion
6. Pedagogical Implications and Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kasper, G.; Rose, K.R. Pragmatic development in a second language. Lang. Learn. 2002, 52 (Suppl. 1), 362. [Google Scholar]
- Roever, C. Testing of second language pragmatics: Past and future. Lang. Test. 2011, 28, 463–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phakiti, A. A closer look at the relationship of cognitive and metacognitive strategy use to EFL reading achievement test performance. Lang. Test. 2003, 20, 26–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Purpura, J.E. An Analysis of the Relationships between Test Takers’ Cognitive and Metacognitive Strategy Use and Second Language Test Performance. Lang. Learn. 1997, 47, 289–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Purpura, J.E. Investigating the effects of strategy use and second language test performance with high- and low-ability test-takers: A structural equation modeling approach. Lang. Test. 1998, 15, 333–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N. L2 Learners’ Strategic Mental Processes during a Listening Test. JALT J. 2001, 23, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spolsky, B.; Bachman, L.F. Fundamental Considerations in Language Testing. Mod. Lang. J. 1991, 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bachman, L.F.; Palmer, A.S. Language Testing in Practice: Designing and Developing Useful Language Tests; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Roever, C.; Wang, S.; Brophy, S. Learner background factors and learning of second language pragmatics. IRAL Int. Rev. Appl. Linguist. Lang. Teach. 2014, 52, 377–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kasper, G.; Schmidt, R. Developmental Issues in Interlanguage Pragmatics. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 1996, 18, 149–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K. Exploring the Interlanguage of Interlanguage Pragmatics: A Research Agenda for Acquisitional Pragmatics. Lang. Learn. 1999, 49, 677–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, M.; Liddicoat, A.J. The development of comprehension in interlanguage pragmatics: The case of request strategies in English. Aust. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 2002, 25, 19–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N. Development of Speed and Accuracy in Pragmatic Comprehension in English as a Foreign Language. TESOL Q. 2007, 41, 313–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N. Pragmatic Comprehension in Japanese as a Foreign Language. Mod. Lang. J. 2008, 92, 558–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K.; Dörnyei, Z. Do Language Learners Recognize Pragmatic Violations? Pragmatic versus Grammatical Awareness in Instructed L2 Learning. TESOL Q. 1998, 32, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Niezgoda, K.; Carsten, R. Pragmatic and grammatical awareness: A function of the learning environment? In Pragmatics in Language Teaching; Rose, K.R., Kasper, G., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 63–79. [Google Scholar]
- Carsten, R. Testing ESL Pragmatics: Development and Validation of a Web-Based Assessment Battery; Peter Lang: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Yamanaka, J.E. Effects of proficiency and length of residence on the pragmatic comprehension of Japanese ESL learners. Second Lang. Stud. 2003, 22, 107–175. [Google Scholar]
- Xu, W.; Case, R.E.; Wang, Y. Pragmatic and grammatical competence, length of residence, and overall L2 proficiency. System 2009, 37, 205–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N. The Effect of L2 Proficiency and Study-Abroad Experience on Pragmatic Comprehension. Lang. Learn. 2011, 61, 904–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takahashi, S. Pragmalinguistic Awareness: Is it Related to Motivation and Proficiency? Appl. Linguist. 2005, 26, 90–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K.; Bastos, M.-T. Proficiency, length of stay, and intensity of interaction and the acquisition of conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. Intercult. Pragmat. 2011, 8, 347–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.-S. Chinese learners’ cognitive processes in writing email requests to faculty. System 2015, 52, 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, A.D.; Olshtain, E. The Production of Speech Acts by EFL Learners. TESOL Q. 1993, 27, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Félix-Brasdefer, J.C. Perceptions of refusals to invitations: Exploring the minds of foreign language learners. Lang. Aware. 2008, 17, 195–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taguchi, N. An application of relevance theory to the analysis of L2 interpretation processes: The comprehension of indirect replies. IRAL Int. Rev. Appl. Linguist. Lang. Teach. 2002, 40, 151–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harvey, S.; Schegloff, E.A. Opening and Closing. Semiotica 1973, 8, 289–327. [Google Scholar]
- Canale, M. From communicative competence to language pedagogy. In Language and Communication; Richards, J.C., Schmidt, R.W., Eds.; Longman: London, UK, 1983; pp. 2–27. [Google Scholar]
- Canale, M.; Swain, M. Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Appl. Linguist. 1980, 1, 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carsten, R. Difficulty and practicality in tests of interlanguage pragmatics. In Studying Speaking to Inform Second Language Learning; Boxer, D., Cohen, A.D., Eds.; Multilingual Matters: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2004; pp. 283–301. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, A.D. Feedback on writing: The use of verbal report. SSLA 1991, 13, 133–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, J. Developing a pragmatics test for Chinese EFL learners. Lang. Test. 2007, 24, 391–415. [Google Scholar]
- Elo, S.; Kyngäs, H. The qualitative content analysis process. J. Adv. Nurs. 2008, 62, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stemler, S.E. A comparison of consensus, consistency, and measurement approaches to estimating interrater reliability. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 2004, 9, 4. [Google Scholar]
- Bouton, L.F. Can NNS skill in interpreting implicature in American English be improved through explicit instruction? A pilot study. In Pragmatics and Language Learning Monograph Series; Bouton, L.F., Kachuru, Y., Eds.; Division of English as an International Language, University of Illinois: Urbana-Champaign, IL, USA, 1994; Volume 5, pp. 88–109. [Google Scholar]
- Taguchi, N.; Carsten, R. Second Language Pragmatics; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Roever, C. What learners get for free: Learning of routine formulae in ESL and EFL environments. Elt J. 2011, 66, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K. Awareness of meaning of conventional expressions in second-language pragmatics. Lang. Aware. 2013, 23, 41–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K. Conventional Expressions as a Pragmalinguistic Resource: Recognition and Production of Conventional Expressions in L2 Pragmatics. Lang. Learn. 2009, 59, 755–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Al-Ali, M.N.; Alawneh, R. Linguistic mitigating devices in American and Jordanian students’ requests. Intercult. Pragmat. 2010, 7, 311–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goffman, E. Replies and responses. Lang. Soc. 1976, 5, 257–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geis, M.L.; Cole, P.; Morgan, J. Syntax and Semantics Volume 3: Speech Acts. Mod. Lang. J. 1976, 60, 301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harvey, S.; Schegloff, E.A.; Jefferson, G. A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 1974, 50, 696–735. [Google Scholar]
- Sperber, D.; Deirdre, W. Relevance: Communication and Cognition, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, D. Relevance theory. In Oxford Handbook of Pragmatics; Huang, Y., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 79–100. [Google Scholar]
- Coulmas, F. Introduction: Conversational Routine. In Rasmus Rask Studies in Pragmatic Linguistics, Volume 2, Conversational Routine; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1981; pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E.; Dulay, H.; Burt, M.; Krashen, S. Language Two. Mod. Lang. J. 1983, 67, 273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wray, A. Formulaic sequences in second language teaching: Principle and practice. Appl. Linguist. 2000, 21, 463–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Derakhshan, A.; Arabmofrad, A. The Impact of Instruction on the Pragmatic Comprehension of Speech Acts of Apology, Request, and Refusal among Iranian Intermediate EFL Learners. Engl. Teach. Learn. 2018, 42, 75–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bardovi-Harlig, K.; Vellenga, H.E. The effect of instruction on conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. System 2012, 40, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
N | Mean | Median | SD | Max | Min | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
High proficiency | 33 | 92.4 | 90 | 7.92 | 100 | 80 |
Low proficiency | 41 | 42.6 | 43 | 1.48 | 45 | 40 |
Strategies | Description | Example | |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Sociopragmatics | Considering social power, social distance, ranking of imposition or level of directness/politeness between interlocutors in the exchange. | For this question, I think C is the best because it sounds more polite. (L1) |
2 | Hearer’s response | Attending to the hearer’s response in the exchange. | For Q5, I chose B because Jack replies “not very long”, In B, Sally asks “Have you been waiting long?” That’s why it’s B. (H6) |
3 | Relevance | Judging the relevance of the interlocutor’s utterances in the exchange. | I think A is correct because Brian doesn’t answer Felicity’s question about his raise but shifts to coffee instead. (H4) |
4 | Keyword/key phrase | Focusing on a particular word/phrase in the exchange. | The answer is A. The word well-typed shows that Tanya doesn’t like Derek’s essay. (H3) |
5 | Life experience/world knowledge | Referring to the learner’s life experience or knowledge of the world. | Normally, August is summertime in the northern hemisphere. So, undoubtedly, summer mornings shouldn’t be cold. So C is the answer. (H5) |
6 | Amount of information | Attending to sufficiency of the information given by the interlocutors in the exchange. | For Q2, I picked D because besides apologizing to Sean, Ella offers a way of repayment, but other options don’t mention repayment. (H6) |
7 | Intuition | Appealing to the learner’s intuitive judgment. | For Q20, I chose D. I can’t say why. I just think D is better. (L7) |
8. | Multiple strategies | Using more than one strategy from 1 to 7 for a single item. | For Q2, I chose D. In A, although she has apologized to Jean, she doesn’t provide a remedy (coded as amount of information)…C is too direct (coded as sociopragmatics) (H10). |
High-Proficiency | Low-Proficiency | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | n | M | SD | n | t | df | Sig. | |
Overall | 22.8 | 4.40 | 33 | 12.8 | 3.53 | 41 | 10.89 | 72 | 0.000 |
Speech acts | 8.33 | 2.19 | 33 | 4.29 | 1.52 | 41 | 9 | 55 | 0.000 |
Implicatures | 7.64 | 2.12 | 33 | 3.51 | 1.76 | 41 | 9.143 | 72 | 0.000 |
Routines | 6.94 | 1.94 | 33 | 4.90 | 1.91 | 41 | 4.536 | 72 | 0.000 |
High Proficiency | Low Proficiency | ||
---|---|---|---|
Speech acts | |||
Apology | 78.2% (97/124) | 42.6% (70/164) | |
Request | 51.6% (67/124) | 26.2% (43/164) | |
Refusal | 89.5% (111/124) | 39.6% (65/164) | |
Implicatures | |||
Idiosyncratic | 68.9% (182/264) | 34.1% (112/328) | |
Formulaic | 52.2% (69/132) | 20.7% (34/164) | |
Routines | |||
Situational | 58.5% (116/198) | 39.8% (98/246) | |
Functional | 38.6% (51/132) | 36.5% (60/164) | |
Adjacency pairs | 59% (39/66) | 51.2% (42/82) |
SP | INT | HR | REL | KWD | AoI | LW | MS | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
H | freq. /p.c. | 10 (50%) | 13 (12.4%) | 19 (47.5%) | 32 (66.7%) | 3 (60.0%) | 7 (87.5%) | 63 (58.9%) | 69 (69.7%) |
L | freq. /p.c. | 10 (50%) | 92 (87.6%) | 21 (52.5%) | 16 (33.3%) | 2 (40.0%) | 1 (12.5%) | 44 (41.1%) | 30 (30.3%) |
a.s.r. | 0.0 | 8.9 | 0.4 | 2.4 | 0.4 | 2.1 | 2.0 | 4.7 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tai, H.-Y.; Chen, Y.-S. The Effects of L2 Proficiency on Pragmatic Comprehension and Learner Strategies. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 174. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11040174
Tai H-Y, Chen Y-S. The Effects of L2 Proficiency on Pragmatic Comprehension and Learner Strategies. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(4):174. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11040174
Chicago/Turabian StyleTai, Hsuan-Yu, and Yuan-Shan Chen. 2021. "The Effects of L2 Proficiency on Pragmatic Comprehension and Learner Strategies" Education Sciences 11, no. 4: 174. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11040174
APA StyleTai, H. -Y., & Chen, Y. -S. (2021). The Effects of L2 Proficiency on Pragmatic Comprehension and Learner Strategies. Education Sciences, 11(4), 174. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11040174