An Early Intervention in Gestural Communication in Chilean Children from Psychosocially At-Risk Backgrounds and Its Impact on Language Skills at 18 Months Old
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Spontaneous Development of Gestures in Early Infancy
1.2. Intentional Development of Symbolic Gestural Communication
1.3. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design
2.2. Participants
2.3. Instruments
2.4. Procedures
2.5. Intervention
2.6. Data Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive and Comparative Analyses before the Intervention
3.2. Comparative Analyses between the IG and GC after the Intervention
3.3. Analysis of the Intervention’s Effect for the Adequate and At-Risk Groups
4. Discussion
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Acredolo, Linda, and Susan Goodwyn. 1988. Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59: 450–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acredolo, Linda, and Susan Goodwyn. 1990. Sign language in babies: The significance of symbolic gesturing for understanding language development. In Annals of Child Development. Edited by Ross Vasta. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, vol. 7, pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar]
- Acredolo, Linda, and Susan Goodwyn. 2000. The Long-Term Impact of Symbolic Gesturing during Infancy on IQ al Age 8. Paper presented at the Meetings of the International Society for Infant Studies, Brighton, UK, July 16–19. [Google Scholar]
- Acredolo, Linda, and Susan Goodwyn. 2001. Los gestos del bebé. Barcelona: Oniro. [Google Scholar]
- Agencia de Calidad de la Educación. 2023. SIMCE. Resultados Educativos 2022 4to básico y II medio; Santiago: Gobierno de Chile. Available online: https://s3.amazonaws.com/archivos.agenciaeducacion.cl/PPT+Conferencia+Prensa+Simce+2022+14+junio.pdf (accessed on 18 December 2023).
- Albers, Craig A., and Adam J. Grieve. 2007. Test Review: Bayley, N. 2006. “Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development—Third Edition”. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. Psychoeducational Assessment 25: 180–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, Sarah S. 2016. The Effect of Baby Sign on Early Language Development for “At-Risk” Populations. Culminating Projects in Communication Sciences and Disorders 3: 1–58. Available online: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/csd_etds/3 (accessed on 18 December 2023).
- Bakken, Linda, Nola Brown, and Barry Downing. 2017. Early childhood education: The long-term benefits. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 31: 255–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, Elizabeth, Laura Benigni, Inge Bretherton, Laura Camaioni, and Virginia Volterra. 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bayley, Nancy. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd ed. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Behrman, Jere, David Bravo, and Sergio Urzúa. 2010. Encuesta Longitudinal de la Primera Infancia: Aspectos metodológicos y primeros resultados. Santiago: Department of Economics, Micro Data Center, University of Chile. [Google Scholar]
- Bermeosolo, Jaime. 2000. Psicología del lenguaje. Santiago de Chile: Ediciones Universidad Católica de Chile. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks-Gunn, Jeanne, Flavio Cunha, Greg Duncan, James J. Heckman, and Aaron Sojourner. 2006. A Reanalysis of the IHDP Program. Evanston: Infant Health and Development Program, Northwestern University, unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Cadime, Irene, Carla Silva, Sandra Santos, Iolanda Ribeiro, and Fernanda L. Viana. 2017. The interrelatedness between infants’ communicative gestures and lexicon size: A longitudinal study. Infant Behavior and Development 48: 88–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camaioni, Luigia, Tiziana Aureli, Francesca Bellagamba, and Alan Fogel. 2003. A longitudinal examination of the transition to symbolic communication in the second year of life. Infant and Child Development 12: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Capirci, Olga, Jana M. Iverson, Elena Pizzuto, and Virginia Volterra. 1996. Gestures and words during the transition to two word speech. Journal of Child Language 23: 645–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carneiro, Pedro, and James Heckman. 2003. Human capital policy. In Inequality in America: What Role for Human Capital Policies? Edited by James Heckman, Alan B. Krueger and Benjamin M. Friedman. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 77–239. [Google Scholar]
- Choi, Boin, Ran Wei, and Meredith L. Rowe. 2021. Show, give, and point gestures across infancy differentially predict language development. Developmental Psychology 57: 851–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colombani, Arianna, Amanda Saksida, Francesco Pavani, and Eva Orzan. 2023. Symbolic and deictic gestures as a tool to promote parent-child communication in the context of hearing loss: A systematic review. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology 165: 111421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Congdon, Eliza L., Miriam A. Novack, Neon Brooks, Naureen Hemani-Lopez, Lucy O’Keefe, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2017. Better together: Simultaneous presentation of speech and gesture in math instruction supports generalization and retention. Learning and Instruction 50: 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cunha, Flavio, and James Heckman. 2007. The technology of skill formation. American Economic Review 97: 31–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daniels, Marilyn. 1996. Seeing language: The effect overtime of sign language on vocabulary development in early childhood education. Child Study Journal 26: 193–208. [Google Scholar]
- Doyle, Lewis, Matthew J. Easterbrook, and Peter R. Harris. 2023. Roles of socioeconomic status, ethnicity and teacher beliefs in academic grading. British Journal of Educational Psychology 93: 91–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Farkas, Chamarrita. 2007a. Comunicación gestual en la infancia temprana: Una revisión de su desarrollo, relación con el lenguaje e implicancias de su intervención. Psykhe 16: 107–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farkas, Chamarrita. 2007b. Desarrollo de la comunicación gestual: Estudio de un caso. Summa Psicológica 4: 3–15. Available online: https://biblat.unam.mx/hevila/SummapsicologicaUST/2007/vol4/no1/1.pdf (accessed on 18 December 2023).
- Farkas, Chamarrita. 2009. Gestos que hablan. Santiago: Ediciones Universidad Católica. [Google Scholar]
- Farkas, Chamarrita. 2011. Inventario del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas McArthur-Bates CDI: Propuesta de una versión abreviada. Universitas Psychologica 10: 245–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farkas, Chamarrita, Carolina Álvarez, María del Pilar Cuellar, Elena Avello, Diana M. Gómez, and Pablo Pereira. 2020. Mothers’ competence profiles and their relation to language and socioemotional development in Chilean children at 12 and 30 months. Infant Behavior and Development 59: 101443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Filp, Johanna. 1995. Éxito y fracaso escolar en sectores pobres: Cómo piensan y actúan las Profesoras. In Educación y Pobreza. Edited by Ximena Sánchez, Francisco Fernández and Carlos Amtmann. Valparaíso: UPLACED. [Google Scholar]
- Fink, Günther, Dana C. McCoy, and Aisha Yousafzai. 2020. Contextual and socioeconomic variation in early motor and language development. Archives of Disease in Childhood 105: 421–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, Evelyn L. 2017. A systematic review and meta-analysis of predictors of expressive-language outcomes among late talkers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 60: 2935–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitzpatrick, Elizabeth M., Jonelle Thibert, Viviane Grandpierre, and J. Cyne Johnston. 2014. How HANDy are baby signs? A systematic review of the impact of gestural communication on typically developing, hearing infants under the age of 36 months. First Language 34: 486–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 1999. The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 419–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2002. Constructing communication by hand. Cognitive Development 17: 1385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodwyn, Susan, and Linda Acredolo. 1998. Encouraging symbolic gestures: A new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech. New Directions for Child Development 79: 61–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goodwyn, Susan, Linda Acredolo, and Catherine Brown. 2000. Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24: 81–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Góngora, Ximena, and Chamarrita Farkas. 2009. Infant sign language program effects on synchronic mother–infant interactions. Infant Behavior and Development. 32: 216–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holler, Judith, Heather Shovelton, and Geoffrey Beattie. 2009. Do iconic hand gestures really contribute to the communication of semantic information in a face-to-face context? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 33: 73–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iverson, Jana M., Olga Capirci, and Cristina Caselli. 1994. From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development 9: 23–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson-Maldonado, Donna, Donna Thal, Larry Fenson, Virginia A. Marchman, Tyler Newton, and Barbara Conboy. 2003. MacArthur Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Communicativas: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Kang, Seokmin, and Barbara Tversky. 2016. From hands to minds: Gestures promote understanding. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 1: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kelly, Spencer D. 2001. Broadening the units of analysis in communication: Speech and nonverbal behaviors in pragmatic comprehension. Journal of Child Language 28: 325–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kendon, Adam. 2004. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- King, Tracy M., Leon A. Rosenberg, Loretta Fuddy, Elizabeth McFarlane, Calvin Sia, and Anne K. Duggan. 2005. Prevalence and early identification of language delays among at-risk three year olds. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 26: 293–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- LLECE. 2008. Los aprendizajes de los estudiantes de América Latina y el Caribe. Resumen ejecutivo del primer reporte de resultados del Segundo Estudio Regional Comparativo y Explicativo. Santiago de Chile: UNECO–OREALC. [Google Scholar]
- Mavilidi, Myrto-Foteini, Anthony D. Okely, Paul Chandler, Dylan P. Cliff, and Fred Paas. 2015. Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children’s foreign language vocabulary learning. Educational Psychology Review 27: 413–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKean, Cristina, Sheena Reilly, Edith L. Bavin, Lesley Bretherton, Eyleen Cini, Laura Conway, Fallon Cook, Patricia Eadie, Margot Prior, Melissa Wake, and et al. 2017. Language outcomes at 7 years: Early predictors and co-occurring difficulties. Pediatrics 139: e20161684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moore, Rosario, Linda Acredolo, and Susan Goodwyn. 2001. Symbolic Gesturing and Joint Atention: Partners in Facilitating Verbal Development. Paper presented at the Biennial Mettings of Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN, USA, April 19–22. [Google Scholar]
- Mueller, Vanessa, Amanda Sepulveda, and Sarai Rodriguez. 2014. The effects of baby sign training on child development. Early Child Development and Care 184: 1178–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Namy, Laura L., and Sandra R. Waxman. 1998. Words and gestures: Infants’ interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development 69: 295–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nicoladis, Elena, Rachel I. Mayberry, and Fred Genesee. 1999. Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35: 514–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nippold, Marilyn A. 2016. Later Language Development: School-Age Children, Adolescents, and Young Adults. Austin: PRO-ED, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Novack, Miriam A., Susan Goldin-Meadow, and Amanda L. Woodward. 2015. Learning from gesture: How early does it happen? Cognition 142: 138–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- OECD. 2018. Chile Should Use Upturn to Address Low Productivity and High Inequality. Available online: https://web–archive.oecd.org/2018-02-26/473843-chile-should-use-upturn-to-address-low-productivity-and-high-inequality.htm (accessed on 18 December 2023).
- OECD–UNESCO. 2007. PISA 2006: Science Competencies for Tomorrow’s World: Volume 1: Analyses. Paris: PISA, OECD Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Premack, David, and Guy Woodruff. 1978. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4: 515–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radford, Luis. 2009. Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings. Educational Studies in Mathematics 70: 111–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robutti, Ornella, Cristina Sabena, Christina Krause, Carlotta Soldano, and Ferdinando Arzarello. 2022. Gestures in mathematics thinking and learning. In Handbook of Cognitive Mathematics. Edited by Marcel Danesi. Cham: Springer, pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodrigo, María José, Angela González, Manuel Ato, Guacimara Rodríguez, Manuel de Vega, and Mercedes Muñetón. 2006. Co-development of child-mother gestures over the second and the third years. Infant and Child Development 15: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Romano, Mollie K., and Kelly S. Windsor. 2020. Increasing deictic gesture use to support the language development of toddlers from high poverty backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 50: 129–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith L., Ran Wei, and Virginia C. Salo. 2022. Early gesture predicts later language development. In Gesture in Language: Development across the Lifespan. Edited by Aliyah Morgenstern and Susan Goldin-Meadow. De Gruyter Mouton: American Psychological Association, pp. 93–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith L., Şeyda Özçalışkan, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2008. Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language 28: 182–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schady, Norbert, Jere Behrman, Maria Caridad Araujo, Rodrigo Azuero, Raquel Bernal, David Bravo, Florencia Lopez-Boo, Karen Macours, Daniela Marshall, Christina Paxson, and et al. 2015. Wealth gradients in early childhood cognitive development in five Latin American countries. Journal of Human Resources 50: 446–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schady, Norbert, Sebastian Galiani, and André P. Souza. 2006. Early childhood development in Latin America and the Caribbean [with comments]. Economía 6: 185–225. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/20065499 (accessed on 18 December 2023). [CrossRef]
- Schonhaut, Luisa, Paulina Rojas, and Ana María Kaempffer. 2005. Factores de riesgo asociados a déficit del desarrollo psicomotor en preescolares de nivel socioeconómico bajo. Comuna urbano rural, Región Metropolitana, 2003. Revista Chilena de Pediatría 76: 589–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sneyers, Elian, and Paul Mahieu. 2020. Bias in primary school teachers’ expectations of students?: A study of general and specific bias towards SES, ethnicity and gender. Studia Paedagogica 25: 71–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sparaci, Laura, and Virginia Volterra. 2017. Hands shaping communication: From gestures to signs. In The Hand. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics. Edited by Marta Bertolaso and Nicola Di Stefano. Cham: Springer, vol. 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vallotton, Claire. 2005. Effects of symbolic gestures as a caregiving tool: Children’s social and language development and mother’s perceptions and behavior. Dissertation Abstracts International 659: 4877B. [Google Scholar]
- Vallotton, Claire D. 2012. Infant signs as intervention? Promoting symbolic gestures for preverbal children in low-income families supports responsive parent–child relationships. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 27: 401–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volterra, Virginia, Olga Capirci, Pasquale Rinaldi, and Laura Sparaci. 2018. From action to spoken and signed language through gesture: Some basic developmental issues for a discussion on the evolution of the human language-ready brain. Interaction Studies 19: 216–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volterra, Virginia, Maria Cristina Caselli, Olga Capirci, and Elena Pizzuto. 2005. Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Beyond Nature-Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates. Edited by Michael Tomasello and Dan Isaac Slobin. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 3–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wakefield, Elizabeth M., and Karin H. James. 2015. Effects of learning with gesture on children’s understanding of a new language concept. Developmental Psychology 51: 1105–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Frequency | Percentage | ||
---|---|---|---|
Children’s gender | Male | 39 | 56.5 |
Female | 30 | 43.5 | |
Mothers’ occupational level (n = 65) | Unemployed | 19 | 29.2 |
Studying | 10 | 15.4 | |
Tasks without a technical certification | 18 | 27.7 | |
Jobs with technical certification | 14 | 21.5 | |
Jobs with a university degree | 4 | 6.2 | |
Fathers’ occupational level (n = 60) | Unemployed | 3 | 5.0 |
Studying | 2 | 3.3 | |
Tasks without a technical certification | 36 | 60.0 | |
Jobs with technical certification | 19 | 31.7 | |
Jobs with a university degree | 0 | 0 | |
Father living with the child | No | 29 | 42.0 |
Family SES (n = 62) | Low, not in poverty | 13 | 21.0 |
In poverty, not indigent | 29 | 46.8 | |
Indigent | 20 | 32.2 | |
Children’s risk factors | |||
Problems during pregnancy | Yes | 16 | 23.2 |
Type of delivery | Cesarean | 16 | 23.2 |
Weight at birth | 2.500 or less | 1 | 1.4 |
Gestational age | 36 weeks or less | 9 | 17.6 |
Health issues during the first year | Respiratory problems | 18 | 26.1 |
Min–Max | M (S.D.) | ||
Children’s age (months) | 5–9 | 7.1 (1.03) | |
Mothers’ age (years) | 14–39 | 26.9 (6.73) | |
Fathers’ age (years) | 15–54 | 30.8 (8.17) | |
Number of persons living at home | 1–12 | 4.4 (1.09) |
Total Sample M (S.D.) | IG M (S.D.) | CG M (S.D.) | F (p) | η2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BSID-III: initial assessment | |||||
Receptive language subscale | 9.20 (2.97) | 9.54 (3.15) | 8.75 (2.72) | 1.13 (0.292) | 0.018 |
Expressive language subscale | 8.66 (3.10) | 8.97 (3.12) | 8.25 (3.07) | 0.87 (0.356) | 0.014 |
Total language scale | 17.86 (5.27) | 18.51 (5.75) | 17.00 (4.51) | 1.32 (0.255) | 0.021 |
BSID-III: 18 months | |||||
Receptive language subscale | 9.29 (2.10) | 9.30 (2.16) | 9.28 (2.07) | 0.02 (0.967) | 0.000 |
Expressive language subscale | 8.55 (2.39) | 9.21 (2.37) | 7.68 (2.17) | 6.38 (0.014) | 0.102 |
Total language scale | 17.85 (3.86) | 18.52 (4.12) | 16.96 (3.36) | 2.37 (0.129) | 0.041 |
Growth delta: teachers | |||||
Comprehension of first sentences | 5.22 (8.50) | 7.66 (5.45) | 2.23 (10.55) | 6.39 (0.014) | 0.102 |
Expressive vocabulary | 35.85 (52.02) | 48.03 (40.80) | 20.85 (60.68) | 4.13 (0.047) | 0.069 |
Comprehensive vocabulary | 5.00 (6.36) | 5.88 (5.88) | 3.92 (6.86) | 1.36 (0.249) | 0.024 |
t (p) | d | ||||
Gestural vocabulary report | 24.6 (16.2) | 29.37 (17.25) | 17.05 (11.08) | 3.043 (0.004) | 0.760 |
IG M (S.D.) | CG M (S.D.) | F (p) | η2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
BSID-III: 18 months, adequate group (n = 38) | ||||
Receptive language subscale | 9.95 (1.90) | 9.93 (1.86) | 0.01 (0.978) | 0.000 |
Expressive language subscale | 10.26 (2.13) | 7.21 (1.42) | 21.50 (0.000) | 0.410 |
Total language scale | 20.21 (3.51) | 17.14 (2.32) | 8.08 (0.008) | 0.207 |
BSID-III: 18 months, at-risk group (n = 27) | ||||
Receptive language subscale | 8.43 (2.24) | 8.46 (2.12) | 0.01 (0.977) | 0.000 |
Expressive language subscale | 7.79 (1.93) | 8.27 (2.83) | 0.26 (0.614) | 0.011 |
Total language scale | 16.21 (3.85) | 16.73 (4.47) | 0.10 (0.761) | 0.004 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Farkas, C. An Early Intervention in Gestural Communication in Chilean Children from Psychosocially At-Risk Backgrounds and Its Impact on Language Skills at 18 Months Old. Languages 2024, 9, 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040146
Farkas C. An Early Intervention in Gestural Communication in Chilean Children from Psychosocially At-Risk Backgrounds and Its Impact on Language Skills at 18 Months Old. Languages. 2024; 9(4):146. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040146
Chicago/Turabian StyleFarkas, Chamarrita. 2024. "An Early Intervention in Gestural Communication in Chilean Children from Psychosocially At-Risk Backgrounds and Its Impact on Language Skills at 18 Months Old" Languages 9, no. 4: 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040146
APA StyleFarkas, C. (2024). An Early Intervention in Gestural Communication in Chilean Children from Psychosocially At-Risk Backgrounds and Its Impact on Language Skills at 18 Months Old. Languages, 9(4), 146. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9040146