Gradual Vowel Epenthesis in Urban Hijazi Arabic
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. High Vowel Deletion
Light | CV | [ba.ɡa.rah] ‘a cow’ | |
Heavy | CVC, CVV | [mak.tab] ‘an office’ | [kaa.tib] ‘a writer’ |
Superheavy | CVCC, CVVC | [ʔa.kalt] ‘I ate’ | [faa.nuus] ‘a lantern’ |
a. | /nidim + u/ | [ˈnid.mu] | ‘they felt remorse’ |
b. | /fihim + at/ | [ˈfih.mat] | ‘she understood’ |
c. | /simiʕ + ak/ | [ˈsim.ʕak] | ‘he heard you (M.SG)’ |
a. | /saħab + u/ | [ˈsa.ħa.bu] | ‘they pulled’ |
b. | /katab + at/ | [ˈka.ta.bat] | ‘she wrote’ |
c. | /madaħ + ak/ | [ˈma.da.ħak] | ‘he praised you (M.SG)’ |
/a/ | [Colour], [Open] |
↓ | |
/i/ or /u/ | [Colour] |
↓ | |
[ə] | [ ] (Bare root node) |
↓ | |
∅ | Vowel deletion completed |
(i) | /a/→[i] or [u]→[ə]→∅ | (three-step derivations, then convergence) |
(ii) | /i/ or /u/→[ə]→∅ | (two-step derivations, then convergence) |
(iii) | [ə]→∅ | (one-step derivation, then convergence) |
a. *VWEAK |
Assign a violation mark to every vowel in the Weak context that bears the features [Colour] and/or [Open]. |
b. *[ ] |
Assign a violation mark for every instance of the featureless vowel [ə]. |
*VWEAK | >> | *[ ] | >> | Max |
- (i)
- HS analysis of /fihim + at/ → [fih.mat] ‘she understood’—Step 1
/fihim + at/ *VWEAK *[ ] Max a. → fi.hə.mat 1 1 b. fi.hi.mat 1W L L - (ii)
- HS analysis of /fihim + at/ → [fih.mat] ‘she understood’—Step 2
fi.hə.mat *VWEAK *[ ] Max a. → fih.mat 1 b. fi.hə.mat 1W L - (iii)
- HS analysis of /fihim + at/ → [fih.mat] ‘she understood’—Step 3 (convergence)
fih.mat *VWEAK *[ ] Max a. → fih.mat b. fi.hə.mat 1W
/katab + at/ | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max |
a. → ka.ta.bat | 1 | ||
b. ka.ti.bat | 1 | 1W |
3. Vowel Epenthesis in UHA
3.1. High Vowel Anaptyxis
(i) | a. | /ʔism/ | → | [ʔi.sim] | ‘name’ |
b. | /ħibr/ | → | [ħi.bir] | ‘ink’ | |
c. | /ʔidn/ | → | [ʔi.din] | ‘ear’ | |
d. | /tʕifl/ | → | [tʕi.fil] | ‘baby’ | |
(ii) | a. | /gutʕn/ | → | [gu.tʕun] | ‘cotton’ |
b. | /rubʕ/ | → | [ru.buʕ] | ‘quarter’ | |
c. | /∫uʁl/ | → | [∫u.ʁul] | ‘work’ | |
d. | /∫ukr/ | → | [∫u.kur] | ‘gratitude’ | |
(iii) | a. | /ʔakl/ | → | [ʔa.kil] | ‘food’ |
b. | /madħ/ | → | [ma.diħ] | ‘praise’ | |
c. | /tamr/ | → | [ta.mur] | ‘dates’ | |
d. | /sʕabʁ/ | → | [sʕa.buʁ] | ‘dye’ |
- (i)
- HS analysis of /ʔakl/ → [ʔa.kil] ‘food’—Step 1
/ʔakl/ SPP *VWEAK *[ ] Max DEP a. → ʔa.kəl 1 1 b. ʔakl 1W L L - (ii)
- HS analysis of /ʔakl/ → [ʔa.kil] ‘food’—Step 2
ʔa.kəl SPP *VWEAK *[ ] Max DEP a. → ʔa.kil 1 b. ʔakl 1W 1W L c. ʔa.kəl 1W L - (iii)
- HS analysis of /ʔakl/ → [ʔa.kil] ‘food’—Step 3 (convergence)
ʔa.kil SPP *VWEAK *[ ] Max DEP a. → ʔa.kil b. ʔa.kal 1W c. ʔa.kəl 1W 1W
3.2. Low Vowel Epenthesis
(i) | a. | /bint + na/ | → | [bin.ta.na] | ‘our daughter’ |
b. | /ʔuχt + ha/ | → | [ʔuχ.ta.ha] | ‘her sister’ | |
c. | /beet + hum/ | → | [bee.ta.hum] | ‘their house’ | |
d. | /faanuus + kum/ | → | [faa.nuu.sa.kum] | ‘your pl. lantern’ | |
(ii) | a. | /∫uft + ha/ | → | [∫uf.ta.ha] | ‘I saw her’ |
b. | /darrast + na/ | → | [dar.ras.ta.na] | ‘you (M.SG) taught us’ | |
c. | / aat + kum/ | → | [ aa.ta.kum] | ‘she came to you pl.’ | |
d. | /foog + hum/ | → | [foo.ga.hum] | ‘above them’ |
/bint + na/ | SYL-MX(μμ) | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a. → bin.tə.na | 1 | 1 | |||
b. bint.na | 1W | L | L |
bin.tə.na | SYL-MX(μμ) | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a. bin.ti.na | 1W | L | 1W | ||
b. bin.tə.na | 1 |
a. DEP-UO |
Every segment of the output has a correspondent in the underlying representation. |
b. *[ ] |
Assign a violation mark for every instance of the featureless vowel [ə]. |
/bint + na/ bin.tə.na | SYL-MX(μμ) | DEP-UO &*[ ] | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a. → bin.ti.na | 1 | 1 | ||||
b. bin.tə.na | 1W | L | 1W | L | ||
c. bint.na | 1W | L | 1W | L |
/bint + na/ bin.ti.na | SYL-MX(μμ) | DEP-UO &*[ ] | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a. bin.ta.na | 1 | 1W | ||||
b. bin.ti.na | 1 |
a. DEP-UO[V+HIGH] |
Every high vowel in the output has a correspondent in the underlying representation. |
b. *VWEAK |
Assign a violation mark to every vowel in the Weak context that bears the features [Colour] and/or [Open]. |
/bint + na/ bin.ti.na | SYL-MX(μμ) | DEP-UO &*[ ] | DEP-UO[V+HIGH] &*VWEAK | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a.→ bin.ta.na | 1 | 1 | |||||
b. bin.ti.na | 1W | 1 | L | ||||
c. bin.tə.na | 1W | L | 1W | 1W | L |
/bint + na/ bin.ta.na | SYL-MX(μμ) | DEP-UO &*[ ] | DEP-UO[V+HIGH] &*VWEAK | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP |
a.→ bin.ta.na | 1 | ||||||
b. bin.ti.na | 1W | 1 | 1W |
SYL-MX(μμ) | DEP-UO &*[ ] | DEP-UO[V+HIGH] &*VWEAK | *VWEAK | *[ ] | Max | DEP | |
Faithful bint.na | 1 | ||||||
Step 1 bin.tə.na | 1 | 1 | 1 | ||||
Step 2 bin.ti.na | 1 | 1 | 1 | ||||
Step 3 bin.ta.na | 1 | 1 | |||||
Step 4 bin.ta.na | convergence |
4. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | The vowels /eː/ and /oː/ are excluded from the vowel inventory of UHA because they are treated as underlying /aj/ and /aw/, respectively. |
2 | In such dialects, only high vowels are deleted when they occur in the Weak context. As shown in (2), a low vowel in differential dialects is not deleted in that environment. However, in non-differential dialects, such as Yamani, all three vowels are deleted in that environment. |
3 | Comparative tableaux (Prince 2002) illustrate the discussion throughout. The arrow shows the most harmonic candidate, and the integers represent the number of violations. Less harmonic candidates are assessed for each constraint. Specifically, a (W) indicates a constraint favouring the winner, and an (L) indicates a constraint favouring the losing candidate being assessed. |
4 | In (7 i), another candidate is possible. This candidate gains featural structure, rather than loses it *fi.ha.mat. As clarified in more detail in the subsequent discussion, such a candidate will also violate *VWEAK. In addition, it will violate the faithfulness constraint DEP, as structure is added. |
5 | As assumed in the subsequent discussion, a candidate such as (7 iii b) also violates the faithfulness constraint DEP, as there is an instance of epenthesis. |
6 | As discussed at some length in Jarrah (1993), there are some forms where the epenthetic vowel is [a], as in /baħr/ → [ba.ħar] ‘sea’. These are conditioned forms, however. The first consonant of the final underlying consonant cluster is always a guttural [+pharyngeal]. Consequently, Jarrah (1993) assumed “that the pharyngeal vowel /a/ always occurs with these pharyngeals” (Jarrah 1993, p. 107). |
7 | The forms in (9 i and ii), in which the preceding vowel is either /i/ or /μ/, might be accounted for as instances of vowel copy. This is an interesting proposition but is beyond the scope of the paper. What is also worthy of further consideration is the variation in the quality of the epenthetic high vowel in (9 iii), /i/ or /μ/. Although this alternation is observed in Jarrah (1993), no phonetic or phonological explanation is offered, leaving it for further research. |
8 | The constraint SPP predicts that the final consonant in each of the forms in (9), being a sonority peak, is a syllable peak. Consequently, GEN is expected to produce a minimally changed candidate such as [ʔakl̩], with a final syllabic consonant. The proposed account assumes that syllabic consonants, which do not occur in UHA at all, are ruled out independently, so they will not be considered in the tableaux. |
9 | The proposed account assumes that a candidate such as *ʔak.lə is ruled out by an alignment constraint such as ALIGN-RIGHT. |
10 | Breaking up a quadri-consonantal cluster will render the epenthetic vowel in a closed syllable, /gult + l + hum/ → [gul.tal.hum] ‘I told them’. Such consonantal clusters only occur when a verb is followed by the dative suffix, /l/ or /b/, which necessarily requires an object suffix to follow. This special and conditioned case, where an epenthetic low vowel is inserted into a closed syllable, can be examined separately on its own merits. |
11 | |
12 | The sad face appears next to the intended winner, and the bomb symbol appears next to the wrongly selected candidate. |
13 | An alternative to the FAITH-UO analysis can benefit from an anti-faithfulness constraint, as formulated in Alderete (2001). Though anti-faithfulness constraints are categorised by McCarthy (2016) as being incompatible with HS, an anti-faithfulness constraint such as ¬ IDENT [ ] will militate against corresponding segments agreeing on the featureless vowel. This will rule out a candidate such as bin.tə.na from Step 2. However, ruling out such a candidate from Step 3 is not possible unless there is a further condition placed on GEN preventing it from generating previous inputs. |
References
- Abu-Mansour, Mahasen. 1987. A Nonlinear Analysis of Arabic Syllabic Phonology, with Special Reference to Makkan. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Florida, Gainsville, FL, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Al-Ani, Salman, and Darlene May. 1978. The phonological structure of the syllable in Arabic. In Readings in Arabic Linguistics. Edited by Salman Al-Ani. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club, pp. 113–26. [Google Scholar]
- Alderete, John. 2001. Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonology 18: 201–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Al-Mohanna, Faisal. 1998. Syllabification and Metrification in Urban Hijazi Arabic: Between Rules and Constraints. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Essex, Colchester, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Al-Mohanna, Faisal. 2010. Positional syllable maximality: Syllabification in Hejazi. Journal of King Saud University–Languages and Translation 22: 47–67. [Google Scholar]
- Bakala, Muhammad Hasan. 1973. The Phonology and Morphology of Meccan Arabic: A Generative Phonological Approach. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Londo, London, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Brame, Michael. 1970. Arabic Phonology: Implication for Phonological Theory and Historical Linguistics. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Broselow, Ellen. 1979. Cairene Arabic syllable structure. Linguistic Analysis 5: 345–82. [Google Scholar]
- Clements, George Nickerson. 1991. Vowel height assimilation in Bantu languages. In BLS 17S: Proceedings of the Special Session on African Language Structures. Edited by Kathleen Hubbard. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistic Society, pp. 25–64. [Google Scholar]
- Clements, George Nickerson. 1997. Berber syllabification: Derivations or constraints? In Derivations and Constraints in Phonology. Edited by Iggy Roca. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 289–330. [Google Scholar]
- Farwaneh, Samira. 1995. Directionality Effects in Arabic Dialect Syllable Structure. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser, Ivy, Coral Hughto, and Megan Somerday. 2016. Faith-UO: Counterfeeding in Harmonic Serialism. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting on Phonology 2014. Washington: Linguistic Society of America. [Google Scholar]
- Hulst, Harry van der. 1989. Atoms of segmental structure: Components, gestures and dependency. Phonology 6: 253–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ingham, Bruce. 1971. Some Characteristics of Meccan Speech. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies 34: 273–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarrah, Mohamed Ali. 1993. The Phonology of Madina Hijazi Arabic: A Non-Linear Analysis. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Essex, Colchester, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Lombardi, Linda. 2003. Markedness and the Typology of Epenthetic Vowels. ROA-578. College Park: University of Maryland. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 1979. Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, Garland Press, New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 2000. Harmonic serialism and parallelism. In Proceedings of the North East Linguistics Society 30. Edited by Masako Hirotani. Amherst: GLSA Publications, pp. 501–24. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 2006. Restraint of analysis. In Wondering at the Natural Fecundity of Things: Essays in Honor of Alan Prince. Edited by Eric Bakovic, Junko Ito and John McCarthy. Santa Cruz: Linguistics Research Center, UC Santa Cruz, pp. 195–219. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 2008. Doing Optimality Theory. Malden and Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 2010. An introduction to Harmonic Serialism. Language and Linguistics Compass 4: 1001–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarthy, John. 2016. The theory and practice of Harmonic Serialism. In Harmonic Grammar and Harmonic Serialism. Edited by John McCarthy and Joe Pater. London: Equinox Publishing Ltd, pp. 47–87. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John. 2019. How to delete. In Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics XXX. Edited by Amel Khalfaoui and Matthew Tucker. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 7–32. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1993a. Generalized Alignment. In Yearbook of Morphology. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 79–153. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1993b. Prosodic Morphology: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction. Technical Report. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John, and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Papers in Optimality Theory. UMOP 18. Edited by Jill Beckman, Laura Dickey and Suzann Urbanczyk. Amherst: GLSA, pp. 249–384. [Google Scholar]
- Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Revision of 1993 Technical Report, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. Malden and Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Prince, Alan. 2002. Arguing optimality. In University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 26: Papers in Optimality Theory II. Edited by Angela Carpenter, Andries Coetzee and Paul de Lacy. Amherst: GLSA, pp. 269–304. [Google Scholar]
- Schane, Sanford. 1984. The fundamentals of particle phonology. Phonology 1: 129–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1981. Epenthesis and degenerate syllables in Cairene Arabic. In Theoretical Issues in the Grammar of the Semitic Languages. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 3. Edited by Hagit Borer and Joseph Aoun. Cambridge: Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, MIT, pp. 111–140. [Google Scholar]
- Smolensky, Paul. 1995. On the Internal Structure of the Constraint Component of UG. ROA-86. Los Angeles: University of California. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, Janet. 2002. The Phonology and Morphology of Arabic. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Al-Mohanna, F.M. Gradual Vowel Epenthesis in Urban Hijazi Arabic. Languages 2021, 6, 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040160
Al-Mohanna FM. Gradual Vowel Epenthesis in Urban Hijazi Arabic. Languages. 2021; 6(4):160. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040160
Chicago/Turabian StyleAl-Mohanna, Faisal M. 2021. "Gradual Vowel Epenthesis in Urban Hijazi Arabic" Languages 6, no. 4: 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040160
APA StyleAl-Mohanna, F. M. (2021). Gradual Vowel Epenthesis in Urban Hijazi Arabic. Languages, 6(4), 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6040160