Psycholinguistic Evidence for Incipient Language Change in Mexican Spanish: The Extension of Differential Object Marking
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Spanish Differential Object Marking
(1) | |||
(a) | María vio | a | Juan. |
María | saw | DOM Juan | |
‘María saw Juan.’ | |||
(b) | María | vio | el programa. |
María | saw | the TV show | |
‘María saw the TV show.’ |
(4) quando dexaron mis fijas en el rrobredo de Corpes CMC 3151 | |
‘when they left my daughters in the oak-forest of Corpes’ |
(5) . . . cuando dejaron a mis hijas en el robledo de Corpes. | |
‘when they left DOM my daughters in the oak-forest of Corpes’ |
(6) Después de conocer mucho a la vida, ya no me interesa el teatro. (Proceso, May 1999) | |
‘After knowing life too much, I am no longer interested in theater.’ |
(7) Para que no nos peleemos, puse a la silla en el medio. (Mexico, spoken Spanish) | |
‘So that we do not fight, I put the chair in the middle.’ |
1.2. Research Questions and Hypotheses
- (1)
- Based on previous studies (Von Heusinger and Kaiser 2005; Bautista Maldonado and Montrul 2019), monolingually raised native speakers of Mexican Spanish were expected to mark categorically animate and specific objects and to show some degree of variation with inanimate objects. Moreover, participants were expected to show more extension of DOM to inanimate objects in the more implicit oral task (narration) than in the more explicit oral task (elicitation).
- (2)
- Participants were expected to show some preference for DOM extension to inanimate objects in the AJT (Bautista Maldonado and Montrul 2019). Therefore, participants were expected to judge sentences with DOM-marked inanimate objects as more acceptable than sentences that omitted DOM with animate objects.
- (3)
- If comprehension precedes production in language variation, as in L1 acquisition (Shipley et al. 1969) and L2 acquisition (Malovrh and Lee 2010), the use of DOM with inanimate objects should be evident in speakers’ sentence processing. Thus, participants were expected to show less grammatical sensitivity with sentences with DOM-marked inanimate objects than with sentences with unmarked animate objects.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Tasks
2.2.1. Oral Tasks
2.2.2. Acceptability Judgment Task
2.2.3. Reading Comprehension Task with Eye-Tracking
(8) El actor liberó al compañero con su llave. | |
‘The actor released his partner with his key.’ | |
¿Qué usó el actor? | |
‘What did the actor use? | |
A) Una llave (B) Unas tijeras | |
‘a key’ ‘a pair of scissors’ |
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. The Oral Production Tasks
3.1.1. Oral Narrative Task
9 | |||||||
(a) | Participant 122: | El | lobo | atacó | Caperucita. | ||
the | wolf | attacked | Little red Riding Hood | ||||
‘The wolf attacked Little red Riding Hood.’ | |||||||
(b) | Participant 130 | Un día | el lobo | al | querer | comerse | Caperucita |
one day | the wolf | prep | want | eat | Little red Riding Hood | ||
‘One day the wolf when wanting to eat Little red Riding Hood.’ | |||||||
(c) | Participant 110: | El | leñador | metió a | las piedras en el | estomago del lobo | |
the | woodcutter | put | DOM the rocks | in the stomach of the wolf | |||
‘The woodcutter put the rocks in the wolf’s stomach.’ |
3.1.2. Oral Elicitation Task
(10) | |
(a) | Participants 100, 103, 105, 110, 122: La mamá abrazó el bebé. |
the mother hugged the baby | |
‘The mother hugged the baby.’ | |
(b) | Participants 103, 111, 128, 130: El niño persiguió el otro niño. |
the boy followed the other boy | |
‘The boy followed the other boy.’ |
11 | ||||||
(a) | Participant 103: | Raquel visitó | a | la ciudad | de | Chicago. |
Raquel visited | DOM | the city | of | Chicago | ||
‘Raquel visited the city of Chicago’ | ||||||
(b) | Participants 120, 124, 125: | Raquel visitó | a | Chicago. | ||
Raquel visited | DOM | Chicago | ||||
Raquel visited Chicago.’ |
12 | |||||
(a) | Participant 100, 126: | Julián tocó | a | la planta | |
Julian touched | DOM | the plant | |||
‘Julian touched the plant.’ | |||||
(b) | Participant 128: | El viejo | llevó | al | paraguas. |
the old man | brought | DOM | the umbrella | ||
The old man brought the umbrella.’ |
3.2. Acceptability Judgment Task
3.3. Reading Comprehension Task with Eye-Tracking
(13) | ‘Kevin said hi to the father in the park of Chicago’ | |||||||
Kevin | salu | dó al/el | padre | en | el | parque | de | |
R 1 | R2 | CR | R4 | R5 | R6 | R7 | R8 |
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | When both the external and the internal argument are inanimate, there can be a potential ambiguity between the subject and the object of the sentence and therefore, DOM is usually used as in: “El ácido corroe al metal”. |
2 | In a previous study (Montrul 2014), we tested verbs that tend to go with animate objects, verbs that tend to go with inanimate objects, and verbs that tend to go with both objects, following Von Heusinger and Kaiser (2007) and no differences in DOM omission were found by type of verb. Therefore, we did not consider different verbs as a variable in the present study. |
3 | Extension refers to the use of DOM with inanimate objects in sentences where the subject is animate. |
4 | For both oral tasks, participants were instructed to use the preterite tense. For the AJT and the reading task, DOM always appeared with a verb in the preterite tense. The reason for this is that when the DOM preposition ‘a’ appears after a verb of the first conjugation in the present indicative, for example Ella visita a la abuelita ‘She visits the grandmother’, the sequence of two [a] sounds (one from the verbal ending and one for the marker) is reduced to one, possibly somewhat lengthened ([a:]), so that the preposition is practically inaudible in speech. In the preterite tense, as in Ella visitó a la abuelita ‘She visited the grandmother’, the vowel is diphthongized with the vowel of the verb ending (/oa/or/ua/). Thus, it is easier to analyze the use of DOM. |
5 | As noted by an anonymous reviewer, some sentences were longer (had more syllables) than others. However, all sentences were between 8 and 9 words in length and were preceded by a prepositional phrase because it is recommended to avoid having the critical, or even the spillover region, at the beginning of a sentence in eyetracking with text tasks. |
6 | Estimated marginals means (emmean) uses regression equations to calculate actual means. |
References
- Agresti, Alan. 2002. Categorical Data Analysis, 2nd ed. Hoboken: Wiley. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21: 435–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aitchison, Jean. 2001. Language Change: Progress or Decay? 3rd ed. Cambridge: University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Alfaraz, Gabriela. 2011. Accusative object marking: A change in progress in Cuban Spanish? Spanish in Context 8: 213–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archibald, John. 2000. Models of L2 phonological acquisition. In Social and Cognitive Factors in Second Language Acquisition: Selected Proceedings of the 1999 Second Language Research Forum. Edited by Bonnie Swierzbin, Frank Morris, Michael E. Anderson, Carol A. Klee and Elaine Tarone. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 125–57. [Google Scholar]
- Arechabaleta Regulez, Begoña. 2020. The Processing of Differential Object Marking by Heritage Speakers of Spanish. In The Acquisition of Differential Object Marking. Trends in Language Acquisition Research. Edited by Alexandru Mardale and Silvina Montrul. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, vol. 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Azcorra, Hugo, and Federico Dickinson. 2020. Introduction. In Culture, Environment and Health in the Yucatán Peninsula: A Human Ecology Perspective. Edited by Hugo Azcorra and Federico Dickinson. Cham: Springer, pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Banks, Marva A. 2004. Semantic Changes in Present-Day English (PDE) Thesis. Senior Thesis, Department of English, Langston University, Langston, OK, USA. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2014. lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Eigen and S4. R Package Version 1.1-7. Available online: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 (accessed on 15 August 2021).
- Bauer, Laurie. 1994. Watching English Change. An Introduction to the Study of Linguistic Change in Standard Englishes in the Twentieth Century. London: Longman. [Google Scholar]
- Baugh, Albert C., and Thomas Cable. 1978. A History of the English Language. London: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bautista Maldonado, Salvador, and Silvina Montrul. 2019. An experimental investigation of differential object marking in Mexican Spanish. Spanish in Context 16: 22–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bossong, Georg. 1991. Differential object marking in romance and beyond. In New Analyses in Romance Lingusitics. Edited by Douglas A. Kibbee and Dieter Wanner. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 143–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chambers, Jack K. 2002. Dialectology. In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. Edited by N. J. Smelser and P. B. Baltes. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Christensen, Rune Haubo B. 2015. Ordinal: Regression Models for Ordinal Data. R Package Version 2015.6-28. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal (accessed on 15 August 2021).
- Clahsen, Harald, and Claudia Felser. 2006. Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 27: 3–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clifton, Charles, Antje S. Meyer, Lee H. Wurm, and Rebecca Treiman. 2013. Language comprehension and production. In Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology, 2nd ed. Experimental Psychology. Edited by Alice F. Healy and Robert W. Proctor. New York: Wiley, vol. 4, pp. 523–47. [Google Scholar]
- Company, Concepción. 2002. El avance diacrónico de la marcación prepositiva en objetos directos inanimados. In Presente y futuro de la lingüística en España. Edited by Alberto Bernabé, José Antonio Berenguer, Margarita Cantarero and José Carlos de Torres Torres. Madrid: SEL, vol. II, pp. 146–54. [Google Scholar]
- Cuza, Alejandro, Lauren Miller, and Mariluz Ortíz. 2016. On the production of differential object marking and wh-question formation in native and nonnative Spanish. Language Acquisition Beyond Parameters: Studies in Honour of Juana M. Liceras 51: 187. [Google Scholar]
- Cuza, Alejandro, Lauren Miller, Rocio Pérez Tattam, and Mariluz Ortiz. 2018. Structure complexity effects and vulnerable domains in child heritage Spanish: The case of Spanish personal a. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 23: 1333–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Czypionka, Anna, and Tanja Kupisch. 2019. (The) polar bears are pink. How (the) Germans interpret (the) definite articles in plural subject DPs. The Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 22: 247–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, Mark. 2002. Un corpus anotado de 100.000.000 palabras del español histórico y moderno. In Revistas - Procesamiento del Lenguaje Natural. Valladolid: Sociedad Española para el Procesamiento del Lenguaje Natural, pp. 21–27. [Google Scholar]
- DeKeyser, Robert. 2003. Implicit and Explicit Learning. In The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Catherine J. Doughty and Micahel H. Long. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, pp. 312–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delille, Karl Heinz. 1970. Die Geschichtliche Entwicklung des Präpositionalen Akkusativs im Portugiesischen. Bonn: Romanisches Seminar. [Google Scholar]
- Eckert, Penelope. 2005. Variation, convention, and social meaning. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Oakland, CA, USA, January 7. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, Rod. 2005. Instructed language learning and task-based teaching. In Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning. Edited by Eli Hinkel. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira, Fernanda, and Benjamin Swets. 2005. The production and comprehension of resumptive pronouns in relative clause "island" contexts. In Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones. Edited by Anne Cutler. Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 263–78. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer, Olga, Anette Rosenbach, and Dieter Stein, eds. 2000. Pathways of Change: Grammaticalization in English. Studies in Language Companion Series; Amsterdam: Benjamins, vol. 53. [Google Scholar]
- Heller, Dieter. 1982. Eye movements in reading. In Cognition and Eye Movements. Edited by Rudolf Groner and Paul Fraisse. Amsterdam: North Holland, pp. 139–54. [Google Scholar]
- Herold, Ruth. 1990. Mechanisms of Merger: The Implementation and Distribution of the Low Back Merger in Eastern Pennsylvania. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Dissertation. [Google Scholar]
- Hulstijn, Jan H. 2005. Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27: 129–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kim, Kitaek, William OGrady ’, and Bonnie D. Schwartz. 2018. Case in heritage Korean. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8: 252–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Labov, William. 1972. Language in the Inner City. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
- Labov, William. 1994. Principles of Linguistic Change. Volume 1: Internal Factors. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Labov, William. 2001. Principles of Linguistic Change. Volume 2: Social Factors. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Laca, Brenda. 2006. El objeto directo. La marcación preposicional. In Sintaxis histórica de la lengua española. Primera parte: La frase verbal. Edited by Concepcion Company Company. México: Fondo de Cultura Económica/UNAM, pp. 423–78. [Google Scholar]
- Léglise, Isabelle, and Claudine Chamoreau, eds. 2013. The Interplay of Variation and Change in Contact Settings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- López, Luis. 2012. Indefinite Objects: Scrambling, Choice Functions, and Differential Marking. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lundquist, Björn, Yulia Rodina, Iirina A Sekerina, and Marit Westergaar. 2016. Gender change in Norwegian dialects: Comprehension is affected before production. Linguistics Vanguard 2: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Malovrh, Paul A., and James F Lee. 2010. Connections between processing, production and placement: Acquiring object pronouns in Spanish as a second language. In Research in Second Language Processing and Parsing. Edited by Bill VanPatten and Jill Jegerski. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 231–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, Elisabeth, and Liana Sánchez. 2018. Typological differences in morphological patterns, gender features, and thematic structure in the L2 acquisition of Ashaninka Spanish. Languages 3: 21. [Google Scholar]
- McMahon, April. 1994. Understanding Language Change. Cambridge: University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2004. Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers. A case of morpho-syntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 125–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina. 2014. Structural changes in Spanish in the United States: Differential object marking in Spanish heritage speakers across generations. Lingua 151B: 177–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina, and Melissa Bowles. 2009. Back to basics: Differential Object Marking under incomplete acquisition in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12: 363–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Montrul, Silvina, and Noelia Sánchez-Walker. 2013. Differential Object Marking in Child and Adult Spanish Heritage Speakers. Language Acquisition 20: 109–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Cortes, Silvia, Michael T Putnam, and Liliana Sánchez. 2019. Differential Access: Asymmetries in Accessing Features and Building Representation in Heritage Language Grammars. Languages 4: 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Perpiñán, Silvia. 2015. L2 Grammar and L2 Processing in the Acquisition of Spanish Relative Clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18: 577–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pottier, Bernard. 1968. L’emploi de la pre’position a devant l’objet en espagnol. Bulletin de la Société de Linguistique 1: 83–95. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, Keith. 1979. Eye guidance in reading: Fixation locations within words. Perception 8: 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodríguez-Ordóñez, Itxaso. 2017. Reexamining differential object marking as a linguistic contact phenomenon in Gernika Basque. Journal of Language Contact 10: 318–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sánchez, Liliana. 2003. Quechua-Spanish Bilingualism: Interference and Convergence in Functional Categories. Amsterdam: Johns Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Sankoff, David. 1988. Sociolinguistics and syntactic variation. In Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: The Socio-Cultural Context. Edited by Frederick J. Newmeyer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 140–61. [Google Scholar]
- Schilling, Natalie. 2013. Sociolinguistic Fieldwork. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shin, Naomi Lapidus. 2014. Grammatical complexification in Spanish in New York: 3sg pronoun expression and verbal ambiguity. Language Variation and Change 26: 303–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shipley, Elizabeth, Carlonta Smith, and Lila Gleitman. 1969. A study in the acquisition of language: Free responses to commands. Language 45: 322–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva-Corvalán, Carmen. 1994. Language Contact and Change. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinnemäki, Kaius. 2014. A typological perspective on differential object marking. Linguistics 52: 281–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tippets, Ian Robert. 2010. Differential Object Marking in Spanish: A Quantitative Variationist Study. Unpublished. Ph.D. dissertation, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Von Heusinger, Klaus, and Georg A Kaiser. 2005. The evolution of differential object marking in Spanish. In Specificity and the Evolution/Emergence of Nominal Determination Systems in Romance. Edited by Klaus von Heusinger, Georg Kaiser and Elisabeth Stark. Konstanz: Universität Konstanz, pp. 33–69. [Google Scholar]
- Von Heusinger, Klaus, and Georg A. Kaiser. 2007. Differential object marking and the lexical semantics of verbs in Spanish. In Proceedings of the workshop “Definiteness, Specificity and Animacy in Ibero-Romance Languages”. Edited by G. A. Kaiser and M. Leonetti. Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft: Universität Konstanz, pp. 85–110. [Google Scholar]
- Weinreich, Uriel, William Labov, and Herzog Marvin. 1968. Directions for historical linguistics. In Empirical Foundations for a Theory of Language Change. Edited by Winfred P. Lehmann and Yakov Malkiel. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 95–188. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, John. 2009. Implicit learning in second language acquisition. In The New Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by William Ritchie and Tej Bhatia. Bingley: Emerald Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Yager, Lisa, Nora Hellmold, Hyoun-A. Joo, Michael Putnam, Eleonora Rossi, Catherine Stafford, and Joseph Salmons. 2015. New structural patterns in moribund grammar: Case marking in heritage German. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Participants | N | Age | AoA of Spanish | AoA of English |
---|---|---|---|---|
Monolinguals | 34 | 19.3 (18–22) | birth | 10 (10–12) |
Direct Object | [+DOM] | [−DOM] |
---|---|---|
Animate | El niño acusó al señor de las gafas azules. | *Diego acogió el estudiante de intercambio. |
‘The kid accused the man with the blue glasses.’ | ‘Diego welcomed the exchange student.’ | |
Inanimate | El joven apreció al esfuerzo económico por parte de sus padres. | La actriz dibujó el carro de sus sueños |
‘The young boy appreciated the economic effort that his parents made.’ | ‘The actress drew her dream car.’ |
Direct Object | [+DOM] | [−DOM] |
---|---|---|
Animate | El actor liberó al compañero con su llave. | *El actor liberó el compañero con su llave. |
‘The actor freed DOM the companion with his key.’ | ‘The actor freed the companion with his key.’ | |
Inanimate | *El joven movió al sofá a la calle para dormir. | El joven movió el sofá a la calle para dormir. |
‘The young man moved DOM the sofa to the street to sleep.’ | ‘The young man moved the sofa to the street to sleep.’ |
Object | Total Objects | DOM- Marked | Unmarked |
---|---|---|---|
Animate | 196 (100%) | 194 (98.98%) | 2 (1.02%) |
Inanimate | 202 (100%) | 1 (0.50%) | 201 (99.50%) |
Direct Object | Total Objects | DOM-Marked | Unmarked |
---|---|---|---|
Animate | 391 (100%) | 382 (97.70%) | 9 (2.30%) |
Inanimate | 387 (100%) | 7 (1.80%) | 380 (98.20%) |
Stage | Measure | Definition |
---|---|---|
Early | First Pass Reading Times | The sum of all fixations in the region before exiting it |
Sum of Skipped regions | The sum of trials where the region was not fixated | |
Late | Second Pass Reading Times | The summed length of all fixations in a region when the reader re-reads it |
Total Reading Time | The sum of all fixations and refixations on the target | |
Regressions Out | The number of times a region is exited (with an eye regression) to a previous region | |
Regressions In | The number of times a region is entered (with an eye regression) from a later region |
TT | FP | SP | RI | RO | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Region 3 | |||||
Animate + DOM | 538.41 (298.77) | 223.81 (72.14) | 314.60 (294.82) | 0.26 (0.49) | 0.24 (0.43) |
Animate − DOM | 665.55 (388.11) | 234.47 (87.99) | 431.08 (377.45) | 0.35 (0.48) | 0.26 (0.35) |
Inanimate − DOM | 618.87 (318.77) | 227.46 (87.46) | 391.41 (310.69) | 0.31 (0.48) | 0.18 (0.39) |
Inanimate + DOM | 664.28 (363.06) | 230.53 (92.94) | 433.75 (355.37) | 0.34 (0.45) | 0.26 (0.44) |
Region 4 | |||||
Animate + DOM | 395.08 (247.29) | 208.14 (97.82) | 186.94 (222.31) | 0.17 (0.38 | 0.14 (0.35) |
Animate − DOM | 473.27 (268.95) | 222.33 (88.12) | 250.93 (248.84) | 0.23 (0.42) | 0.14 (0.35) |
Inanimate − DOM | 419.08 (265.26) | 209.78 (88.09) | 209.30 (241.40) | 0.17 (0.38) | 0.13 (0.34) |
Inanimate + DOM | 413.73 (252.20) | 209.51 (93.75) | 204.22 (228.48) | 0.16 (0.37) | 0.20 (0.40) |
Region 5 | |||||
Animate + DOM | 396.04 (282.18) | 214.01 (105.82) | 182.03 (248.93) | 0.34 (0.48) | 0.15 (0.36) |
Animate − DOM | 429.31 266.80 | 218.08 (106.10) | 211.23 (229.40) | 0.29 (0.45) | 0.16 (0.37) |
Inanimate − DOM | 391.61 (273.26) | 210.11 (87.80) | 181.50 (250.88) | 0.35 (0.48) | 0.12 (0.34) |
Inanimate + DOM | 396.47 (252.15) | 206.91 (96.22) | 189.56 (216.77) | 0.30 (0.46) | 0.12 (0.34) |
Region 6 | |||||
Animate + DOM | 416.82 (262.25) | 224.59 (112.23) | 192.23 (228.98) | 0.21 (0.41) | 0.25 (0.44) |
Animate − DOM | 420.98 (263.24) | 226.14 (100.53) | 194.84 (242.58) | 0.18 (0.39) | 0.25 (0.43) |
Inanimate − DOM | 381.46 (281.70) | 213.85 (96.82) | 167.61 (254.64) | 0.24 (0.43) | 0.23 (0.42) |
Inanimate + DOM | 392.20 (274.48) | 216.53 (113.38) | 175.67 (245.18) | 0.25 (0.43) | 0.21 (0.41) |
Marking | Animacy | Emmean6 | SE | DF | Lower CL | Upper CL |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[−DOM] | animate | 670.75 | 35.80 | 105.06 | 599.75 | 741.75 |
[+DOM] | animate | 551.55 | 35.51 | 101.79 | 481.10 | 622.00 |
[−DOM] | inanimate | 625.14 | 35.26 | 99.24 | 555.16 | 695.12 |
[+DOM] | inanimate | 664.37 | 35.48 | 101.85 | 593.98 | 734.76 |
Marking | Animacy | Emmean | SE | DF | Lower CL | Upper CL |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[−DOM] | animate | 469.78 | 23.89 | 136.56 | 422.54 | 517.03 |
[+DOM] | animate | 389.46 | 24.182 | 142.64 | 341.66 | 437.26 |
[−DOM] | inanimate | 415.72 | 23.66 | 132.07 | 368.91 | 462.53 |
[+DOM] | inanimate | 409.52 | 23.69 | 132.86 | 362.65 | 456.38 |
Marking | Animacy | Emmean | SE | DF | Lower CL | Upper CL |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[−DOM] | animate | 436.43 | 34.26 | 113.44 | 368.56 | 504.30 |
[+DOM] | animate | 326.15 | 33.96 | 109.70 | 258.84 | 393.46 |
[−DOM] | inanimate | 395.25 | 33.70 | 106.80 | 328.42 | 462.07 |
[+DOM] | inanimate | 431.40 | 33.93 | 109.81 | 364.16 | 498.65 |
Marking | Animacy | Emmean | SE | DF | Lower CL | Upper CL |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[−DOM] | animate | 247.65 | 21.28 | 138.35 | 205.56 | 289.74 |
[+DOM] | animate | 182.07 | 21.55 | 144.78 | 139.46 | 224.67 |
[−DOM] | inanimate | 203.92 | 21.06 | 133.56 | 162.24 | 245.59 |
[+DOM] | inanimate | 198.33 | 21.09 | 134.42 | 156.61 | 240.06 |
Marking | Animacy | Emmean | SE | DF | Lower CL | Upper CL |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[−DOM] | animate | 0.34 | 0.03 | 112.02 | 0.27 | 0.42 |
[+DOM] | animate | 0.26 | 0.03 | 105.70 | 0.19 | 0.33 |
[−DOM] | inanimate | 0.31 | 0.03 | 101.54 | 0.23 | 0.38 |
[+DOM] | inanimate | 0.34 | 0.03 | 107.44 | 0.27 | 0.41 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Arechabaleta Regulez, B.; Montrul, S. Psycholinguistic Evidence for Incipient Language Change in Mexican Spanish: The Extension of Differential Object Marking. Languages 2021, 6, 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030131
Arechabaleta Regulez B, Montrul S. Psycholinguistic Evidence for Incipient Language Change in Mexican Spanish: The Extension of Differential Object Marking. Languages. 2021; 6(3):131. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030131
Chicago/Turabian StyleArechabaleta Regulez, Begoña, and Silvina Montrul. 2021. "Psycholinguistic Evidence for Incipient Language Change in Mexican Spanish: The Extension of Differential Object Marking" Languages 6, no. 3: 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030131
APA StyleArechabaleta Regulez, B., & Montrul, S. (2021). Psycholinguistic Evidence for Incipient Language Change in Mexican Spanish: The Extension of Differential Object Marking. Languages, 6(3), 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030131