Contribution of Nonverbal Cognitive Skills on Bilingual Children’s Grammatical Performance: Influence of Exposure, Task Type, and Language of Assessment
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Role of Exposure on Bilingual Language Development
1.2. The Role of Working Memory and Processing Speed in Bilingual Development
1.3. Morphosyntactic Task Complexity and Cognitive Demands
1.4. Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedures
2.3. Language Exposure
2.4. Measures of Nonverbal Cognitive Ability
2.5. Morphosyntactic Ability
2.6. Analytical Strategy
3. Results
3.1. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions
3.2. Predicting Performance on Cloze Tasks in English and Spanish
3.3. Predicting Performance on Narrative Tasks in English and Spanish
3.4. Predicting Performance on Cloze Tasks Based on Language Dominance
3.5. Predicting Performance on Narrative Tasks Based on Language Dominance
4. Discussion
4.1. A Balance of Tasks
4.2. Findings in Light of the Theory of Cognitive Abilities
4.3. Implications
4.4. Limitations
4.5. Future Directions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
TNL Item Type | English Items | Spanish Items |
---|---|---|
Temporal relationships—uses one or more adverbial phrases or clauses between actions or events | Task 4, #19 Task 6, #8 | Task 4, #20 Task 6, #8 |
Causal relationships—indicates causal relationships between actions or events; using (because, so that, since, in order to…) | Task 4, #20 Task 6, #9 | Task 4, #21 Task 6, #9 |
Grammaticality—uses grammatically correct sentences without errors | Task 4, #21 Task 6, #15 | Task 4, #22 Task 6, #15 |
Tense—uses the same tense throughout the story | Task 4, #22 Task 6, #14 | Task 4, #23 Task 6, #14 |
Reference—consistent references to characters; appropriate pronoun use | Task 6, #13 | Task 6, #13 |
Total items: | 18 | 18 |
Appendix B
English Forms | Example | # of items on BESA | # of items on BESA-ME |
Progressive | They are watching | 3 | -- |
Negative | Doesn’t like | 3 | 2 |
Plural | Apples | 3 | 3 |
Possessive | Duck’s eggs | 3 | 3 |
Third-person singular | Plays | 3 | 4 |
Copula | Flower is pretty | 3 | 2 |
Passive | Dog was chased | 3 | 4 |
Past | Walked the dog | 3 | 4 |
Irregular past | Ate the banana | -- | 4 |
Question inversion | What is it…? | -- | 6 |
Prepositions | On the plate | -- | 2 |
Relative clause | Dog that has black fur | -- | 3 |
Spanish forms | Example | # of items on BESA | # of items on BESA-ME |
Progressive | La niña está nadando | 3 | -- |
Article | Unas/las manzanas | 4 | 3 |
Direct object clitic | Él los abraza | 4 | 3 |
Subjunctive | Quiere que coman | 4 | 6 |
Irregular past | La niña fue | -- | 4 |
Prepositions | Encima de la mesa | -- | 2 |
Negative | No tiene | -- | 3 |
Relative clause | Niño que juega con la pelota | -- | 4 |
Adjective | Zapato morado | -- | 4 |
Conditional | Iría | -- | 5 |
Imperfect past | Ella nadaba | -- | 4 |
References
- Abu-Rabia, Salim, and Linda S. Siegel. 2002. Reading, syntactic, orthographic, and working memory. Skills of bilingual Arabic-English speaking Canadian children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31: 661–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aiken, Leona S., Stephen G. West, and Raymond R. Reno. 1991. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newcastle upon Tyne: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, Lisa M. 2017. Working memory and language learning: A review. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 33: 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan. 1992. Working memory. Science 255: 556–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan. 2012. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barac, Raluca, and Ellen Bialystok. 2012. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development 83: 413–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bedore, Lisa M., and Elizabeth D. Peña. 2008. Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 11: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedore, Lisa M., Elizabeth D. Peña, Connie L. Summers, Karin M. Boerger, Maria D. Resendiz, Kai Greene, and Ronald B. Gillam. 2012. The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and cognition 15: 616–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bedore, Lisa M., Elizabeth D. Peña, Jissel B. Anaya, Ricardo Nieto, Mirza J. Lugo-Neris, and Alisa Baron. 2018. Understanding disorder within variation: Production of English grammatical forms by English language learners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49: 277–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bishop, Dorothy V., Maragaret J. Snowling, Paul A. Thompson, Trisha Greenhalgh, and the Catalise-2 Consortium. 2017. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58: 1068–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bock, J. Kathryn. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology 18: 355–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bracken, Bruce A., and R. Steve McCallum. 1998. Examiner’s Manual: Universal Nonverbal Intelligence Test (UNIT). Itasca: Riverside Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chondrogianni, Vasliki, and Theodoros Marinis. 2012. Production and processing asymmetries in the acquisition of tense morphology by sequential bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chondrogianni, Vasliki, Nada Vasić, Theodoros Marinis, and Elma Blom. 2015a. Production and on-line comprehension of definiteness in English and Dutch by monolingual and sequential bilingual children. Second Language Research 31: 309–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chondrogianni, Vasliki, Theodoros Marinis, Susan Edwards, and Elma Blom. 2015b. Production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and clitic pronouns by Greek sequential bilingual children and monolingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 36: 1155–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clahsen, Harald, and Claudia Felser. 2006. Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 27: 3–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Crivello, Cristina, Olivia Kuzyk, Monyka Rodrigues, Margaret Friend, Pascal Zesiger, and Diane Poulin-Dubois. 2016. The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers’ executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 141: 121–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cunnings, Ian. 2017. Parsing and working memory in bilingual sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20: 659–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Da Fontoura, Helena A., and Linda S. Siegel. 1995. Reading, syntactic, and working memory skills of bilingual Portuguese-English Canadian children. Reading and Writing 7: 139–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dick, Anthony S., Nelcida L. Garcia, Shannon M. Pruden, Wesley K. Thompson, Samuel W. Hawes, Mathew T. Sutherland, and Raul Gonzalez. 2019. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour 3: 692–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, John, and Adrian M. Owen. 2000. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences 23: 475–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Durant, Kathleen, Elizabeth D. Peña, Anna M. Peña, Lisa M. Bedore, and María R. Muñoz. 2019. Not all nonverbal tasks are equally nonverbal: Comparing two tasks in bilingual kindergartners with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62: 3462–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ebert, Kerry D. 2014. Role of auditory non-verbal working memory in sentence repetition for bilingual children with primary language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 49: 631–36. [Google Scholar]
- Festman, Julia. 2021. Learning and processing multiple languages: The more the easier? Language Learning. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Field, John. 2011. Cognitive Validity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, vol. 30. [Google Scholar]
- Flanagan, Dawn P., and Kevin S. McGrew. 1998. Interpreting intelligence tests from contemporary Gf-Gc theory: Joint confirmatory factor analysis of the WJ-R and KAIT in a non-white sample. Journal of School Psychology 36: 151–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freund, Rudolf J., Ramon C. Littell, and Lee Creighton. 2003. Regression Using JMP. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Gagarina, Natalia, Daleen Klop, Sari Kunnari, Koula Tantele, Taina Välimaa, Ingrida Balčiūnienė, and Joel Walters. 2015. Assessment of narrative abilities in bilingual children. In Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Croydon: Deanta Global Publishing Services Limited, pp. 243–76. [Google Scholar]
- Gangopadhyay, Ishanti, Meghan M. Davidson, Susan E. Weismer, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2016. The role of nonverbal working memory in morphosyntactic processing by school-aged monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 142: 171–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, Susan E., and Alan D. Baddeley. 2014. Working Memory and Language. Austin: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gillam, Ronald B., and Nils A. Pearson. 2004. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. Austin: Pro-ed. [Google Scholar]
- Gillam, Ronald B., Elizabeth D. Peña, Bedore Lisa M., and Nils Pearson. Forthcoming. Test of Narrative Language–Spanish Adaptation.
- Gutiérrez-Clellen, Vera F., and Elisabeth Peña. 2001. Dynamic assessment of diverse children: A tutorial. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 32: 21–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hair, Joseph F., Christian M. Ringle, and Marko Sarstedt. 2013. Partial least squares structural equation modeling: Rigorous applications, better results and higher acceptance. Long Range Planning 46: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hipfner-Boucher, Kathleen, Trelani Milburn, Elanie Weitzman, Janice Greenberg, Janette Pelletier, and Luigi Girolametto. 2015. Narrative abilities in subgroups of English language learners and monolingual peers. International Journal of Bilingualism 19: 677–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hollingshead, August B. 1973. Medical sociology: A brief review. Health and Society 51: 531–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopp, Holger. 2015. Semantics and morphosyntax in predictive L2 sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 53: 277–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiao, Lu, Cong Liu, Ruming Wang, and Baoguo Chen. 2019. Working memory demand of a task modulates bilingual advantage in executive functions. International Journal of Bilingualism 23: 102–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Young-Suk Grace, Cheahyung Park, and Younghee Park. 2015. Dimensions of discourse level oral language skills and their relation to reading comprehension and written composition: An exploratory study. Reading and Writing 28: 633–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuczaj, Stan A., II. 1977. The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16: 589–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lemmerth, Natalia, and Holger Hopp. 2019. Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic influences. Language Acquisition 26: 21–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levenston, Eddie A., N. Nir, and Shoshana Blum-Kulka. 1984. Discourse analysis and the testing of reading comprehension by cloze techniques. In Reading for Professional Purposes: Studies and Practices in Native and Foreign Languages. Edited by Pugh Anthoy K. and Jan M. Ulijn. London: Heinemann Educational, pp. 202–12. [Google Scholar]
- Macnamara, Brooke N., and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2014. Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21: 520–25. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall, Chloe R., and Heather K. J. van der Lely. 2008. Recognition of gated verbs by children with Grammatical-Specific Language Impairment: Effects of inflection and frequency. Journal of Neurolinguistics 21: 433–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMillen, Stephanie, Linda Jarmulowicz, Michael M. Mackay, and D. Kimbrough Oller. 2020. Rapid shift in naming efficiency on a rapid automatic naming task by young Spanish-speaking English language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 41: 847–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mizumoto, Atsushi, Maiko Ikeda, and Osamu Takeuchi. 2016. A comparison of cognitive processing during cloze and multiple-choice reading tests using brain activation. ARELE: Annual Review of English Language Education in Japan 27: 65–80. [Google Scholar]
- Norbury, Courtenay Frazier, Debbie Gooch, Charlotte Wray, Gillian Baird, Tony Charman, Emily Simonoff, George Vamvakas, and Andrew Pickles. 2016. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57: 1247–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oberauer, Klaus. 2004. The measurement of working memory capacity. Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence 2005: 393–408. [Google Scholar]
- Oppenheim, Gary M., Zenzi Griffin, Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2020. Longitudinal evidence for simultaneous bilingual language development with shifting language dominance, and how to explain it. Language Learning 70: 20–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2010. Bilingual children’s acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and task type. Language Learning 60: 651–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, Johanne, Martha Crago, Fred Genesee, and Mabel Rice. 2003. French-English Bilingual Children With SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46: 113–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Park, Jisook, Carol A. Miller, and Elina Mainela-Arnold. 2015. Processing speed measures as clinical markers for children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 58: 954–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Peña, Elizabeth D., Lisa M. Bedore, and Zenzi M. Griffin. 2010. Cross-language Outcomes of Typical and Atypical Development in Bilinguals. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. [Google Scholar]
- Peña, Elizabeth D., Lisa M. Bedore, Vera F. Gutiérrez-Clellen, Aquiles Iglesias, and Brian A. Goldstein. 2016. Bilingual English–Spanish Assessment—Middle Extension Field Test Version (BESA-ME). Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Peña, Elizabeth D., Vera F. Gutiérrez-Clellen, Aquiles Iglesias, Brian A. Goldstein, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2018. Bilingual English Spanish Assessment (BESA). Baltimore: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Peña, Elizabeth D., Vera F. Gutiérrez-Clellen, Aquiles Iglesias, Brian A. Goldstein, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2018. Bilingual Input–Output Survey (BIOS). Baltimore: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Pratt, Amy S., John A. Grinstead, and Rebecca J. McCauley. 2020. Emergent Literacy in Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Preliminary Findings of Delays in Comprehension-and Code-Related Skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63: 4193–4207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ribot, Krystal M., Erika Hoff, and Andrea Burridge. 2018. Language use contributes to expressive language growth: Evidence from bilingual children. Child Development 89: 929–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sabourin, Laura, Marco Haverkort, Roeland Van Hout, Aafke Hulk, Folkert Kuiken, and Richard Towell. 2003. The Lexicon–Syntax Interface in Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Sagarra, Nuria, and Julia Herschensohn. 2010. The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua 120: 2022–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, Jeong-Ah, and Christianson Kiel. 2009. Syntactic processing in Korean–English bilingual production: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition 112: 175–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shin, Jeong-Ah, and Christianson Kiel. 2012. Structural priming and second language learning. Language Learning 62: 931–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Summers, Connie, Thomas M. Bohman, Ronald B. Gillman, Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2010. Bilingual performance on nonword repetition in Spanish and English. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 45: 480–493. [Google Scholar]
- Talli, Ioanna, and Stavroula Stavrakaki. 2020. Short-term memory, working memory and linguistic abilities in bilingual children with Developmental Language Disorder. First Language 40: 437–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uccelli, Paola, and Mariela M. Páez. 2007. Narrative and Vocabulary Development of Bilingual Children from Kindergarten to First Grade: Developmental Changes and Associations among English and Spanish Skills. Available online: https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/024) (accessed on 27 November 2020).
- Valdés, Guadalupe. 2004. Between support and marginalisation: The development of academic language in linguistic minority children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 7: 102–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Patten, Bill, Gregory D. Keating, and Michael J. Leeser. 2012. Missing verbal inflections as a representational problem: Evidence from self-paced reading. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2: 109–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber-Fox, Christine M., and Helen J. Neville. 1996. Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8: 231–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Demographic Information | |||
---|---|---|---|
Range | Mean | (SD) | |
Age (months) | 61–138 | 92.22 | (18.67) |
English exposure (% week) | 0–100 | 56.84 | (26.85) |
Age of 1st English exposure (years) | 0–6 | 2.30 | (1.92) |
SES a | 0–7 | 3.01 | (1.73) |
Nonverbal IQ b | 60–132 | 100.48 | (14.57) |
Gender | 47% male | ||
Ethnicity | 99% Hispanic |
Morphosyntax | English: | Spanish: | Best Language: | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Range | Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | Mean | (SD) | |
Cloze a (% correct) | 0–100 | 53.36 | (29.28) | 48.42 | (25.52) | 66.19 | (20.70) |
Narrative b (% correct) | 0–100 | 43.23 | (23.89) | 54.68 | (23.67) | 61.78 | (18.47) |
Cognition | |||||||
Range | Mean | (SD) | |||||
Working memory c | 0–25 | 9.28 | 4.53 | ||||
Processing speed c | 0–43 | 16.22 | 8.08 |
2. Exp | 3. WM | 4. PS | 5. Eng. cl | 6. Sp cl | 7. Eng. narr | 8. Sp. narr. | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. | Age | 0.36 ** | 0.57 ** | 0.60 ** | 0.57 ** | −0.04 | 0.53 ** | 0.39 ** |
2. | Exposure | -- | 0.14 * | 0.26 ** | 0.63 ** | −0.44 ** | 0.46 ** | 0.00 |
3. | Working memory | −0.02 | -- | 0.51 ** | 0.40 ** | 0.10 | 0.39 ** | 0.27 ** |
4. | Processing speed | 0.06 | 0.25 ** | -- | 0.46 ** | −0.02 | 0.46 ** | 0.23 ** |
5. | English cloze | 0.35 ** | 0.20 ** | 0.21 ** | -- | −0.05 | 0.68 ** | 0.28 ** |
6. | Spanish cloze | −0.24 ** | 0.10 | 0.03 | 0.15 * | -- | −0.03 | 0.52 ** |
7. | English narrative | 0.25 ** | 0.12 | 0.20 ** | 0.53 ** | 0.10 | -- | 0.26 ** |
8. | Spanish narrative | −0.28 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.04 | 0.60 ** | 0.04 | -- |
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | ||
English cloze | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.57 | 11.99 ** | 0.39 | 9.25 ** | 0.24 | 4.35 ** | 0.24 | 4.54 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | 0.49 | 11.53 ** | 0.49 | 11.93 ** | 0.47 | 11.14 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.13 | 2.76 ** | 0.14 | 2.86 ** |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.13 | 2.62 ** | 0.13 | 2.69 ** | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.03 | 0.74 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.09 | 2.11 * | |
R2 | 0.32 | 0.53 | 0.56 | 0.57 | |||||
R2 change | 0.32 ** | 0.21 ** | 0.03 ** | 0.01 * | |||||
Spanish cloze | |||||||||
1. | Age | −0.04 | −0.66 | 0.18 | 3.06 | 0.09 | 1.22 | 0.10 | 1.27 |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | −0.5 | −8.77 ** | −0.51 | −8.58 ** | −0.52 | −8.20 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.13 | 1.90 | 0.13 | 1.85 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.02 | 0.24 | 0.00 | 0.01 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.02 | 0.35 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.12 | −1.97 * | |
R2 | 0.00 | 0.22 | 0.24 | 0.25 | |||||
R2 change | 0.00 | 0.22 ** | 0.01 | 0.01 |
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | ||
English narrative | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.53 | 10.90 ** | 0.42 | 8.51 ** | 0.25 | 3.92 ** | 0.25 | 3.96 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | 0.31 | 6.42 ** | 0.31 | 6.57 ** | 0.31 | 6.17 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.11 | 1.95 | 0.11 | 1.95 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.18 | 3.12 ** | 0.18 | 3.12 ** | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.01 | −0.14 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.04 | −0.84 | |
R2 | 0.29 | 0.37 | 0.41 | 0.41 | |||||
R2 change | 0.29 ** | 0.09 ** | 0.04 ** | 0.00 | |||||
Spanish narrative | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.39 | 6.71 ** | 0.55 | 8.32 ** | 0.52 | 6.15 ** | 0.54 | 6.37 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | −0.30 | −4.56 ** | −0.30 | −4.52 ** | −0.32 | −4.71 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.06 | 0.90 | 0.03 | 0.36 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.00 | 0.02 | −0.03 | −0.40 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.10 | −1.51 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.06 | −0.80 | |
R2 | 0.15 | 0.22 | 0.22 | 0.23 | |||||
R2 change | 0.15 ** | 0.07 ** | 0.00 | 0.02 |
Narrative: Stronger Language | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cloze: Stronger language | English: | Spanish: | Total: | ||
English: | n = 51 | 74 | 125 (50%) | ||
Spanish: | 13 | 112 | 125 (50%) | ||
Total: | 64 (26%) | 186 (74%) | 250 |
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | ||
Child’s weaker language | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.43 | 7.78 ** | 0.54 | 9.24 ** | 0.38 | 5.04 ** | 0.39 | 5.28 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | −0.27 | −4.58 ** | −0.26 | −4.45 ** | −0.27 | −4.56 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.20 | 3.03 ** | 0.19 | 2.90 ** |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.13 | 1.16 | 0.06 | 0.86 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.02 | 0.32 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.24 | −4.06 ** | |
R2 | 0.18 | 0.24 | 0.28 | 0.33 | |||||
R2 change | 0.18 ** | 0.06 ** | 0.04 ** | 0.05 ** | |||||
Child’s stronger language | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.32 | 5.52 ** | 0.22 | 3.49 | 0.09 | 1.16 | 0.10 | 1.17 |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | 0.24 | 3.89 ** | 0.25 | 3.96 ** | 0.25 | −3.70 ** |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.13 | 1.79 | 0.13 | 1.73 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.08 | 1.14 | 0.08 | 1.06 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.01 | 0.10 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.05 | −0.77 | |
R2 | 0.10 | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.17 | |||||
R2 change | 0.10 ** | 0.05 ** | 0.02 | 0.00 |
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | t | β | t | β | t | β | t | ||
Child’s weaker language | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.55 | 10.44 ** | 0.58 | 9.21 ** | 0.49 | 6.17 ** | 0.50 | 6.32 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | −0.05 | −0.83 | −0.06 | −0.92 | −0.06 | −0.88 |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.06 | 0.85 | 0.05 | 0.73 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.10 | 1.52 | 0.05 | 0.65 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.01 | 0.13 |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.14 | −2.13 * | |
R2 | 0.31 | 0.31 | 0.32 | 0.33 | |||||
R2 change | 0.31 ** | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.02 | |||||
Child’s stronger language | |||||||||
1. | Age | 0.56 | 10.64 ** | 0.56 | 8.91 ** | 0.44 | 5.59 ** | 0.46 | 5.86 ** |
2. | Exposure to English | -- | -- | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.06 | −0.03 | −0.46 |
3. | Working memory | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.11 | 1.62 | 0.06 | 0.88 |
Processing speed | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.10 | 1.52 | 0.10 | 1.45 | |
4. | Exp. * Working mem. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | −0.15 | −2.33 * |
Exp. * Processing sp. | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.03 | 0.47 | |
R2 | 0.31 | 0.31 | 0.33 | 0.35 | |||||
R2 change | 0.31 ** | 0.00 | 0.02 * | 0.02 † |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wood, T.; Pratt, A.S.; Durant, K.; McMillen, S.; Peña, E.D.; Bedore, L.M. Contribution of Nonverbal Cognitive Skills on Bilingual Children’s Grammatical Performance: Influence of Exposure, Task Type, and Language of Assessment. Languages 2021, 6, 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6010036
Wood T, Pratt AS, Durant K, McMillen S, Peña ED, Bedore LM. Contribution of Nonverbal Cognitive Skills on Bilingual Children’s Grammatical Performance: Influence of Exposure, Task Type, and Language of Assessment. Languages. 2021; 6(1):36. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6010036
Chicago/Turabian StyleWood, Taffeta, Amy S. Pratt, Kathleen Durant, Stephanie McMillen, Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2021. "Contribution of Nonverbal Cognitive Skills on Bilingual Children’s Grammatical Performance: Influence of Exposure, Task Type, and Language of Assessment" Languages 6, no. 1: 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6010036
APA StyleWood, T., Pratt, A. S., Durant, K., McMillen, S., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M. (2021). Contribution of Nonverbal Cognitive Skills on Bilingual Children’s Grammatical Performance: Influence of Exposure, Task Type, and Language of Assessment. Languages, 6(1), 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6010036