Next Article in Journal
Examination of Manner of Motion Sound Symbolism for English Nonce Verbs
Next Article in Special Issue
The Development of Subject Position in Dutch-Dominant Heritage Speakers of Spanish: From Age 9 to Adulthood
Previous Article in Journal
“Esa no soy Yo”: Self-Image and Name Change from the Perspective of Female Immigrants
Previous Article in Special Issue
Bilingual Alignments
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Lack of Syllable Duration as a Post-Lexical Acoustic Cue in Spanish in Contact with Maya

by
Nuria Martínez García
1,* and
Melanie Uth
2,*
1
CRC 1252 Prominence in Language, University of Cologne, 50923 Köln, Germany
2
Department of Romance Studies, University of Cologne, 50939 Köln, Germany
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Languages 2019, 4(4), 84; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4040084
Submission received: 20 September 2019 / Revised: 10 October 2019 / Accepted: 20 October 2019 / Published: 31 October 2019

Abstract

:
This paper focuses on the duration of stressed syllables in broad versus contrastive focus in Yucatecan Spanish and examines its connection with Spanish–Maya bilingualism. We examine the claim that phonemic vowel length in one language prevents the use of syllable duration as a post-lexical acoustic cue in another. We study the duration of stressed syllables of nouns in subject and object position in subject-verb-object (SVO) sentences (broad and contrastive focus) of a semi-spontaneous production task. One thousand one hundred and twenty-six target syllables of 34 mono- and bilingual speakers were measured and submitted to linear mixed-effects models. Although the target syllables were slightly longer in contrastive focus, duration was not significant, nor was the effect of bilingualism. The results point to duration not constituting a cue to focus marking in Yucatecan Spanish. Finally, it is discussed how this result relates to the strong influence of Yucatec Maya on Yucatecan Spanish prosody observed by both scholars and native speakers of Yucatecan Spanish and other Mexican varieties of Spanish.

1. Introduction

Empirically, the main aim of this article is to investigate whether the speakers of Yucatecan Spanish (in Quintana Roo, Mexico) make use of post-lexical duration as an acoustic cue in order to mark contrastive focus. There is considerable terminological confusion concerning the denomination of the different focus types, since the categories and terms differ from one research tradition to the other (see e.g., Stevens 2017). For the sake of simplicity, we stick to the established dichotomy of ‘information focus’ and ‘contrastive focus’, following most of the syntactic literature on focus marking in (standard) Spanish (see Cruschina 2012, p. 109f; Domínguez 2004; Gutiérrez Bravo 2008; López 2009, pp. 34–37; Sainz-Maza Lecanda 2017, pp. 1–7; Zagona 2002, pp. 248–54; Zubizarreta 1998, 1999), and the references cited therein. In this tradition, the notion of contrastive focus (CFOC in (1)) may be, and generally is, used to refer to corrective focus, that is, “when the focus marks a constituent that is a direct rejection of an alternative, either spoken by the speaker himself […] or by the hearer” (Gussenhoven 2007, p. 84), see (1). In contrast, the notion of information focus is used in order to refer to the new, non-presupposed information of a sentence in the sense of Halliday (1967). Finally, the category of information focus is usually further subdivided into the subtypes narrow focus and broad focus. The notion of narrow focus is used whenever a single constituent of a sentence is non-contrastively focalized, whereas broad focus (BFOC in (2)) is used whenever “there is no particular constituent which is focused (or, alternatively, the entire expression is considered the focus constituent)” (Gussenhoven 2007, p. 91), as in (2).
  • El gato de botas [CFOC ROJAS] se comió un ratón y no el de botas [CFOC AZULES].
    “The cat with red boots ate a mouse, not the one with blue boots.” (Zubizarreta 1999, p. 4230)
  • ¿Qué pasa?—[BFOC Juan está comiendo un helado].
    “What’s happening?—John is eating ice cream.”
Although focus marking is taken to be a universal characteristic of all languages, the cues that a given language uses to that end are not; these cues can be prosodic, syntactic, morphological, or a combination of several of them (Zimmermann and Onea 2011). Prosodically, languages may use pitch accents (e.g., English, German), the insertion of a phonological phrase boundary (Bengali), or changes in pitch and phonological phrase boundaries (Japanese) (Büring 2009; Zimmermann and Onea 2011; see below as well).
In Spanish, focus can be marked by changes in word order (see e.g., Zubizarreta 1998), but also through prosodic means (Face 2002), as several studies on different varieties of Spanish have shown. Contrastive focus seems to be marked prosodically in Castilian and Mexican Spanish by means of F0 differences in F0 peak (which is reached earlier in the stressed syllable, and has a larger F0 range), longer duration, and higher intensity when compared to words in broad focus statements (De-la-Mota et al. 2010; see also Face 2000, 2002; García-Lecumberri 1995; Kim and Avelino 2003; De la Mota Gorriz 1995, 1997). For example, for standard Peninsular Spanish, De la Mota Gorriz (1997) conducted a read-text task with six participants where words with subject, verb, and object functions were presented in broad focus (in the original, “with no special emphasis”, “non contrastive”) and in contrastive focus (in the original, “with contrastive new information”). Lengthening of the stressed syllable was found for all word types in contrastive focus. For Mexico City Spanish, Kim and Avelino (2003) conducted a read-text task with five participants, which looked at differences between broad, narrow, and contrastive focus. For subjects and objects in canonical word order (that is, subject-verb-object, SVO) they found significant durational differences for the stressed syllables of the target words (half of them proparoxytones, half of them paroxytones), but they did not find differences for subjects and objects in non-canonical word order. Specifically, they found that stressed syllables were longer in contrastive focus than in broad focus. From this work, Kim and Avelino conclude that “duration is the most consistent cue in distinguishing contrastive and narrow focus types from broad focus” (p. 372). In this paper, we investigate the issue of post-lexical duration in Yucatecan Spanish in subjects and objects in canonical word order, since there are reasons to doubt that Kim and Avelino’s (2003) generalization may be extended to the Yucatán peninsula (see below).
One important point to bear in mind in this context is that Yucatecan Spanish has been in close contact with Yucatec Maya for more than 500 years by now. According to the 2010 census of the Mexican National Institute of Indigenous Languages (Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas-INALI 2010), the population of Mexico speaking Yucatec Maya as their first or second language amounts to approximately 760,000 persons. The great majority of them (758,000 persons) live in the Yucatán peninsula, which includes the federal states of Yucatán, Campeche, and Quintana Roo. According to the last population census of the National Institute of Statistics and Geography (Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía-INEGI 2015) in 2015, the percentage of the Mayan-speaking population living in the Yucatán peninsula amounts to 8% in Campeche, 12% in Quintana Roo, and 27% in Yucatán, that is, an average of 18% across the entire Yucatán peninsula. In 1970, the Mayan-speaking population on the Yucatán peninsula amounted to 55% (in Lope Blanch 1987, p. 9f). During the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, contact between Spanish and Mayan increased considerably, as more and more Mayan speakers migrated to urban areas to work as domestic help, vendors, manual laborers, and nannies to Spanish-speaking families (Barnes and Michnowicz 2013; Lipski 2004, p. 99; Michnowicz 2008). According to Lope Blanch (1987), the Yucatán peninsula clearly outranks all other regions in Mexico with respect to both the considerable standing of the indigenous language as well as the rate of bilingual speakers (Lope Blanch 1987, p. 22).
The close contact between these two languages is particularly important for the present purposes due to at least the following three reasons. First of all, Yucatecan Spanish is characterized by a broad range of peculiarities, at all levels of linguistic representation, which are mostly traced back to language contact with Yucatec Maya in the literature. On the phonological level, this is the case for features such as the labialization of final nasals (Michnowicz 2006a, 2006b, 2007, 2008), the aspiration of voiceless plosives (Lope Blanch 1987; Mediz Bolio 1951; Michnowicz and Carpenter 2013; Suárez Molina 1996), or glottalization (Lope Blanch 1987; Michnowicz and Kagan 2016; Suárez Molina 1996), and it is especially true for the peculiar intonation of the variety (see, e.g., Barrera Vásquez 1977; Mediz Bolio 1951; Michnowicz and Barnes 2013; Suárez Molina 1996):
[…] lo primero que llama la atención del forastero […] es la entonación fraseal, lenta y pausada, fenómenos que no son sino reflejos de la fonética nativa [maya]. ‘[…] the first thing that attracts the attention of the foreigner […] is the unhurried and deliberate pronunciation, which is nothing else than the mirror image of the native [Mayan] phonetics.’
Secondly, the issue of language contact deserves further attention in the context of our study of prosodic focus marking in Yucatecan Spanish because Yucatec Maya and (central) Mexican Spanish are typologically unrelated languages that dispose of very different strategies of focus marking. Most importantly, Yucatec Maya is known to have a focus position left-adjacent to the main verb and right-adjacent to an extra-phrasal topic (if present), where focused constituents are generally fronted to (see e.g., Gutiérrez Bravo 2015, pp. 21–23; Kügler and Skopeteas 2007).
3.
Tumen  to’on-e’ [FOC maaya] k    t’an-ik-ø
because  1.PL-TOP Maya HAB.ERG.1PL  speak-IND-ABS.3SG
“Porque nosotros lo que hablamos es MAYA.” [“Because what we speak is MAYA”]
(Gutiérrez Bravo 2015, p. 21; our English translation)
Moreover, Yucatec Maya does not seem to make use of any prosodic means in order to signal the information structural content of syntactic constituents. On the contrary, Gussenhoven and Teeuw (2008), Kügler and Skopeteas (2007), and Kügler et al. (2007) argue that information structure is realized by only syntactic means, that is, fronting to the focus position, in Yucatec Maya. Regarding possible effects of language contact, Gutiérrez-Bravo et al. (2019), building on Sobrino (2010), report on a variety of focus fronting constructions that are entirely ungrammatical in standard Mexican Spanish (cf. (4)).
4.
Ellos, VENIR  hicieron    acá en Yucatán
they  come-INF do.PAST.3PL  here   in  Yucatán
“They, they came here to Yucatán.”    (Gutiérrez-Bravo et al. 2019, p. 279)
A detailed comparison of these fronting constructions to their equivalents in Yucatec Maya reveals that the syntax of the Yucatecan Spanish constructions is strikingly similar to the syntax of comparable constructions in Yucatec Maya. Hence, Gutiérrez-Bravo et al. (2019) conclude that sentences such as the one exemplified by (4) are due to direct syntactic transfer from Yucatec Maya. Against this background, we may expect duration (or prosodic cues to focus marking in general) to be much less important in Yucatecan Spanish than in other varieties of Spanish, and since Gutiérrez-Bravo et al. (2019)’s research is based on data from monolingual speakers of Yucatecan Spanish, it suggests that the phenomenon has developed into a regional feature of the variety irrespective of particular language profiles (such as, e.g., monolingual vs. bilingual speakers). This particular issue is further discussed in Section 4 of the present paper.
The third reason for considering language contact in the context of our investigation of post-lexical syllable duration in Yucatecan Spanish is that Spanish and Yucatec Maya furthermore differ with respect to the following three prosodic features. Firstly, Spanish is a language without phonemic vowel length. That is, vowel length is not used to differentiate between word meanings in this language. By contrast, in Yucatec Maya, vowel length is used in order to signal meaning differences at the lexical level (see, e.g., Lehmann 1990; Martínez Huchim 2004; Pike 1946). Table 1 presents some examples of phonemic vowel length in Yucatec Maya.
The second crucial differentiating feature is post-lexical duration itself. As mentioned above, there are a considerable amount of studies showing that post-lexical syllable duration is one of the acoustic cues in order to signal post-lexical pragmatic categories in Spanish, such as in particular contrastive focus, whereas Yucatec Maya has been shown to be a language that does not possess any prosodic means of focus marking at all.
Third, duration can be an acoustic cue to word stress. In Spanish, words can be oxytone, paroxytone, or proparoxytone words, that is, stress is contrastive. The main acoustic correlates of stress in this language are F0, duration, and amplitude (see, e.g., Llisterri et al. 2016). To the best of our knowledge, there are no studies on the acoustic correlates of stress in Yucatecan Spanish; however, we estimate that the aforementioned correlates also fit this variety. On the other hand, stress in Yucatec Maya seems to be noncontrastive and it is debated in which position in the word it appears (Frazier 2009, p. 22), although it seems that heavy syllables attract stress (England and Baird 2017). Because of the uncertainty regarding stress in Yucatec Maya and the lack of acoustic studies on the matter (which could shed some light on whether duration is in fact a cue for stress in this language), we cannot make predictions on how stress in Yucatecan Spanish could be influenced by Yucatec Maya.
Importantly for the present purposes, Baird (2017) suggests that phonemic vowel length and (the lack of) post-lexical duration are interrelated in Spanish–Maya contact. He investigated two groups of Spanish–K’ichee’ (Maya) bilinguals in Guatemala, one in the community of Cantel and the another one in Nahualá. Cantel K’ichee’ has a vocalic system of six vowels that does not possess phonemic vowel length, whereas Nahualá K’ichee’, with 10 vowels, does. Baird conducted focus elicitation (production) tasks for both the Spanish and the K’ichee’ varieties of the bilingual speakers in said communities. He then measured the duration of the stressed syllables of particular target words in broad focus compared to contrastive focus (oxytones in K’ichee’, oxytones and paroxytones in Spanish). As far as the results are concerned, the data obtained for the Mayan varieties suggest, first of all, that duration is used to mark contrastive focus in Cantel K’ichee’ but not in Nahualá K’ichee’: in other words, the speakers of the former variety employ post-lexical duration to mark contrastive focus, whereas the speakers of the latter do not. Secondly, the results from the Spanish production tasks suggest that the speakers from Nahualá (i.e., those who make use of phonemic vowel length in their Mayan variety) do not use post-lexical duration in order to mark contrastive focus, similarly to what has been observed for their Mayan variety. Contrary to this, the Cantel speakers use post-lexical duration to mark contrastive focus in their Spanish variety just as they do in their variety of K’ichee’. Nevertheless, the analysis revealed differences between female and male speakers of Nahualá in that only female speakers show the lack of post-lexical duration in the Spanish production tasks, and only for oxytone words—in K’ichee’, stress appears only in oxytone words (Baird 2014). In view of the fact that these female speakers report using Spanish less than the male ones (according to the Bilingual Language Profile questionnaire that Baird used in this study; see also Section 2.1 below), Baird concludes that the relation between knowledge/proficiency in Spanish and knowledge/proficiency in K’ichee’ seems to play a role in the use of post-lexical duration in the Spanish under study. Thus, Maya-dominant speakers of Spanish have phonemic vowel length in their Mayan variety and hence do not make use of post-lexical duration in general (i.e., neither in their Mayan nor in their Spanish languages). On the other hand, Spanish-dominant speakers seem to have lost the ability to phonemically discriminate between different vowel lengths and may thus employ syllable duration as an acoustic cue for post-lexical pragmatic purposes. However, irrespective of the issue of language dominance, Baird’s (2017) analysis suggests that post-lexical duration as a device of focus marking in these Spanish varieties is closely related to the presence or absence of phonemic vowel length in the respective K’ichee’ Maya contact varieties.
As for Yucatecan Spanish, it is important to recall that it has been in close contact with Yucatec Maya for more than 500 years (see above), and that it presents a similar picture to Nahualá Spanish in that the contact language, Yucatec Maya, has phonemic vowel length but no post-lexical duration to mark focus.
Cross-linguistically, it is not always the case that the presence of phonemic vowel length precludes the use of post-lexical duration to mark focus, since there are also linguistic varieties that have phonemic vowel length but do nevertheless make use of post-lexical duration, such as Serbian; in this variety, stressed syllables are also longer (Smiljanić 2004).1 Nahualá K’ichee’, similarly to Belgrade Serbian, has phonemic vowel length, but does not use duration as a cue to stress (Baird 2014), nor post-lexically, as has already been explained. Thus, Yucatec Maya could be similar to Nahualá K’ichee’ in its lack of duration as a cue to stress, which makes it still possible to relate the presence of phonemic vowel length to the lack of post-lexical duration.2
A final point to bear in mind is that speaker age and gender have been identified as important variables in various sociolinguistic and dialectal studies on Yucatán Spanish (see, e.g., García Fajardo 1984; Michnowicz 2006, 2008, 2015; Pfeiler Blaha 1992; Yager 1989, among others), although the results are anything but conclusive when compared to each other. For example, García Fajardo (1984) studied the vowels and consonants of Yucatecan Spanish in Valladolid, Yucatán, with reference to age, gender and sociocultural groups. She studied labial realizations of nasals in absolute final position (e.g., saying “Yucatám” [jukatam] instead of “Yucatán” [jukatan]) and concludes that all speakers utter them, and there is no pattern according to gender or age (pp. 75–76). However, both Yager (1989) and Michnowicz (2008) studied bilabial realizations of nasals in absolute final position in Mérida, Yucatán, and conclude that labialization is more frequent among women than among men. In Yager’s study, speakers in their twenties produced more bilabial realizations, whereas in Michnowicz’s this can be said about speakers under 50 years of age. In sum, although age and gender seem to play a role, it is unclear what could be hypothesized based on previous studies.
Consequently, against the background of the above insights on language contact, post-lexical duration and phonemic vowel length in Spanish and Maya, the present study addresses the following three research questions by measuring the duration of the target words’ stressed syllables: (i) Do speakers of Yucatecan Spanish use syllable duration as a post-lexical acoustic cue to mark contrastive focus? (ii) Does knowledge of Maya (or language dominance) play a role in how speakers differ with respect to their use of post-lexical duration? (iii) Do the variables age and gender have any effect on the use of syllable duration as a post-lexical acoustic cue in order to mark contrastive focus? The hypotheses that can be posited on the basis of the literature reviewed above are: (i) Contrary to what has been observed for Central Mexican Spanish, speakers of Yucatecan Spanish generally do not make use of greater duration of the stressed syllable to mark contrastive focus in their Spanish variety, although inter-individual variation is expected to exist, (ii) in particular, knowledge of Maya might still correlate with a higher degree of lack of syllable duration in order to mark contrastive focus, since the linguistic system of Yucatec Maya provides information with respect to phonemic vowel length that the monolingual speakers of Yucatecan Spanish do not possess (iii) age and gender may have an effect, although it is not possible to deduce any concrete hypotheses from the literature due to the aforementioned reasons.
The outline of the article is as follows. In Section 2 and Section 3, we present a production investigation on Yucatecan Spanish (Section 2) as well as the results (Section 3). In Section 4, we discuss the results against the background of the presumed interrelation of phonemic vowel length and lack of post-lexical duration presented by Baird (2017) and we mention several points worthy of further study in future research. Finally, in Section 5, we present the main conclusions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study focuses on Yucatecan Spanish as it is spoken in the state of Quintana Roo, specifically in the municipality of Felipe Carrillo Puerto. In Felipe Carrillo Puerto, 21.2% of the population speak Yucatec Maya (Gobierno del Estado de Quintana Roo-Secretaría de Desarrollo Social 2010). Forty-one speakers of Yucatecan Spanish were recorded for this study. All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. Seven were excluded, which resulted in a total of 34 participants (18 females and 16 males, age range = 17–84). Speakers were excluded either because they did not understand the task properly, they hesitated throughout the task, or they answered most questions in a way that would not allow for a comparison with the other speakers’ utterances (for example, by describing the slide at length instead of giving a concrete answer to it). As will be indicated in Section 2.2, literacy was necessary to complete the task; however, because all participants were literate in Spanish, none were excluded.
Participants’ language dominance was assessed by means of the Bilingual Language Profile, or BLP (Birdsong et al. 2012). The BLP is a thorough questionnaire made up of questions about language history, use, proficiency, and attitudes. It yields a score quantifying language dominance; each end of the score continuum (−/+218) corresponds to the highest dominance for a given language—in the present study, Maya and Spanish, respectively—whereas values around 0 indicate balanced bilingualism. Figure 1 shows the scores corresponding to each participant. The scores are distributed along the continuum; values closer to the highest Mayan dominance (e.g., −200, −150) would represent speakers who speak little or no Spanish, which explains the absence of such values.
None of the 34 speakers fall into the −50–−150 range, which would have been desirable considering that there is clearly variation in BLP scores on the Spanish dominant side. It must be kept in mind that BLP scores were obtained in the same session as the recordings, that is: BLP scores were not used to decide which participants should be included or excluded from the study. Although a number of participants was recruited under the assumption that they spoke both languages to different degrees, the actual BLP scores were computed only after the recordings took place. Appendix A lists all BLP scores, gender, age of participants, and number of observations (both for subject and object positions).

2.2. Experimental Design

The duration of the target words’ stressed syllables in both broad and contrastive foci, both in subject and object positions in SVO-sentences, constitutes the focus of this study. Words in subject and object position were chosen to obtain data from two positions in the utterance, namely initial and final. If greater duration is used to mark contrastive focus, the results should be comparable in the two positions. For example, in his study of Madrid Spanish, Face (2002) found differences in the duration of the stressed syllable between broad and contrastive focus in all utterance positions considered in his study (i.e., initial, medial, and final). Consequently, we would expect that utterance-final lengthening does not play a role in the results of words in object position. In addition, we predict comparable results for subjects (in utterance-initial position) and objects (in utterance-final position). Moreover, although a certain degree of variation was expected, it was also expected that the most frequent word order would be SVO, meaning that subjects appear in utterance-initial position while objects appear at the end of the utterance.
Participants were shown a slide presentation intended to elicit broad or contrastive foci responses for words in subject and object positions. First, participants were shown an example of a slide, similar to that in Figure 2. Then, they were given several suggestions about possible ways to answer. They were instructed to give only one answer, which should be a whole sentence, and to answer it in a natural way; they were also told that there were no incorrect answers. Second, they were shown two trial examples (similar to that of Figure 2), one intended to elicit (subject or object) contrastive focus and the other one, broad focus. Finally, they proceeded to answer to the questions of the task proper.
Figure 2 is an example of a pair of slides that the participants were shown. In this example, contrastive focus for the word in subject position is elicited. The procedure was as follows: first, a participant would see an image (Figure 2) where the three lexical items corresponding to the action taking place would appear. Thus, a verb in infinitive form plus two names preceded by the corresponding definite article (one for the subject and the other for the object) appeared on this slide so that the participant would use them. It was made sure that the order and position in which the three lexical items were shown on the slide varied as much as possible across slides so as to avoid a reading style. This reading style could have appeared if the words had always been written in the same order (i.e., from top to bottom, the words for the subject, the verb, and the object). Providing the words in written form was considered a necessary compromise between spontaneity and the elicitation of the same lexical items from all participants. In the next step, one of the researchers asked the participant a question related to the image provided, this time without any written text. Second, the question in Figure 2 (below) aimed at eliciting contrastive focus for the subject: El abogado está abriendo el regalo, ¿verdad? “The lawyer is opening the present, right?” One possible answer to this question would be: (No,) la bailarina está abriendo el regalo “No, the dancer is opening the present.” Slides intended to elicit broad focus had the same structure, but the question used was always the same (¿Qué pasa aquí? “What’s happening here?”).
Broad and contrastive questions were presented in a pseudo-randomized order to control for habituation effects and also to avoid a reading style. Target words for subjects (abogado ‘lawyer’, bailarina ‘dancer’, enfermera ‘nurse’, marinero ‘sailor’, vendedora ‘seller’; stressed syllables are indicated in boldface) referred to occupations; objects (ballena ‘whale’, bombero ‘firefighter’, granada ‘pomegranate’, merengue ‘meringue’) were of a more varied nature. In the broad focus context, target words included both subjects and objects; also, more target words were used for the subject condition, which accounts for the fact that there are more instances of subject target words in the broad focus than in the contrastive focus condition. There was another subject target word (panadero ‘baker’) which was not taken into account in the present study because examples of it appeared only in one of the focus conditions. Similarly, other words were also used for objects, but were excluded because they appeared in only one focus condition. In sum, 27 questions that elicited subject target words (17 for broad focus, 10 for contrastive focus) and 8 that elicited object target words (4 for broad focus, 4 for contrastive focus) were taken into account in this investigation.
Crucially, what was measured were not the target words, but the stressed syllables of said target words (e.g., “ri” in bailarina). Target word is used in the article for ease of explanation. All target words were trisyllabic, paroxytone words (the paroxytone stress pattern is the most common in Spanish); thus, the position of the target syllables in the word, and ultimately in the utterance, was further controlled. Although the segmental make-up of the studied syllables differs, it allows for an analysis of pitch characteristics. This possibility is not undertaken in this study.
Recordings (44.1 kHz, 16 bit, wav) were made in a silent room with an AKG C 544 L head-mounted microphone connected to a Presonus Audiobox USB in the presence of at least one of the researchers.

2.3. Data Analysis

Recordings were orthographically transcribed by a native speaker of Yucatecan Spanish. They were then automatically aligned using BAS Pipeline online service (v. 2.22; Kisler et al. 2017). Later, they were manually corrected by two labelers in Praat (v. 6.0.43; Boersma and Weenink 2009) under the supervision of the first author, who also conducted final corrections on the labels to ensure accuracy and coherence in the measurement. Target syllables were labeled based on visual inspection of the spectrograms and waveforms; the boundaries were put at the nearest zero crossing on the waveform. For the extraction of the duration, a script (Hirst 2009) was used. Figure 3 shows an example of the labeling performed.
Of the possible total of 918 answers for target syllables in subject position and 272 in object position, 54 (5.9 %) and 10 (3.7 %) were excluded, respectively. Syllables were excluded because (i) a different word instead of the target word was uttered (e.g., señor ‘man’ instead of abogado ‘lawyer’; 4 in utterances where the target word appeared in subject position, 0 in object position); (ii) participants hesitated in a way that could influence the duration of the target word (16 in subject position, 1 in object position); or (iii) participants reversed the roles of subject and object (e.g., El bombero está llamando a la bailarina ‘The firefighter is calling the dancer’ instead of La bailarina está llamando al bombero ‘The dancer is calling the firefighter’; 10 in subject position, 7 in object position). In addition, several utterances were excluded for syntactic reasons, specifically because of clefting (e.g., No, es la vendedora la que vende el helado ‘No, it is the seller who is selling the ice cream’; 5 in subject position, 0 in object position), fronting (e.g., No, la vela la está vendiendo la vendedora ‘No, the candle is being sold by the seller’; 12 in subject position, 1 in object position), or because they used a VOS word order (Lava la herida la enfermera ‘Washes the wound the nurse’; 4 in subject position, 1 in object position).
Sentences where the indefinite article was used (e.g., Una enfermera está lavando una herida ‘A nurse is washing a wound’) were also kept. The indefinite and definite articles in Spanish differ in that the first ones are lexically stressed (Hualde 2009; Quilis 1999). In principle, this difference should not affect the target syllable. Nevertheless, previous studies have reported that the indefinite article can receive accentual prominence instead of the word it complements (Muntendam and Torreira 2016). Consequently, all contrastive focus sentences with indefinite articles were further checked auditorily by the first author, a native speaker of (European) Spanish, to make sure that no such cases would be included in the analysis. As a result, 3 target syllables (all uttered by the same speaker in subject position) were excluded. In sum, the analysis was conducted on a total of 864 stressed syllables in subject position and on 262 in object position.

3. Results

We measured the duration of the stressed syllables of the 1126 target words in subject position (864 syllables) and object position (262 syllables) in SVO sentences in broad and contrastive focus sentences uttered by 34 speakers of Yucatecan Spanish with different degrees of Maya–Spanish dominance according to BLP scores (Birdsong et al. 2012; see Section 2.1). The measurements were submitted to two separate generalized linear mixed-effects models (one for subject position, another for object position) using the lmer function in the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015) in R (version 3.5.1; R Core Team 2018), with syllable duration (in ms) as the (continuous) dependent variable, and with the following independent variables: focus condition (categorical: broad vs. contrastive), BLP score (continuous), gender (categorical: male vs. female), and age (interval). Random intercepts and random slopes for focus were fitted both for speakers and target syllables. Satterthwaite approximations to degrees of freedom were used to calculate p values. We also checked for two-way interactions between BLP score and focus by means of a likelihood ratio test that compared the model that included the interaction with the model without the interaction.

3.1. Results for Subject Position

Table 2 shows the mean duration and standard deviation for each stressed syllable in subject position.
The model for subject position shows that there is no effect of focus nor of BLP score (Table 3). Older speakers seem to utter longer target syllables. There is no interaction between BLP score and focus (likelihood ratio test: p = 0.44). Figure 4 provides information about the duration of each stressed syllable in broad and contrastive focus conditions. Figure 5 shows the duration of said syllables in broad and contrastive focus conditions per speaker. Instances of stressed syllables are represented by circles, blue for broad focus condition and orange for contrastive focus condition. Each vertical line represents a speaker. “Blue” and “orange” circles do not show any pattern for any of the speakers. The horizontal line represents the mean duration value for stressed syllables in broad focus condition (in blue) and contrastive focus condition (in orange) as a function of BLP score. Both the representation for each speaker and the mean duration values show the lack of interaction between BLP score and focus.

3.2. Results for Object Position

Table 4 shows the mean duration and standard deviation for each stressed syllable in object position.
The model for object position shows that there is no effect of focus nor of BLP score. There is a difference between female and male speakers, with male speakers producing shorter target syllables. There is no interaction between BLP score and focus (likelihood ratio test: p = 0.52). Figure 6 provides information about the duration of each stressed syllable in broad and contrastive focus conditions. Figure 7 shows the duration of said syllables in broad and contrastive focus conditions per speaker.

3.3. Summary of Results

Table 2 and Table 4 and Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7 show that the mean duration of almost all stressed syllables is longer in contrastive than in broad focus. Nevertheless, they also show large standard deviations and great inter-speaker variation. The mixed-effects models for the duration of stressed syllables in subject (utterance-initial) and object (utterance-final) position yield the same result: speakers of Yucatecan Spanish do not employ post-lexical duration (Table 3 and Table 5).
In summary, the results in the previous subsections provide the following answers to the three hypotheses of the study (see Section 1): (i) Speakers of Yucatecan Spanish do not use duration (of the stressed syllable) as a post-lexical acoustic cue in order to mark contrastive focus, neither in subject nor in object position; (ii) knowledge of Maya does not seem to play a direct role in how speakers differ with respect to their use of post-lexical duration, neither in subject nor in object position; finally, (iii) some differences are observed in regard to age (older speakers utter longer syllables in subject position, but not in object position) and gender (male speakers utter shorter target syllables in object position, but not in subject position). In sum, hypothesis (i) is supported by the results, whereas hypothesis (ii) is not. Hypothesis (iii) is only partially supported by the results, with age and gender having some effect on post-lexical duration.

4. Discussion

Keeping in mind Baird’s (2017) argumentation about Nahualá K’ichee’ and Gutiérrez-Bravo et al.’s (2019) work on the syntactic transfer of Yucatec Mayan focus fronting constructions into Yucatecan Spanish, the obvious question is if the lack of post-lexical duration in Yucatecan Spanish may be related to cross-linguistic influence from Yucatec Maya, and, if yes, how exactly this influence may have originated across the years. On the one hand, the situation of Yucatecan Spanish is analogous to the Nahualá case in that in both contact situations there is an indigenous contact language with phonemic vowel length and a Spanish contact variety that does not show any effects of post-lexical duration. However, contrary to what Baird (2017) reports for the female Nahualá speakers, the bilingual speakers of our experiment were balanced bilinguals, and as such, they were as highly proficient in Spanish as they were in Yucatec Maya, meaning that no effect could be attributed to a reduced knowledge of Spanish to begin with.
Moreover, the lack of a direct effect of the Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores on the duration variable shows that the phenomenon at stake here cannot be attributed to any particular degree of Maya proficiency of the current speakers either. However, it is important to note that the region at stake here was entirely Maya-dominated until the end of the Caste War in 1901.3 Indigenous uprisings continued on a smaller scale for several decades (Grube 2000, p. 420; Reed 1968), and the supra-regional activities in the regional commercial sector were considerably curtailed by the resistance of large parts of the Mayan population (see, e.g., Bracamonte y Sosa 1986), the socialist movement, and land reforms by Salvador Alvarado (1915–1918) and Felipe Carrillo Puerto (1922–1924) (Higuera Bonfil 1986; Joseph 2003; Scheuzger 2009, among others). Most importantly for the present purposes, Spanish schools were not implemented in the region until the 1930s for various reasons (Eiss 2004; Fallaw 2004; Hostettler 1999). Consequently, the institution-driven hispanization of the area only started around 1940–1950, meaning that, although they have mastered Spanish at a native or near-native level, the majority of the current population of Felipe Carrillo Puerto acquired Spanish either as a second language (L2) or as a first language (L1) from L2 speakers or immediate descendants of L2 speakers. Experts in language contact studies such as Winford (2014), Thomason (2001), and Van Coetsem (1988), among many others, assume that this type of language shift scenario favours phenomena related to “second-language acquisition strategies” (Thomason 2001, p. 60), including interlanguage phenomena and transfer (Thomason 2001, p. 60), due to the fact that the strategies emanate from the speakers of the source language. From this perspective, and in view of the particularity of the lack of post-lexical syllable duration in this variety compared to other varieties of Spanish (see Section 1), it is still probable that the particular nature of the syntax–prosody interface of the corresponding Yucatec Maya speakers did indeed have an impact on the prosodic realization of contrastive focus in the Spanish variety that is spoken by the current Spanish speakers of that region. Since the Spanish of the region has been influenced, and shaped, by three generations of Maya speakers, at least the first generation of which was definitely dominant in Yucatec Maya, the lack of post-lexical duration may very well have originated as a second language interference phenomenon and may then have passed on to the native Spanish of the subsequent generations during their acquisition of Spanish as a first language, so that it developed into an integral part of what is nowadays called Yucatecan Spanish due to cross-linguistic influence (without there being any current influence of the contact language on post-lexical syllable duration in Yucatecan Spanish).
Another intricate issue concerns the relation between the lack of post-lexical duration and the apparent importance of syntactic means of focalization in Yucatecan Spanish suggested by the syntactic investigation of Gutiérrez-Bravo et al. (2019). In view of the patterns reported in this latter work, it might be questioned whether the lack of post-lexical duration in our experiment is to be attributed to a cross-linguistic phonological influence as described above, or if it is rather due to the high(er) importance of syntactic focus marking, thus being an indirect effect of another contact-related particularity. However, note, first of all, that the speakers in our experiment did not show any marked preference for syntactic strategies of focus marking (a fact that is probably due to the rather controlled, and hence formal, nature of the experimental setting, although this did not hinder the use of some non-SVO sentences by some participants). Moreover, the intricate relation between the lack of post-lexical duration and the preference for syntactic over prosodic focus marking does obviously hold for both Yucatecan Spanish and Yucatec Maya alike, both features being generally considered two sides of the same coin in the works on focus realization in Yucatec Maya (see Gussenhoven and Teeuw 2008; Kügler et al. 2007, among others). Thus, it may be argued that this interrelation extended from Yucatec Maya to Yucatecan Spanish together with the two isolate features or tendencies, respectively, meaning that all three of them are best to be considered as cross-linguistic particularities of the entire region, comparable to those non-genealogically determined similarities between neighboring languages that have been uncovered in the tradition of areal linguistics for various linguistic areas (see, e.g., (Bisang 2006; Campbell 2006; Hickey 2017), and the references cited therein).
It is true that, since our study leads to a negative result (i.e., that speakers of Yucatecan Spanish do not seem to use post-lexical duration), it is notably confronted with the issue of validity. That is, it might be objected that the speakers simply failed to show effects of post-lexical duration, since they did not properly re-enact the intended pragmatic condition/s (particularly in the context of correction and contrastivity). Although this problem may never be entirely excluded in semi-spontaneous production tasks, we have reasons not to be too pessimistic in this regard. A crucial piece of evidence has already been alluded to in Section 2.3, where we saw that several utterances were disregarded because they did not comply with the SVO order. The largest group of utterances was that of fronted constituents (12 where the target word was in subject position, 1 in object position), followed by cleft constructions (5 in subject position). The fact that other ways of marking focus have been used in the corpus is an index of the degree of spontaneity obtained in spite of the somehow experimental task. This provides further validity to the results of the study. Thus, we are inclined to think that the study does indeed provide valid evidence against the use of post-lexical duration in Yucatecan Spanish.
The Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores of the participants (Figure 1) show that bilingual speakers’ knowledge of Maya and Spanish was arguably balanced. As indicated in Section 2.1, BLP scores were computed after the recordings were made; also, literacy in Spanish was required to complete the task. Consequently, people who could speak both Maya and Spanish but that were not literate in the second could not be included. It is unclear whether having such participants in the study would have had an effect on the results. Although we cannot exclude this possibility, the number of participants (seven) with a “—score” (i.e., more dominant in Maya) may be large enough to sustain our interpretation of the results.
Age and gender deserve further discussion. For the duration of syllables in subject position, older speakers seem to utter longer target syllables. This could be easily explained if we were to pose that older speakers have a slower speech rate; however, this interpretation is contradicted by the results for object position, for which no such tendency is observed. In addition, in object position, male speakers seem to produce shorter target syllables than female speakers; however, this is not paralleled in the subject group, nor does there seem to be an immediate explanation for it. Baird (2017) also found different results for female and male speakers (see Section 1), but he related it to female speakers having a better knowledge of K’ichee’; this situation is not the same as ours.
Our study takes into account the duration of stressed syllables of paroxytone words in subject and object position exclusively. Breen et al. (2010) consider that studying only one acoustic feature of single words is an important limitation in the study of the relationship between prosody and information structure because it leaves out the context in which the target words appear (p. 1052). In this line of argumentation, it could be considered that duration—and also other acoustic cues—should be measured not only for the stressed syllable, but also for the word it appears in and even in a broader context such as the prosodic constituent. However, the work by De la Mota Gorriz (1997) already reviewed in Section 1 did not only take into consideration the whole utterance, but also several F0 measures, and still found differences in duration between contrastive and non-contrastive (broad) focus. Nevertheless, in a multidimensional view it is the conjunction of several cues which may indicate differences in focus marking. Even if duration by itself is not enough to distinguish between broad and contrastive focus, be it absolute (such as in this study) or relative (see, e.g., Muntendam and Torreira 2016, or Van Rijswijk et al. 2017) it may do so if duration is taken into consideration with other acoustic cues such as intensity, F0 height or alignment, and also by taking into account the context. This possibility could be addressed in further research.
Finally, a complementary possibility is the insight that a perceptual study might provide. If indeed there are prosodic differences between broad and contrastive focus, even if they are not durational ones, or differences from the conjunction of duration and other acoustic cues, they may be apparent to Yucatecan Spanish listeners. Further research may shed some light on this.

5. Conclusions

In this article, we dealt with the duration of stressed syllables in broad versus contrastive focus in Yucatecan Spanish and discussed its connection with Spanish–Maya bilingualism. We studied the duration of stressed syllables of nouns in subject and object position in 1126 SVO sentences (broad and contrastive focus) of a semi-spontaneous production task run with 34 mono- and bilingual speakers in Quintana Roo, Mexico. The main result of the study is that, although slightly longer in contrastive focus, syllable duration turned out not to be a significant variable in our data set, suggesting that duration does not constitute a cue to focus marking in Yucatecan Spanish. Finally, it was discussed how this result relates to the strong influence of Yucatec Maya on Yucatecan Spanish prosody observed by both scholars and native speakers of Yucatecan Spanish and other Mexican varieties of Spanish. In particular, the discussion concentrated on the claim that phonemic vowel length in a language might prevent the use of syllable duration as a post-lexical acoustic cue in another, as suggested by Baird (2017) for the variety of Nahualá Spanish spoken by the Maya-dominant women of the corresponding Guatemalan community. However, since the arguments in favor of such an interdependence are less conclusive in the case of Yucatecan Spanish than in the Nahualá case, we argued that the most evident conclusion that may be drawn from our analysis would rather be that the lack of post-lexical duration in Yucatecan Spanish is a regional feature that (i) is characteristic of both Yucatec Maya and Yucatecan Spanish alike, and that (ii) originated as a second language interference phenomenon and then passed on to the native (Yucatecan) Spanish of the subsequent generations during their acquisition of Spanish as a first language. Interestingly, Yucatecan Spanish has been argued to be similar to Yucatec Maya in that (syntactic) focus fronting is rather high and seems to show Mayan influence in this variety. From this perspective, it might be interesting to pursue the hypothesis that the cross-linguistic influence extends to the entire prosody–syntax interface, and that the corresponding interrelated features are cross-linguistic particularities non-genealogically determined of the entire region. To us, this seems an interesting hypothesis to be pursued in future research.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.M.G. and M.U.; Data curation, N.M.G.; Formal analysis, N.M.G. and M.U.; Funding acquisition, M.U.; Investigation, N.M.G. and M.U.; Methodology, N.M.G. and M.U.; Project administration, M.U.; Supervision, M.U.; Visualization, N.M.G.; Writing—original draft, N.M.G. and M.U.; Writing—review & editing, N.M.G. and M.U.

Funding

This research is funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) as part of the CRC 1252 “Prominence in Language”, project A5 “Prominence marking and language contact in Spanish”, directed by Melanie Uth.

Acknowledgments

This paper was presented at the Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World conference, held in Leiden University on 9–11 January 2019. The authors are most grateful to the audience of the conference and to the two reviewers of its abstract for their feedback. We also wish to thank the two anonymous reviewers of the present article for their highly valuable comments and suggestions. Finally, we thank Patrick Auhagen (for the annotation and correction of the recordings, as well as for the modification of the task pictures), Leonard Rick (for the annotation of the recordings), and Anna Wördehoff (for the correction of the transcriptions).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Participants’ Bilingual Language Profile scores (BLP scores), gender, age, and number of observations (both for subject and object positions). BLP scores quantify language dominance; each end of the score corresponds to the highest dominance for Maya (−218) and Spanish (+218), respectively. Values around 0 indicate balanced bilingualism.
Table A1. Participants’ Bilingual Language Profile scores (BLP scores), gender, age, and number of observations (both for subject and object positions). BLP scores quantify language dominance; each end of the score corresponds to the highest dominance for Maya (−218) and Spanish (+218), respectively. Values around 0 indicate balanced bilingualism.
Number of Observations
SubjectObject
Participant IDBLP ScoreGenderAgeBroad FocusContrastive FocusBroad FocusContrastive Focus
0195.994male22171044
02153.742male18161044
03129.86female20171044
0465.754female3117104 4
05101.618male2118 94 4
0637.146female841694 4
07202.05female541794 4
0896.444male7213103 4
10158.92male3816104 4
11195.24female671610 2 2
12178.896male34142 4 4
13177.08female361794 4
149.992male461693 3
15−26.246female4316 94 4
17−45.952female3016104 4
18−23.702 female381710 4 4
19−24.426female34176 4 4
20−20.434male471594 3
22−25.146male381774 4
2399.172female3716104 4
24141.668male471673 3
2535.334 female441794 4
2656.764female411710 4 4
2722.798female5817 104 4
28177.08female6317 10 4 4
29159.828male6615104 4
30157.104male4617 104 4
31179.804female3417 104 4
3249.858 male331694 4
3669.658female261764 4
3889.906male1717104 4
394.99 male2015104 4
40−9.984male1817104 4
4145.228female431794 4

References

  1. Baird, Brandon O. 2014. Acoustic Correlates of Stress in K’ichee’: A Preliminary Investigation. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 74: 21–34. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baird, Brandon O. 2017. Prosodic Transfer among Spanish-K’ichee’ Bilinguals. In Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World. Edited by Kate Bellamy, Michael W. Child, Paz González, Antje Muntendam and M. Carmen Parafita Couto. Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistic 13. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 149–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barnes, Hilary, and Jim Michnowicz. 2013. Peak Alignment in Semispontaneous Bilingual Chipilo Spanish. In Selected Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Spanish. Edited by Ana M. Carvalho and Sara Beaudrie. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 109–22. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrera Vásquez, Alfredo. 1977. El idioma español en Yucatán. In Enciclopedia Yucatanense 6. Yucatán Actual, 2nd ed. Edited by Luis H. Hoyos Villanueva, Renan Irigoyen Rosado, Rodolfo Ruz Menéndez and Humberto Lara y Lara. Ciudad de México: Gobierno de Yucatán, pp. 341–75. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Birdsong, David, Libby M. Gertken, and Marc Amengual. 2012. Bilingual Language Profile: An Easy-to-Use Instrument to Assess Bilingualism. Austin: COERLL, University of Texas at Austin, Available online: https://sites.la.utexas.edu/bilingual/ (accessed on 19 December 2017).
  7. Bisang, Walter. 2006. Linguistic Areas, Language Contact and Typology: Some Implications from the Case of Ethiopia as a Linguistic Area. In Linguistic Areas: Convergence in Historical and Typological Perspective. Edited by Yaron Matras, April McMahon and Nigel Vincent. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, pp. 75–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2009. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version 6.0.43): [Computer Program]. Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 10 December 2018).
  9. Bracamonte y Sosa, Pedro. 1986. La montaña y la resistencia de los mayas yucatecos frente al colonialismo español. In Quintana Roo: Procesos Políticos y Democracia. Edited by Maria Cristina Castro, Gabriel Macías, Antonio Higuera and Luz del Carmen Vallarta. México: Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social, pp. 39–63. [Google Scholar]
  10. Breen, Mara, Evelina Fedorenko, Michael Wagner, and Edward Gibson. 2010. Acoustic Correlates of Information Structure. Language and Cognitive Processes 25: 1044–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Büring, Daniel. 2009. Towards a Typology of Focus Realization. In Information Structure. Edited by Malte Zimmermann and Caroline Féry. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 177–205. [Google Scholar]
  12. Campbell, Lyle. 2006. Areal Linguistics: A Closer Scrutiny. In Linguistic Areas: Convergence in Historical and Typological Perspective. Edited by Yaron Matras, April McMahon and Nigel Vincent. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, pp. 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Van Coetsem, Frans. 1988. Loan Phonology and the Two Transfer Types in Language Contact. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cruschina, Silvio. 2012. Discourse-Related Features and Functional Projections. Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Domínguez, Laura. 2004. Mapping Focus: The Syntax and Prosody of Focus in Spanish. Boston: Boston University. [Google Scholar]
  16. Eiss, Paul K. 2004. Deconstructing Indians, Reconstructing Patria: Indigenous Education in Yucatan from the Porfiriato to the Mexican Revolution. Journal of Latin American Anthropology 9: 119–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. England, Nora C., and Brandon O. Baird. 2017. Phonology and Phonetics. In The Mayan Languages. Edited by Judith Aissen, Nora C. England and Roberto Zavala Maldonado. Routledge Language Family Series; New York: Routledge, pp. 175–200. [Google Scholar]
  18. Face, Timothy L. 2000. Prosodic Manifestations of Focus in Spanish. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 19: 45–62. [Google Scholar]
  19. Face, Timothy L. 2002. Intonational Marking of Contrastive Focus in Madrid Spanish. Muenchen: LINCOM Europa. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fallaw, Ben. 2004. Rethinking Mayan Resistance: Changing Relations between Federal Teachers and Mayan Communities in Eastern Yucatan, 1929–1935. Journal of Latin American Anthropology 9: 151–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Frazier, Melissa. 2009. The Production and Perception of Pitch and Glottalization in Yucatec Maya. Ph.D. thesis, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA. [Google Scholar]
  22. García Fajardo, Josefina. 1984. Fonética Del Español de Valladolid, Yucatán. México City: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Investigaciones Filológicas. [Google Scholar]
  23. García-Lecumberri, María Luisa. 1995. Intonational Signalling of Information Structure in English and Spanish: A Comparative Study. London: University of London, Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10068/448827 (accessed on 1 September 2018).
  24. Gobierno del Estado de Quintana Roo-Secretaría de Desarrollo Social. 2010. Preservación de la Cultura y la Lengua Maya. Available online: http://www.coneval.org.mx/sitios/RIEF/Documents/Quintanaroo-diagnostico-lengua-cultura-maya-2010.pdf (accessed on 20 December 2017).
  25. Grube, Nikolai. 2000. Maya heute—Vom entrechteten Indio zum Indianischen Aufbruch. In Maya: Gottkönige im Regenwald. Edited by Nikolai Grube. Köln: Könemann, pp. 416–26. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2007. Types of Focus in English. In Topic and Focus. Studies in Edited by Chungmin Lee, Matthew Gordon and Daniel Büring. Linguistics and Philosophy 82. Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 83–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gussenhoven, Carlos, and Renske Teeuw. 2008. A Moraic and a Syllabic H-Tone in Yucatec Maya. In Fonología Instrumental: Patrones Fónicos y Variación. Edited by Esther Z. Herrera and Pedro Martín Butragueño. México: El Colegio de México, pp. 49–71. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gutiérrez Bravo, Rodrigo. 2008. La Identificación de Los Tópicos y Los Focos. Nueva Revista de Filología Hispánica 56: 363–401. Available online: https://www.doi.org/10.24201/nrfh.v56i2.969 (accessed on 10 October 2017).
  29. Gutiérrez Bravo, Rodrigo. 2015. El Foco de Verbo En Maya Yucateco. In Nuevos Estudios de Teoría de La Optimidad. Sintaxis, Estudios Fónicos y Lingüística Computacional. Edited by Rodrigo Gutiérrez Bravo, Francisco Arellanes Arellanes and Mario Ernesto Chávez Peón Herrero. México: El Colegio de México, pp. 17–48. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gutiérrez-Bravo, Rodrigo, Carlos Martín Sobrino, and Melanie Uth. 2019. Contrastive Focus in Yucatecan Spanish. In The Syntactic Variation of Spanish Dialects. Edited by Ángel J. Gallego. Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 275–96. [Google Scholar]
  31. Halliday, Michael A. K. 1967. Notes on Transitivity and Theme in English: Part 2. Journal of Linguistics 3: 199–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hickey, Raymond. 2017. The Cambridge Handbook of Areal Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Higuera Bonfil, Antonio. 1986. La penetración protestante en la zona maya de Quintana Roo. In Quintana Roo: Procesos Políticos y Democracia. Edited by Ma. Cristina Castro, Gabriel Macías, Antonio Higuera and Luz del Carmen Vallarta. México: Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropología Social, pp. 51–66. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hirst, Daniel. 2009. Analyse Tier.Praat (Version 2009-08-07). Available online: http://uk.groups.yahoo.com/group/praat-users/files/Daniel_Hirst/analyse_tier.praat (accessed on 1 February 2011).
  35. Hostettler, Ueli. 1999. ‘Supimos ayudar en algo al progreso del pueblo’: Maestros y escuelas rurales en comunidades del centro de Quintana Roo, México. In Interculturalidad e Identidad Indígena: Preguntas Abiertas a la Globalización en México. Edited by Barbara Pfeiler. Hannover: Verlag für Ethnologie, pp. 237–70. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hualde, José Ignacio. 2009. Unstressed Words in Spanish. Language Sciences 31: 199–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía-INEGI. 2015. Encuesta Intercensal (2015). Panorama Sociodemográfico de México. Available online: http://seieg.iplaneg.net/seieg/doc/Panorama_Sociodemografico_2015_1452886126.pdf (accessed on 13 July 2019).
  38. Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas-INALI. 2010. Población de 5 años y más Hablante de Alguna Lengua Indígena por Familia y Agrupación Lingüística para los Años 2000, 2005 y 2010. Available online: https://www.inali.gob.mx/es/transparencia/datos-abiertos.html (accessed on 13 July 2019).
  39. Joseph, Gilbert M. 2003. Revolution from without. Yucatán, Mexico, and the United States, 1880–1924, 3rd ed. Duke University: Durham. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kim, Sahyang, and Heriberto Avelino. 2003. An Intonational Study of Focus and Word Order Variation in Mexican Spanish. In La Tonía: Dimensiones Fonéticas y Fonológicas. Edited by Herrera Z. Esther and Pedro Martín Butragueño. Estudios de Lingüística 4. México: El Colegio de México, pp. 357–74. [Google Scholar]
  41. Kisler, Thomas, Uwe Reichel, and Florian Schiel. 2017. Multilingual Processing of Speech via Web Services. Computer Speech & Language 45: 326–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kügler, Frank, and Stavros Skopeteas. 2007. On the Universality of Prosodic Reflexes of Contrast: The Case of Yucatec Maya. Paper presented at 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Saarbrücken, Germany, August 6–10; pp. 1025–28. Available online: https://pub.uni-bielefeld.de//luur/download?func=downloadFile&recordOId=2143409&fileOId=2276179 (accessed on 10 October 2017).
  43. Kügler, Frank, Stavros Skopeteas, and Elisabeth Verhoeven. 2007. Encoding Information Structure in Yucatec Maya: On the Interplay of Prosody and Syntax. Interdisciplinary Studies on Information Structure 8: 187–208. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lehmann, Christian. 1990. Yukatekisch. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 9: 28–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lipski, John M. 2004. A History of Afro-Hispanic Language: Five Centuries, Five Continents. New York: Cambridge Univ. [Google Scholar]
  46. Llisterri, Joaquim, María J. Machuca, Antonio Ríos, and Sandra Schwab. 2016. La percepción del acento léxico en un contexto oracional. Loquens 3: e033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lope Blanch, Juan M. 1987. Estudios Sobre el español de Yucatán. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. [Google Scholar]
  48. López, Luis. 2009. A Derivational Syntax for Information Structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Martínez Huchim, Ana Patricia. 2004. Diccionario Maya: Español-Maya, Maya-Español, 2nd ed.Mérida: Dante. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mediz Bolio, Antonio. 1951. Interinfluencia del Maya con el Español de Yucatán. Mérida: Zamná. [Google Scholar]
  51. Michnowicz, James Casimir. 2006. Linguistic and Social Variables in Yucatan Spanish. Ph.D. thesis, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA. Available online: https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/files/final_submissions/2585 (accessed on 10 October 2017).
  52. Michnowicz, Jim. 2006. Final -m in Yucatan Spanish. A Rapid and Anonymous Survey. In New Perspectives on Romance Linguistics: Vol. II: Phonetics, Phonology and Dialectology. Selected Papers from the 35th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL), Austin, Texas, February 2005. Edited by Jean-Pierre Y. Montreuil. Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 276. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Michnowicz, Jim. 2007. El Habla de Yucatám: Final [m] in a Dialect in Contact. In Selected Proceedings of the Third Workshop on Spanish Sociolinguistics. Edited by Jonathan Holmquist, Augusto Lorenzino and Lotfi Sayahi. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 38–43. Available online: http://www.lingref.com/cpp/wss/3/paper1524.pdf (accessed on 9 July 2019).
  54. Michnowicz, Jim. 2008. Final Nasal Variation in Merida, Yucatan. Spanish in Context 5: 278–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Michnowicz, Jim. 2015. Maya-Spanish Contact in Yucatan, Mexico: Context and Sociolinguistic Implications. In New Perspectives on Hispanic Contact Linguistics in the Americas. Edited by Sandro Sessarego and Melvin González-Rivera. Madrid: Iberoamericana/Vervuert, pp. 21–42. [Google Scholar]
  56. Michnowicz, Jim, and Hilary Barnes. 2013. A Sociolinguistic Analysis of Pre-Nuclear Peak Alignment in Yucatan Spanish. In Selected Proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Edited by Chad Howe, Sarah E. Blackwell and Margaret Lubbers Quesada. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 221–35. [Google Scholar]
  57. Michnowicz, Jim, and Lindsey Carpenter. 2013. Voiceless Stop Aspiration in Yucatan Spanish: A Sociolinguistic Analysis. Spanish in Context 10: 410–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Michnowicz, Jim, and Laura Kagan. 2016. On Glottal Stops in Yucatan Spanish. In Spanish Language and Sociolinguistic Analysis. Edited by Sandro Sessarego and Fernando Tejedo-Herrero. Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistic 8. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 217–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. De-la-Mota, Carme, Pedro Martín Butragueño, and Pilar Prieto. 2010. Mexican Spanish Intonation. In Transcription of Intonation of the Spanish Language. Edited by Pilar Prieto and Paolo Roseano. München: Lincom Europa, pp. 319–50. [Google Scholar]
  60. De la Mota Gorriz, Carme. 1995. La Representación Gramatical de la información Nueva en el Discurso; Bellaterra: Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10803/4883 (accessed on 18 August 2019).
  61. De la Mota Gorriz, Carme. 1997. Prosody of Sentences with Contrastive New Information in Spanish. In Intonation: Theory, Models, and Applications. Athens: European Speech Communication Association, pp. 75–78. [Google Scholar]
  62. Muntendam, Antje, and Francisco Torreira. 2016. Focus and Prosody in Spanish and Quechua: Insights from an Interactive Task. In Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Subfields. Edited by Meghan E. Armstrong, Nicholas Henriksen and Maria del Mar Vanrell. Issues in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 6. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 69–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Pfeiler Blaha, Barbara. 1992. Así som, los de Yucatam: El proceso fonológico Vn -> m/__ (#, C) en dos lenguas en contacto. In Memorias Del Primer Congreso Internacional de Mayistas. México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, pp. 110–22. [Google Scholar]
  64. Pike, Kenneth L. 1946. Phonemic Pitch in Maya. International Journal of American Linguistics 12: 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Quilis, Antonio. 1999. Tratado de Fonología y Fonética Españolas, 2nd ed. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
  66. R Core Team. 2018. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 19 December 2017).
  67. Reed, Nelson. 1968. The Caste War of Yucatán. Stanford: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  68. Sainz-Maza Lecanda, Lorena. 2017. Interactions among Focus, Exhaustivity, and Constituent Order in Spanish and Basque; Columbus: Ohio State University. Available online: https://etd.ohiolink.edu/ (accessed on 18 August 2019).
  69. Scheuzger, Stephan. 2009. Der Andere in der Ideologischen Vorstellungskraft: Die Linke und die Indigene Frage in Mexiko. Editionen der Iberoamericana. Serie C: Geschichte und Gesellschaft 12. Frankfurt am Main: Vervuert. [Google Scholar]
  70. Smiljanić, Rajka. 2004. Lexical, Pragmatic, and Positional Effects on Prosody in Two Dialects of Croatian and Serbian. An Acoustic Study. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sobrino, Carlos Martín. 2010. Contacto lingüístico maya-español: Transferencias en la morfosintaxis del español yucateco. Temas Antropológicos 32: 79–94. [Google Scholar]
  72. Stevens, Jon Scott. 2017. Pragmatics of Focus. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Suárez Molina, Víctor M. 1996. El Español que se Habla en Yucatán: Apuntamientos Filológicos, 3rd ed. Revised and Corrected by Miguel Güémez Pineda. Mérida: Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán. [Google Scholar]
  74. Thomason, Sarah G. 2001. Language Contact. An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. [Google Scholar]
  75. Van Rijswijk, Remy, Antje Muntendam, and Ton Dijkstra. 2017. Focus Marking in Dutch by Heritage Speakers of Turkish and Dutch L1 Speakers. Journal of Phonetics 61: 48–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Winford, Donald. 2014. In Search of a Unified Model of Language Contact. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16: 734–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Yager, Kent. 1989. La-m bilabial en posición final absoluta en el español hablado En Mérida, Yucatán (México). Nueva Revista de Filología Hispánica 27: 83–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Zagona, Karen T. 2002. The Syntax of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  79. Zimmermann, Malte, and Edgar Onea. 2011. Focus Marking and Focus Interpretation. Focus Marking Strategies and Focus Interpretation 121: 1651–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Zubizarreta, María Luisa. 1998. Prosody, Focus, and Word Order. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  81. Zubizarreta, María Luisa. 1999. Las Funciones informativas: Tema y foco. In Gramática Descriptiva de la Lengua Española, Volume 3. Edited by Ignacio Bosque and Violeta Demonte. Madrid: Espasa Calpe, pp. 4215–44. [Google Scholar]
1
We thank a reviewer for this useful piece of information.
2
Secondly, we might draw our attention to other contact varieties of Spanish, since it has been found that Peruvian Spanish does not seem to have post-lexical duration either (Muntendam and Torreira 2016). However, this issue is beyond the scope of the present paper so we have decided to tackle it in future research.
3
The war was declared officially over in 1915, but it is generally considered to have ended already when the military governor Francisco Bravo and his troops occupied Chan Santa Cruz (i.e., the present city of Felipe Carrillo Puerto) in 1901.
Figure 1. Bilingual Language Profile scores of participants (N = 34). Each individual score is indicated by a white vertical line.
Figure 1. Bilingual Language Profile scores of participants (N = 34). Each individual score is indicated by a white vertical line.
Languages 04 00084 g001
Figure 2. Example from the production task. Above: slide with lexical items. Below: slide plus researcher’s question to elicit contrastive focus on the subject (Translation: ‘The lawyer is opening the present, right?’).
Figure 2. Example from the production task. Above: slide with lexical items. Below: slide plus researcher’s question to elicit contrastive focus on the subject (Translation: ‘The lawyer is opening the present, right?’).
Languages 04 00084 g002
Figure 3. Example of labeling of the target syllable (ri) in the broad focus utterance La bailarina está ondeando la bandera ‘The dancer is waiving the flag’, uttered by a female speaker.
Figure 3. Example of labeling of the target syllable (ri) in the broad focus utterance La bailarina está ondeando la bandera ‘The dancer is waiving the flag’, uttered by a female speaker.
Languages 04 00084 g003
Figure 4. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in subject position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions.
Figure 4. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in subject position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions.
Languages 04 00084 g004
Figure 5. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in subject position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions in relation to Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores. Each vertical line corresponds to a participant’s BLP score.
Figure 5. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in subject position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions in relation to Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores. Each vertical line corresponds to a participant’s BLP score.
Languages 04 00084 g005
Figure 6. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in object position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions.
Figure 6. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in object position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions.
Languages 04 00084 g006
Figure 7. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in object position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions in relation to Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores. Each vertical line corresponds to a participant’s BLP score.
Figure 7. Duration (in ms) of the stressed syllables of the target words in object position in broad focus (“bf”) and contrastive focus (“cf”) conditions in relation to Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) scores. Each vertical line corresponds to a participant’s BLP score.
Languages 04 00084 g007
Table 1. Yucatec Maya examples of phonemic short/long vowel contrasts.
Table 1. Yucatec Maya examples of phonemic short/long vowel contrasts.
Short VowelLong Vowel
chich [t͡ʃit͡ʃ] ‘hard’chiich [t͡ʃìːt͡ʃ] ‘granny’
k’an [k’an] ‘yellow’k’áan [k’áːn] ‘hammock’
pik [pik] ‘a bug’piik [pìːk] ‘petticoat’
Table 2. Descriptive results for the stressed syllables (in boldface) of the target words in subject position. Mean duration and standard deviations (SD) are indicated in ms.
Table 2. Descriptive results for the stressed syllables (in boldface) of the target words in subject position. Mean duration and standard deviations (SD) are indicated in ms.
Target SyllablenMean (SD)
abogado
 broad focus32182.51 (59.6)
 contrastive focus29195.1 (49.78)
bailarina
 broad focus129131.82 (30.36)
 contrastive focus89135.54 (33.5)
enfermera
 broad focus126177.04 (49.25)
 contrastive focus65194.32 (42.9)
marinero
 broad focus98191.38 (47.6)
 contrastive focus62206.43 (34.32)
vendedora
 broad focus171182.96 (47.03)
 contrastive focus63179.69 (38.65)
Table 3. Results of the mixed-effects model for stressed syllables in subject position (N = 864).
Table 3. Results of the mixed-effects model for stressed syllables in subject position (N = 864).
Fixed EffectEstimateSEtp
(Intercept)154.7718.558.34<0.001
focus6.625.201.270.23 (n.s.)
BLP score−0.080.07−1.060.30 (n.s.)
gender−3.109.86−0.320.76 (n.s.)
age0.650.302.140.04
focus BLP score0.020.040.600.55 (n.s.)
Table 4. Descriptive results for the stressed syllables (in boldface) of the target words in object position. Mean duration and standard deviations (SD) are indicated in ms.
Table 4. Descriptive results for the stressed syllables (in boldface) of the target words in object position. Mean duration and standard deviations (SD) are indicated in ms.
Target SyllablenMean (SD)
ballena
 broad focus32181.52 (44.61)
 contrastive focus33197.36 (37.95)
bombero
 broad focus31114.22 (26.62)
 contrastive focus31121.12 (25.68)
granada
 broad focus34190.3 (37.56)
 contrastive focus33185.25 (37.65)
merengue
 broad focus34222.81 (34.29)
 contrastive focus34228.9 (40.28)
Table 5. Results of the mixed-effects model for stressed syllables in object position (N = 262).
Table 5. Results of the mixed-effects model for stressed syllables in object position (N = 262).
Fixed EffectEstimateSEtp
(Intercept)174.2725.526.830.02
focus8.606.311.360.21 (n.s.)
BLP score−0.010.06−0.220.82 (n.s.)
gender−22.007.01−3.140.004
age0.340.221.600.12 (n.s.)
focus BLP score−0.030.05−0.610.54 (n.s.)

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

García, N.M.; Uth, M. Lack of Syllable Duration as a Post-Lexical Acoustic Cue in Spanish in Contact with Maya. Languages 2019, 4, 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4040084

AMA Style

García NM, Uth M. Lack of Syllable Duration as a Post-Lexical Acoustic Cue in Spanish in Contact with Maya. Languages. 2019; 4(4):84. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4040084

Chicago/Turabian Style

García, Nuria Martínez, and Melanie Uth. 2019. "Lack of Syllable Duration as a Post-Lexical Acoustic Cue in Spanish in Contact with Maya" Languages 4, no. 4: 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages4040084

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop