The Effects of Code-Switching and Lexical Stress on Vowel Quality and Duration of Heritage Speakers of Spanish
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Is there an effect of code-switching and lexical stress on the vowel quality of heritage speakers of Spanish?
- Is there an effect of code-switching and lexical stress on the vowel duration of heritage speakers of Spanish?
- Is there an effect of distance from the code-switching site on the vowel quality of heritage speakers of Spanish?
- Is there an effect of distance from the code-switching site on the vowel duration of heritage speakers of Spanish?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Experimental Design
- Spanish to English intra-sentential code-switch: Había una vez una joven muy bella (‘There once was a beautiful young woman’) whose mother had died when she was very young.
- English to Spanish intra-sentential code-switch: She spent toda la noche llorando (‘the whole night crying’).
- Spanish to English inter-sentential code-switch: Te enseñaré cómo es mi vida en la ciudad (‘I will show you how my life is like in the city’). No country squirrel could refuse the invitation.
- Spanish to English inter-sentential code-switch: They called her Cinderella. Un día el Príncipe de aquel país anunció… (‘One day the Prince from that country announced …’).
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Normalized F1/F2 Values and Vowel Duration
3.2. Distance from Code-Switching Site
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. El Ratón de Ciudad y la Ardilla de Campo (The City Mouse and Country Squirrel)
Appendix B. El Príncipe Pordiosero (The Beggar Prince)
Appendix C. Cenicienta (Cinderella)
Appendix D. Blancanieves y los Siete Enanitos (Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs)
Appendix E. El Gato con Botas (Puss in Boots)
“Mis hermanos”- dijo él, -”pueden hacer una bonita vida juntando sus bienes, pero por mi parte, después de haberme comido al gato, y hacer unas sandalias con su piel, entonces no me quedará más que morir de hambre.”
“No te preocupes tanto, mi buen dueño. Si me das un bolso, y un par de botas, con las que yo pueda atravesar lodos y zarzales, entonces verás que no eres tan pobre conmigo como te lo imaginas.”
“Majestad, le traigo a usted un conejo enviado por mi noble señor, el Marqués de Andalucía.
“Dile a tu dueño” dijo el rey, “que se lo agradezco mucho, y que estoy muy complacido con su regalo.”
“Si sigues mi consejo, tu fortuna está lista. Todo lo que debes hacer es ir al río a bañarte en el lugar que te enseñaré, y déjame el resto a mí.”
References
- Beaudrie, S.; Fairclough, M. Spanish as a Heritage Language in the US: State of the Field; Georgetown University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Dumitrescu, D.; Piña-Rosales, G. El Español en los Estados Unidos: E Pluribus Unum? Enfoques Multidisciplinarios; Academia Norteamericana de la Lengua Española: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual y Cabo, D. Advances in Spanish as a Heritage Language; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Potowski, K.; Rothman, J. Bilingual Youth: Spanish in English-Speaking Societies; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Rivera-Mills, S.; Villa, D.J. Spanish of the U.S. Southwest: A Language in Transition; Iberoamericana: Madrid, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Roca, A. Spanish in the United States: Linguistic Issues and Challenges; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Roca, A.; Colombi, M.C. Mi Lengua: Spanish as a Heritage Language in the United States; Georgetown University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Beaudrie, S. Research on university-based Spanish heritage language programs in the United States: The current state of affairs. In Spanish as a Heritage Language in the US: State of the Field; Beaudrie, S., Fairclough, M., Eds.; Georgetown University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2012; pp. 203–221. [Google Scholar]
- Lynch, A. The first decade of the heritage Language Journal: A retrospective view of research on heritage languages. Heritage Lang. J. 2014, 11, 224–242. [Google Scholar]
- Magaña, D. From pedagogy to communities: Issues within and beyond the Spanish heritage language classroom. Stud. Hisp. Lusoph. Linguist. 2015, 8, 375–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, S. Current issues in heritage language acquisition. Annu. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 2010, 30, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual y Cabo, D. Issues in Spanish heritage morphosyntax. Stud. Hisp. Lusoph. Linguist. 2015, 8, 389–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potowski, K. Heritage learners of Spanish. In Handbook of Second Language Spanish; Geeslin, K., Ed.; Wiley Blackwell: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 404–422. [Google Scholar]
- Rao, R.; Ronquest, R. The heritage Spanish phonetic/phonological system: Looking back and moving forward. Stud. Hisp. Lusoph. Linguist. 2015, 8, 403–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valdés, G.; Geoffrion-Vinci, M. Heritage language students: The case of Spanish. In The Handbook of Hispanic Sociolinguistics; Díaz-Campos, M., Ed.; Wiley Blackwell: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011; pp. 598–622. [Google Scholar]
- Ronquest, R. An acoustic examination of unstressed vowel reduction in heritage Spanish. In Selected Proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA, 6–9 October 2011; Howe, C., Blackwell, S., Lubbers Quesada, M., Eds.; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 151–171. [Google Scholar]
- Valdés, G. AATSP Professional Development Series Handbook for Teachers K-16; Harcourt College Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Montrul, S.; Foote, R.; Perpiñán, S. Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Lang. Learn. 2008, 58, 503–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual y Cabo, D.; Rothman, J. The (il)logical problem of heritage speaker bilingualism and incomplete acquisition. Appl. Linguist. 2012, 33, 450–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valdés, G. Bilingualism, heritage language learners, and SLA research: Opportunities lost or seized? Mod. Lang. J. 2005, 89, 410–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, S. How native are heritage speakers? A look at gender agreement in Spanish. Heritage Lang. J. 2013, 10, 15–39. [Google Scholar]
- Kupisch, T.; Lein, T.; Barton, D.; Schröder, D.J.; Stangen, I.; Stoehr, A. Acquisition outcomes across domains in adult simultaneous bilinguals with French as weaker and stronger language. J. Fr. Lang. Stud. 2014, 24, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hualde, J.I. Los Sonidos del Español; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Alvord, S.; Rogers, B. Miami-Cuban Spanish vowels in contact. Sociol. Stud. 2014, 8, 139–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Au, T.K.; Knightly, L.M.; Jun, S.A.; Oh, J. Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 238–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Knightly, L.M.; Jun, S.A.; Oh, J.S.; Au, T.K. Production benefits of childhood overhearing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 114, 465–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Au, T.K.; Oh, J.S.; Knightly, L.M.; Jun, S.A.; Romo, L. Salvaging a childhood language. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 58, 998–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oh, J.S.; Jun, S.A.; Knightly, L.M.; Au, T.K. Holding on to childhood language memory. Cognition 2003, 86, B53–B64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hrycyna, M.; Lapinskaya, N.; Kochetov, A.; Nagy, N. VOT drift in 3 generations of heritage language speakers in Toronto. Can. Acoust. 2011, 39, 166–167. [Google Scholar]
- Godson, L. Vowel production in the speech of Western Armenian heritage speakers’. Heritage Lang. J. 2004, 2, 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Saadah, E. The Production of Arabic Vowels by English L2 Learners and Heritage Speakers of Arabic. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, C.B.; Yao, Y.; Haynes, E.F.; Rhodes, R. Production of phonetic and phonological contrast by heritage speakers of Mandarin. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 29, 3964–3980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Willis, E. An initial examination of Southwest Spanish vowels. Southwest J. Linguist. 2005, 24, 185–198. [Google Scholar]
- Quilis, A.; Esgueva, M. Realización de los fonemas vocálicos españoles en posición fonética normal. In Estudios de Fonética; Esgueva, M., Cantarero, M., Eds.; Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas: Madrid, Spain, 1983; pp. 159–251. [Google Scholar]
- Gor, K. Raspberry, not a car: Context predictability and a phonological advantage in early and late learners’ processing of speech in noise. Front. Psychol. 2014, 1449, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lukyanchenko, A.; Gor, K. Perceptual correlates of phonological representations in heritage speakers and L2 learners. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA, 5–7 November 2010; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2011; pp. 414–426. [Google Scholar]
- Silva-Corvalán, C. Language Contact and Change: Spanish in Los Angeles; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Grosjean, F. An attempt to isolate, and then differentiate, transfer and interference. Int. J. Biling. 2011, 16, 11–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simonet, M. Phonetic consequences of dynamic cross-linguistic interference in proficient bilinguals. J. Phon. 2014, 43, 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bullock, B.E.; Toribio, A.J. Trying to hit a moving target: On the sociophonetics of code-switching. In Multidisciplinary Approaches to Code Switching; Isurin, L., Winford, D., de Bot, K., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 189–206. [Google Scholar]
- Balukas, C.; Koops, C. Spanish-English bilingual voice onset time in spontaneous code-switching. Int. J. Biling. 2015, 19, 423–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piccinini, P.; Arvaniti, A. Voice onset time in Spanish-English spontaneous code-switching. J. Phon. 2015, 52, 121–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olson, D.J. The phonetics of insertional code-switching: Suprasegmental analysis and a case for hyper-articulation. Ling. Appl. Biling. 2012, 2, 439–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, S. Second language acquisition and first language loss in adult early bilinguals: Exploring some differences and similarities. Second Lang. Res. 2005, 21, 199–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toribio, A.J. Spanish-English code-switching among US Latinos. Int. J. Sociol. Lang. 2003, 158, 89–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Instituto Cervantes. Diploma de Español como Lengua Extranjera [Assessment Instrument]; Instituto Cervantes: Madrid, Spain, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Montrul, S. The Acquisition of Spanish: Morphosyntactic Development in Monolingual and Bilingual L1 Acquisition and Adult L2 Acquisition; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Cuza, A.; Frank, J. Transfer effects at the syntax-semantics interface: The case of double-que questions in heritage Spanish. Heritage Lang. J. 2011, 8, 66–89. [Google Scholar]
- Colantoni, L.; Cuza, A.; Mazzaro, N. Task-related effects in the prosody of Spanish heritage speakers and long-term immigrants. In Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches Across Linguistic Subfields; Armstrong, M.E., Henriksen, N., Vanrell, M.M., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 3–24. ISBN 9789027267450. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, T.K. Spanish-English Bilinguals’ Attitudes toward Codeswitching: Proficiency, Grammaticality and Familiarity. Ph.D. Thesis, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA, May 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Saville-Troike, M. The Ethnography of Communication: An Introduction, 3rd ed.; Blackwell: Malden, MA, USA, 2003; ISBN 0631228411. [Google Scholar]
- Torres Cacoullos, R.; Ferreira, F. Lexical frequency and voiced labiodental bilabial variation in New Mexican Spanish. Southwest J. Linguist. 2000, 19, 1–17. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, E.; Harper, D. Phonological evidence of crosslinguistic exemplar connections. Stud. Hisp. Lusoph. Linguist. 2009, 2, 257–274. [Google Scholar]
- Amengual, M. Interlingual influence in bilingual speech: Cognate status effect in a continuum of bilingualism. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 517–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldrick, M.; Runnqvist, E.; Costa, A. Language switching makes pronunciation less nativelike. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 1031–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lennes, M.; McCloy, D. Collect_formant_data_from_files.praat. Available online: https://depts.washington.edu/phonlab/resources/getDurationPitchFormants.praat (accessed on 15 December 2016).
- Lobanov, B.M. Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different speakers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1971, 49, 606–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adank, P.; Smits, R.; Van Hout, R. A comparison of vowel normalization procedures for language variation research. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 116, 3099–3107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poplack, S.; Meechan, M. How languages fit together in code-mixing. Int. J. Biling. 1998, 2, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lisker, L.; Abramson, A.S. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word 1964, 20, 384–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fletcher, J. The prosody of speech: Timing and rhythm. In Essential Clinical Skills: The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences, 2nd ed.; Hardcastle, W.J., Laver, J., Gibbon, F.E., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; Volume 4, pp. 523–602. [Google Scholar]
- Rao, R. On the status of the phoneme /b/ in heritage speakers of Spanish. Sintagma 2014, 26, 37–54. [Google Scholar]
- Rao, R. Manifestations of /bdg/ in heritage speakers of Spanish. Heritage Lang. J. 2015, 12, 48–74. [Google Scholar]
Participant Number | Score | Level of Proficiency |
---|---|---|
1 | 46/50 | Advanced |
2 | 37/50 | Intermediate |
3 | 35/50 | Intermediate |
4 | 34/50 | Intermediate |
5 | 48/50 | Advanced |
6 | 43/50 | Advanced |
7 | 43/50 | Advanced |
8 | 25/50 | Intermediate |
9 | 44/50 | Advanced |
10 | 45/50 | Advanced |
11 | 41/50 | Advanced |
Interaction | Factors | n | Mean (SD) | 95% CI | Effect Size (d) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vowel and lexical stress | Stressed /a/ | 389 | 0.93 (0.92) | [0.84, 1.02] | 0.49 |
Unstressed /a/ | 560 | 0.60 (0.87) | [0.53, 0.67] | ||
Stressed /e/ | 315 | 0.22 (0.72) | [0.12, 0.32] | 0.44 | |
Unstressed /e/ | 480 | −0.09 (0.72) | [−0.16, −0.01] | ||
Stressed /i/ | 307 | −0.62 (0.85) | [−0.72, −0.51] | Pairwise difference not significant (ns) | |
Unstressed /i/ | 297 | −0.61 (0.92) | [−0.71, −0.50] | ||
Stressed /o/ | 346 | 0.18 (0.65) | [0.08, 0.27] | 0.34 | |
Unstressed /o/ | 364 | −0.07 (0.74) | [−0.17, 0.02] | ||
Stressed /u/ | 273 | −0.55 (0.76) | [−0.66, −0.44] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Unstressed /u/ | 258 | −0.52 (1.22) | [−0.63, −0.42] | ||
Vowel and language session | Code-switching /a/ | 489 | 0.45 (0.92) | [0.37, 0.54] | 0.77 |
Monolingual /a/ | 460 | 1.08 (0.78) | [1.00, 1.16] | ||
Code-switching /e/ | 364 | 0.04 (0.82) | [−0.06, 0.13] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /e/ | 431 | 0.09 (0.65) | [0.02, 0.18] | ||
Code-switching /i/ | 194 | −0.40 (0.93) | [−0.52, −0.28] | 0.47 | |
Monolingual /i/ | 410 | −0.82 (0.84) | [−0.91, −0.74] | ||
Code-switching /o/ | 230 | 0.09 (0.80) | [−0.02, 0.21] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /o/ | 480 | 0.01 (0.65) | [−0.07, 0.09] | ||
Code-switching /u/ | 218 | −0.26 (1.36) | [−0.38, −0.15] | 0.54 | |
Monolingual /u/ | 313 | −0.81 (0.57) | [−0.90, −0.71] |
Parameter | Estimate | SE | df | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | −0.88 | 0.11 | 2099.12 | −8.19 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /a/ | 2.10 | 0.09 | 590.99 | 23.97 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /e/ | 1.108 | 0.90 | 503.87 | 11.96 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /i/ | 0.03 | 0.09 | 493.32 | 0.33 | 0.74 |
Vowel = /o/ | 0.93 | 0.09 | 516.67 | 10.52 | <0.001 |
Lexical stress = no | 0.03 | 0.10 | 738.25 | 0.30 | 0.76 |
Language session = code-switching | 0.54 | 0.11 | 1096.64 | 5.15 | <0.001 |
DELE scores | 0.00 | 0.00 | 3469.39 | 0.76 | 0.45 |
Unstressed vowel = /a/ | −0.44 | 0.13 | 764.00 | −3.48 | 0.001 |
Unstressed vowel = /e/ | −0.34 | 0.13 | 718.3 | −2.71 | 0.007 |
Unstressed vowel = /i/ | −0.09 | 0.13 | 658.93 | −0.70 | 0.48 |
Unstressed vowel = /o/ | −0.23 | 0.13 | 670.20 | −1.85 | 0.064 |
Code-switching vowel = /a/ | −1.25 | 0.14 | 1056.46 | −9.07 | <0.001 |
Code-switching vowel = /e/ | −0.61 | 0.15 | 935.65 | −4.22 | <0.001 |
Code-switching vowel = /i/ | −0.19 | 0.15 | 918.87 | −1.28 | 0.20 |
Code-switching vowel = /o/ | −0.41 | 0.14 | 894.13 | −2.89 | 0.004 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /a/ | 0.17 | 0.11 | 1670.30 | 1.48 | 0.14 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /e/ | 0.02 | 0.13 | 938.54 | 0.18 | 0.86 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /i/ | 0.14 | 0.15 | 1082.11 | 0.94 | 0.35 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /o/ | −0.09 | 0.14 | 911.75 | −0.66 | 0.51 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /u/ | 0.00 | 0.15 | 1066.28 | 0.02 | 0.98 |
Interaction | Factors | n | Mean (SD) | 95% CI | Effect Size (d) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vowel and lexical stress | Stressed /a/ | 389 | −0.10 (0.55) | [−0.10, 0.7] | Pairwise difference (ns) |
Unstressed /a/ | 560 | −0.01 (0.68) | [−0.18, −0.02] | ||
Stressed /e/ | 315 | 0.33 (0.67) | [−0.00, 0.19] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Unstressed /e/ | 480 | 0.26 (0.76) | [0.42, 0.58] | ||
Stressed /i/ | 307 | 0.93 (1.08) | [0.83, 1.04] | 0.20 | |
Unstressed /i/ | 297 | 0.75 (1.06) | [0.64, 0.85] | ||
Stressed /o/ | 346 | −0.70 (0.74) | [−0.80, −0.60] | 0.14 | |
Unstressed /o/ | 364 | −0.58 (0.79) | [−0.68, −0.48] | ||
Stressed /u/ | 273 | −0.60 (0.95) | [−0.71, −0.49] | 0.37 | |
Unstressed /u/ | 258 | −0.30 (0.93) | [−0.40, −0.19] | ||
Vowel and language session | Code-switching /a/ | 489 | −0.02 (0.73) | [−0.10, 0.07] | Pairwise difference (ns) |
Monolingual /a/ | 460 | −0.10 (0.51) | [−0.18, −0.12] | ||
Code-switching /e/ | 364 | 0.10 (0.80) | [0.00, 0.19] | 0.58 | |
Monolingual /e/ | 431 | 0.50 (0.60) | [0.42, 0.58] | ||
Code-switching /i/ | 194 | 0.54 (1.06) | [0.41, 0.66] | 0.60 | |
Monolingual /i/ | 410 | 1.15 (1.02) | [1.06, 1.23] | ||
Code-switching /o/ | 230 | −0.43 (0.87) | [−0.54, −0.32] | 0.55 | |
Monolingual /o/ | 480 | −0.85 (0.67) | [−0.93, −0.77] | ||
Code-switching /u/ | 218 | −0.26 (1.03) | [−0.37, −0.14] | 0.42 | |
Monolingual /u/ | 313 | −0.64 (0.86) | [−0.74, −0.54] |
Parameter | Estimate | SE | df | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | −0.89 | 0.12 | 2127.50 | −8.27 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /a/ | 0.67 | 0.09 | 809.68 | 7.51 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /e/ | 1.40 | 0.09 | 529.22 | 15.06 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /i/ | 2.09 | 0.09 | 643.01 | 22.47 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /o/ | −0.07 | 0.09 | 650.37 | −0.76 | 0.45 |
Lexical stress = no | 0.39 | 0.10 | 812.09 | 3.97 | <0.001 |
Language session = code-switching | 0.47 | 0.11 | 1220.00 | 4.41 | <0.001 |
DELE scores | 0.00 | 0.00 | 3439.15 | 0.625 | 0.532 |
Unstressed vowel = /a/ | −0.27 | 0.13 | 938.85 | −2.08 | 0.04 |
Unstressed vowel = /e/ | −0.52 | 0.13 | 767.08 | −4.00 | <0.001 |
Unstressed vowel = /i/ | −0.62 | 0.13 | 808.61 | −4.71 | <0.001 |
Unstressed vowel = /o/ | −0.28 | 0.13 | 837.56 | −2.23 | 0.03 |
Code-switching vowel = /a/ | −0.35 | 0.14 | 1316.06 | −2.52 | 0.01 |
Code-switching vowel = /e/ | −0.93 | 0.15 | 1050.23 | −6.33 | <0.001 |
Code-switching vowel = /i/ | −1.12 | 0.14 | 1120.83 | −7.55 | <0.001 |
Code-switching vowel = /o/ | −0.06 | 0.14 | 1073.382 | −0.40 | 0.69 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /a/ | −0.07 | 0.11 | 2206.76 | −0.652 | 0.51 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /e/ | 0.11 | 0.13 | 1042.46 | 0.92 | 0.36 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /i/ | 0.08 | 0.15 | 1382.76 | 0.57 | 0.57 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /o/ | 0.02 | 0.14 | 1173.25 | 0.15 | 0.88 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /u/ | −0.17 | 0.15 | 1207.95 | −1.15 | 0.25 |
Interaction | Factors | n | Mean (SD) | 95% CI | Effect Size (d) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vowel and lexical stress | Stressed /a/ | 389 | 2.03 (0.12) | [2.00, 2.07] | 0.52 |
Unstressed /a/ | 560 | 1.96 (0.15) | [1.92, 1.99] | ||
Stressed /e/ | 315 | 1.94 (0.12) | [1.90, 1.97] | 0.29 | |
Unstressed /e/ | 480 | 1.90 (0.15) | [1.86, 1.93] | ||
Stressed /i/ | 307 | 1.92 (0.13) | [1.88, 1.95] | 0.34 | |
Unstressed /i/ | 297 | 1.87 (0.16) | [1.83, 1.90] | ||
Stressed /o/ | 346 | 2.01 (0.15) | [1.98, 2.05] | 0.25 | |
Unstressed /o/ | 364 | 1.97 (0.17) | [1.93, 2.00] | ||
Stressed /u/ | 273 | 1.93 (0.14) | [1.89, 1.97] | 0.67 | |
Unstressed /u/ | 258 | 1.83 (0.16) | [1.79, 1.86] | ||
Vowel and language session | Code-switching /a/ | 489 | 1.99 (0.14) | [1.95, 2.02] | Pairwise difference (ns) |
Monolingual /a/ | 460 | 2.00 (0.14) | [1.97, 2.04] | ||
Code-switching /e/ | 364 | 1.91 (0.14) | [1.87, 1.94] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /e/ | 431 | 1.93 (0.14) | [1.89, 1.96] | ||
Code-switching /i/ | 194 | 1.89 (0.12) | [1.85, 1.93] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /i/ | 410 | 1.89 (0.16) | [1.86, 1.93] | ||
Code-switching /o/ | 230 | 2.00 (0.16) | [1.96, 2.04] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /o/ | 480 | 1.98 (0.17) | [1.94, 2.01] | ||
Code-switching /u/ | 218 | 1.87 (0.16) | [1.83, 1.91] | Pairwise difference (ns) | |
Monolingual /u/ | 313 | 1.89 (0.16) | [1.85, 1.92] |
Parameter | Estimate | SE | df | t-Value | p-Value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 2.09 | 0.09 | 9.40 | 22.46 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /a/ | 0.11 | 0.02 | 1822.10 | 6.33 | <0.001 |
Vowel = /e/ | 0.02 | 0.02 | 890.51 | 1.10 | 0.27 |
Vowel = /i/ | 0.00 | 0.02 | 1414.51 | 0.21 | 0.83 |
Vowel = /o/ | 0.06 | 0.02 | 1465.00 | 3.44 | 0.001 |
Lexical stress = no | −0.09 | 0.02 | 1486.71 | −4.49 | <0.001 |
Language session = code-switching | 0.00 | 0.10 | 2445.62 | −0.14 | 0.89 |
DELE scores | 0.00 | 0.00 | 8.98 | −1.73 | 0.12 |
Unstressed vowel = /a/ | 0.00 | 0.02 | 1838.52 | 0.38 | 0.71 |
Unstressed vowel = /e/ | 0.04 | 0.03 | 1268.93 | 1.40 | 0.16 |
Unstressed vowel = /i/ | 0.00 | 0.03 | 1632.94 | 0.30 | 0.76 |
Unstressed vowel = /o/ | 0.06 | 0.02 | 1757.60 | 2.27 | 0.02 |
Code-switching vowel = /a/ | −0.02 | 0.03 | 2719.57 | −0.642 | 0.52 |
Code-switching vowel = /e/ | −0.03 | 0.03 | 2083.54 | −0.98 | 0.33 |
Code-switching vowel = /i/ | −0.04 | 0.03 | 2380.31 | −1.28 | 0.20 |
Code-switching vowel = /o/ | 0.04 | 0.03 | 2342.13 | 1.63 | 0.10 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /a/ | 0.01 | 0.02 | 3418.00 | 0.57 | 0.57 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /e/ | 0.02 | 0.02 | 1918.86 | 0.80 | 0.42 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /i/ | 0.06 | 0.03 | 2753.65 | 2.35 | 0.02 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /o/ | −0.03 | 0.03 | 2516.14 | −1.32 | 0.17 |
Code-switching, unstressed vowel = /u/ | −0.03 | 0.03 | 2398.68 | −1.00 | 0.32 |
1 | |
2 | Au et al. [27] defined childhood speakers as individuals who spoke Spanish regularly for at least three years before the age of six. After the age of six, they continued to speak Spanish, but with less
frequency than before. For childhood overhearers, Au et al. defined them as individuals who overheard spoken Spanish for several hours each week for a minimum of three years before the age of six [25]. However, they did not speak the language and received less Spanish input after the age of six. |
3 | Silva-Corvalán defines the first generation as
individuals immigrated to the U.S. after the age of 11 [37]. The second generation are immigrants who came to the U.S. before the age of six or who were born in the U.S. to at least one first-generation parent. Third generation immigrants were born in the U.S. and have at least one parent from the second
generation. |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Elias, V.; McKinnon, S.; Milla-Muñoz, Á. The Effects of Code-Switching and Lexical Stress on Vowel Quality and Duration of Heritage Speakers of Spanish. Languages 2017, 2, 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040029
Elias V, McKinnon S, Milla-Muñoz Á. The Effects of Code-Switching and Lexical Stress on Vowel Quality and Duration of Heritage Speakers of Spanish. Languages. 2017; 2(4):29. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040029
Chicago/Turabian StyleElias, Vanessa, Sean McKinnon, and Ángel Milla-Muñoz. 2017. "The Effects of Code-Switching and Lexical Stress on Vowel Quality and Duration of Heritage Speakers of Spanish" Languages 2, no. 4: 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040029
APA StyleElias, V., McKinnon, S., & Milla-Muñoz, Á. (2017). The Effects of Code-Switching and Lexical Stress on Vowel Quality and Duration of Heritage Speakers of Spanish. Languages, 2(4), 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2040029