Next Article in Journal
In Vitro Rooting of Capparis spinosa L. as Affected by Genotype and by the Proliferation Method Adopted During the Multiplication Phase
Next Article in Special Issue
Enhanced UV-Reflection Facilitated a Shift in the Pollination System of the Red Poppy, Papaver rhoeas (Papaveraceae)
Previous Article in Journal
Development and Characterization of an Ethyl Methane Sulfonate (EMS) Induced Mutant Population in Capsicum annuum L.
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Flower Colour Polymorphism, Pollination Modes, Breeding System and Gene Flow in Anemone coronaria

1
Department of Evolutionary Biology and Institute of Evolution, University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel
2
Department of Biology and Environment University of Haifa-Oranim, Tivon 36006, Israel
3
Agroecology & Pollination Group, Landscape Ecology & Plant Production Systems, Université Libre de Bruxelles (ULB), Boulevard du Triomphe CP 264/2, B-1050 Brussels, Belgium
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Plants 2020, 9(3), 397; https://doi.org/10.3390/plants9030397
Submission received: 6 February 2020 / Revised: 7 March 2020 / Accepted: 19 March 2020 / Published: 23 March 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Plant-Pollinator Interactions in Mediterranean-Type Ecosystems)

Abstract

:
The flower colour of Anemone coronaria (Ranunculaceae) is a genetically inherited trait. Such intra-specific flower colour polymorphism might be driven by pollinators, other non-pollinating agents, or by abiotic factors. We investigated the genetic relations among red, white and purple-blue flower colour morphs growing in 10 populations of A. coronaria in Israel, in relation to their breeding system, pollination modes, differential perception by bees and visitors’ behaviour. Flowers of these three morphs differed in their reflectance that could be perceived by bees. Honeybees, solitary bees and flies demonstrated only partial preferences for the different colour morphs. No spontaneous self-pollination was found; however, fruit set under nets, excluding insects but allowing wind pollination, was not significantly lower than that of natural free pollinated flowers, indicating a potential role of wind pollination. Anemone coronaria flowers were visited by various insects, honeybees and Andrena sp. preferred the white and purple-blue morphs, while the syrphid flies preferred the white flowers. Thus, visitor behaviour can only partially explain the evolution or maintenance of the colour polymorphism. No significant genetic differences were found among the populations or colour morphs. Wind pollination, causing random gene flow, may explain why no significant genetic divergence was found among all studied populations and their colour morphs. The existence of monomorphic red populations, along other polymorphic populations, might be explained by linked resistance to aridity and/or grazing.

1. Introduction

Flower colour polymorphism (hereafter FCP) refers to the variation in flower colours within or between natural populations of the same species. FCP may include cases of gradual [1], but mainly refers to discrete differences in flower colours among morphs [2,3,4,5,6]. FCP may occur sympatrically [7], parapatrically [8], or allopatrically [9].
Pollinators’ constancy, especially of bees, occurs when they return to feed on any preferred rewarding flower after quickly learning its visual and chemical properties. Such constancy to one flower morph and avoidance of others may cause divergent evolution [10]. Therefore, the coexistence of multiple colour morphs within any species requires special attention. FCP may be explained by pollinator mediated selection (e.g., [11,12]), antagonist selection by pollinators and herbivores (e.g., [11,12,13]), or adaptations to abiotic factors that are linked to inherited floral traits [14,15].
Pollinators may maintain FCP when colour preferences vary among pollinator guilds, morphs, individuals, foraging bouts, or when it varies with the frequencies of FCP [2,11,16,17,18,19,20,21]. A number of studies have shown that balancing and/or negative frequency-dependent selection may explain FCP [20,22,23]. Pollinators may also have fluctuating preferences depending on nectar properties, plant or inflorescence height, warmth, or flower abundance [11,16,21,22,24,25,26,27].
In some cases, the strength of the pollinators’ preferences may be insufficient to affect the maintenance of FCP [28,29].
When pollinators discriminate among FCPs, gene flow is expected to be more frequent within each colour morph than between morphs [7]. Consequently, genetic variation is expected to be lower within morphs than between them [30].
Flower traits act as signals not just for pollinators but also for herbivores that can discriminate among flower colour morphs [31,32,33], and their feeding preferences may vary among sites, over time, or across taxa [14,34,35]. Consequently, the same flower trait may make plants conspicuous to both pollinators and herbivores that may drive different selection pressures [34,36,37,38].
When flower colour morphs that are preferred by pollinators are also more susceptible to herbivores, there might be a conflicting selection pressure on flower colour [39]. Flower colour may have pleiotropic effects on plant defence [40].
Correlations between petal colour and herbivore defence chemicals in leaves or fruit have been observed [31,41] and may affect FCP either via genetic linkage or pleiotropy [39,42,43]. Such correlations may contribute to indirect selection by herbivores [31,32,33]. Herbivores that oviposit in flowers, causing pre-dispersal seed predation, are likely to use the same flower cues as pollinators to locate their host flowers [43]. Pollinators and predators may ultimately exert conflicting pressures on FCP because flowers that are most visited by pollinators are likely to produce more food for pre-dispersal seed predators [12,39], but see [13].
Selection along abiotic gradients may also maintain FCP, in part because anthocyanins are linked to increased tolerance to extreme drought, heat, pests, soil infertility, or to UV radiation [28,29,44,45]; but anthocyanin production also has costs [25,26,28,29,46].
Experiments showed selection on flower colour due to correlations between petal colour and heat tolerance [29,42,47]. Fluctuating selection associated with rainfall contributes to the maintenance of a blue-white flower colour polymorphism in Linanthus parraye [4,5], but anthocyanin production has also cost [25,26,28,29,46].
Anemone coronaria L. is an east Mediterranean species [48]. growing in diverse habitats around the Mediterranean Sea, and each plant produces 1–10 flowers, depending on age and local conditions. It exhibits an extreme case of FCP with red, white and purple-blue discrete colours, and with at least 30 gradual colour variants (Figure 1) [48,49]. Flower colour is determined by the anthocyanins and flavonoids content of the sepals [49].
At least four epistatic genes that act in the following sequence, Alb (albino), Sc (scarlet), W (white) and B (blue), were found to be responsible for flower colour [50,51,52]. Red A. coronaria populations were found to be pollinated mainly by Glaphyrid beetles [53] but also by honeybees [54]. Glaphyrid beetles find shelter and mate within large red flowers of A. coronaria; some floral toxic chemicals and the red background provide them some protection from bird predators [55,56].
Populations growing in semi-arid areas in southern Israel are monomorphic red. Many other populations have various mixed colours, while a few northern ones are purely white. Consequently, it has been hypothesised that the red morph is connected to aridity, while the white and purple-blue morphs are connected to rock and soil properties [50]. Red genotypes of A. coronaria were found to be resistant to heavy cattle grazing, and their population declined when grazing stopped [57], but not to roe-deer grazing [58]. Our aims were: (1) To explore the role of FCP in the pollination ecology of A. coronaria. (2) To examine the degree of genetic differences among populations and among flower morphs within populations. (3) To propose possible mechanisms for the maintenance of FCP in A. coronaria in Israel.
To do so, we asked the following questions: (1) Are different colour morphs (as seen by humans) also perceived differently by bees? (2) Are different colour morphs favoured by different flower visitors? (3) What are the possible roles of spontaneous self and wind pollination? (4) Does the genetic structure indicate the possibility of reproductive isolation among populations or between colour morphs within populations?

2. Results

2.1. Flower Reflectance and Perception

The different flower colour morphs (red, white and purple-blue) of A. coronaria L. (Megiddo site) differ markedly in their relative reflectance (Figure 2), and in their locations in the bee colour perception hexagon (Figure 3). The average (± SD) location of the red flowers (N = 20) was X = −0.08850 ± 0.08869 and Y = −0.03450 ± 0.01050, that of the white flowers (N = 20) was X = 0.08450 ± 0.03069 and Y = 0.14350 ± 0.02207, and that of the purple-blue flowers (N = 20) was X = −0.1730 ± 0.04438 and Y = 0.1675 ± 0.03024. The pairwise average Euclidean distances between the average locations of the different colour morphs were: purple-blue to red: 0.261 ± 0.050, purple-blue to white: 0.287 ± 0.050, red to white: 0.269 ± 0.079.

2.2. Number of Carpels

The average (± SD) number of ovules in red (N = 5), purple-blue (N = 5) and white (N = 5) flower morphs, from the Megiddo population, was 584.6 ± 75.7, 582.1 ± 69.1 and 573.8 ± 50.3, respectively, with no significant differences among the colour morphs (F2,14 = 0.007, P = 0.993).

2.3. Pollination Modes

2.3.1. Spontaneous Self-Pollination

Under experimental exclusion of both pollinator activity and of wind pollination, in the Beit Lehem Haglilit site, there was no fruit set in red, white and purple-blue flower morphs (N = 20 flowers for each colour morph), indicating that there was no spontaneous self-pollination.

2.3.2. Insect and Wind Pollination

In the 2012 season, the average (± SD) percentage of fruit set in the pollinator exclusion cages was 78% ± 10% (N = 11). In the 2014 season, the percentage of fruit set under natural free pollination (insects and wind) and of only wind pollinated flowers (under nets) in all colour morphs, ranged from 95.8% to 100% (N = 18, six from each colour morph) and from 90.3% to 96.1% (N = 18, six from each colour morph). There were no significant differences in fruit set among the colour morphs under free pollination (Kruskal–Wallis, χ = 1.11, df = 2, P = 0.775), and no significant differences among the colour morphs under pollinator exclusion nets (Kruskal–Wallis, χ = 1.72, df = 2, P = 0.632). However, there was a small but significant difference in the average percentage (± SD) of fruit set between open (98.0 ± 1.2%) and net covered (93.0 ± 1.7%) plants (all colours pooled (Mann–Whitney U = 160, Z = 2,519, P = 0.012, N = 24).
During three periods of 24 hours, 30 airborne pollen traps captured on average (± SD) 36.3 ± 0.6 (N = 80) A. coronaria pollen grains. Considering trap area and duration, the average pollen deposition rate was 0.05 grains per hour per mm2. The average (± SD) stigma surface (measured under microscope) was 0.491 ± 0.0092 mm2 (N = 100), and the average duration of stigmatic receptivity of a flower is 3.5 days (= 84 h) [54]. Thus, during a flower’s lifetime, 2.1 pollen grains can be deposited by wind on each stigma that contains a single ovule.
We interpreted any fruit production under pollinator exclusion as a result of wind pollination due to: (1) the absence of autogamy in A. coronaria (see the results), (2) the spatial separation of individual flowers under the cages (about 5 cm between adjacent flowers), and (3) flower morphology, which decrease the probability of pollination due to contact between neighbouring flowers is expected to be low.

2.4. Flower Visitors

During 2011 and 2012, the flower visitors were mostly honeybees, solitary bees and flies, while almost no beetles were observed. The total number of visitors that landed on flowers of all colour morphs was extremely low (175), and the average (± SD) visitation rate was only 0.072 ± 0.078 visits per flower per hour. Due to the scarcity of visitors, this dataset was insufficient for analysing whether or not there was a preference of any visitor type to any flower colour morph.
Four independent observation sessions were made on 6.02.2014 (two sessions), 9.02.2014 and 21.02.2014, in which the sequence of the visits of 13, 120, 30 and 43 individual bees to the various colour morphs were recorded, respectively (Table 1). Honeybees, solitary bees (mostly Andrena sp.), and syrphid flies mostly visited the white and purple-blue flower morphs, usually avoiding the red ones. During that season, only one beetle was observed visiting a red flower (Table 1). A χ2 test revealed that honeybees and Andrena sp. did not visit the colour morphs randomly, according to their frequencies, by avoiding visiting the red flowers (Table 1).

2.5. Genetic Structure of Populations and Colour Morphs

The amplified fragment length polymorphism (AFLP) analyses revealed 462 polymorphic loci. The levels of polymorphism (P) and gene diversity (He-unbiased expected heterozygosity) for each population and colour morph are summarized in Table 2. The mean (± SE) polymorphism level was 70.67% ± 5.25%, and gene diversity level was 0.124 ± 0.002 (Table 2). Nei unbiased genetic identity (I) between the tested populations (1. Mt. Meron, 2. Zippori, 3. Mt. Carmel (University), 4. Mt. Carmel (Nof Carmel), 5. Alonim, 6. Beit Shearim, 7. Balfouria, 8. Megiddo, 9. Purra, 10. Lahav) was very high (I = 0.99), indicating no differentiation between either populations or colour morphs (Exact test, P > 0.99).
Analysis of molecular variance (AMOVA) [60] revealed that the major part of the molecular variance (99%) was found within populations (Φ9,226 = 0.014, P = 0.001). Likewise, the Bayesian assignment test [61,62], indicated no differentiation between populations or colour morphs. The highest delta-K value (where K is a putative number of populations) of 33.8 was found for K = 2, and further for K = 4 (32.6), where the structure bar-plot (representing all tested individuals) appeared to be similar through the sampling range (Figure 4).

3. Discussion

3.1. Colour Discrimination by Visiting Insects

Colour morphs of the Anemone coronaria flower (Figure 1) clearly differ in their relative reflectance curves (Figure 2) and to the human eye. However, because different insects differ in the sensitivity of their photoreceptors to various spectral wavelengths, colour vision perceptual models must be used to examine whether or not an insect can distinguish between any given colours [59]. The threshold values of Euclidean distance between any two colours in the hexagon units above which bees can perceive them as different signals ranges between 0.062 [63,64] and 0.100 [65]. The distances between the three A. coronaria colour morphs ranged between 0.26 and 0.27. Therefore, it can be assumed that honeybees, and probably also solitary bees, can differentiate among them. The centre of the hexagon represents the background colour, thus the shorter distance of the red flowers from this point indicates that bees can differentiate them from the background less than white or purple blue flowers.
Direct evidence for colour vision, the ability to discriminate colours according to spectral shape but independent of intensity, has been demonstrated for only a few fly species [66]. Therefore, we do not really know to what extent hoverflies (Syrphidae), which were observed visiting A. coronaria flowers, can discriminate between its colour morphs.
Although almost not recorded in our study, the interaction between the beetles of the Glaphyridae family and the red bowl-shaped flowers in the Mediterranean region is well-documented [53]. These beetles, in contrast to bees, have photoreceptors that are sensitive to the UV, green and red ranges of the spectrum [67].
Thus, it seems that the combination of different colour reflection curves among A. coronaria colour morphs, and the different sensitivity of the photoreceptors of the major flower visitors (bees, flies and Glaphyrid beetles) could have been the basis for differential visitation, and pollination of any given flower colour morph by any visitor or pollinator type.

3.2. Spontaneous Self-Pollination and Wind Pollination

Earlier field experiments demonstrated that A. coronaria is strongly self-incompatible, and hybridization experiments showed that the different colour morphs could be readily crossed as they produced achenes with viable seeds [50].
Our results show the absence of spontaneous self-pollination in all flower colour morphs of A. coronaria. In contrast, a very high rate of fruit production (98%) under natural free pollination and only slightly less (93%) under insect exclusion with a net cover that allowed wind pollination. These surprising results indicate the possibility that wind pollination might cause free gene flow among populations and between colour morphs within them, independent of possible pollinators’ preferences to any flower colour morph. Since A. coronaria is a widespread species with almost continuous dispersal, wind pollination may contribute to free gene exchange and thus mask the role of the various putative agents, which may maintain colour polymorphism.
Ambophily is a pollination syndrome by both wind and insects and is generally related to harsh environmental conditions with low activity of pollinators [68,69,70,71]. Ambophily has evolved independently at least three times in the Ranunculaceae, in Aconitum gymnandrum [69], in Ranunculus weyleri [72] and in Thalictrum [73]. The Mediterranean winter is mild, and thus it is hard to explain the presence of ambophily in A. coronaria solely on environmental selective pressure via the scarcity of pollinators. Therefore, we propose it is a kind of ’pollination assurance’ mechanism, under unfavourable rainy conditions [74]. Anemone coronaria flowers close at night and on rainy days, thus its pollen is protected and wind pollination can occur also during sunny days, which are not uncommon in the mild Mediterranean winter.

3.3. Flowers Visitors

The red morph of A. coronaria was studied previously, and Glaphyrid beetles were found to be its major pollinators [53]. In contrast to bees, these beetles have red receptors [67]. However, early in the season, when other flower resources are scarce, red A. coronaria flowers are also visited by honeybees [54]. In the present study, honeybees were the most frequent visitors to A. coronaria flowers visitors, followed by solitary bees (mostly Andrena sp.) and syrphid flies, and only one visit of a Glaphyrid beetle was observed (Table 1).
Honeybees and Andrena sp. did not visit the colour morphs randomly, according to their frequencies, by avoiding visiting the red flowers (Table 1). The differences in the composition and frequency of flower visitors to A. coronaria flowers, in our study and among other studies [54,55,56], may reflect the high inter annual variability in temperatures and precipitation during the Mediterranean winter.

3.4. Population Genetic Analysis

Molecular analyses of ten A. coronaria populations consisting of red, white, and purple-blue colour morphs located along a rainfall gradient in Israel revealed high levels of polymorphism and genetic diversity (Table 2), and high genetic identity (similarity) among populations. The major part of the molecular variance was found to be within populations with no differentiation among populations or between colour morphs. In conclusion, the genetic analyses substantiate a free gene flow among all populations and colour morphs. These results raise questions concerning the evolutionary drivers and the ecological mechanisms that created and maintain this extreme case of flower colour polymorphism.

3.5. The Evolution and Maintenance of Flower Colour Polymorphism

The selective mechanisms that might create and/or maintain within-population variation in flower colours are diverse. For example, temporal or spatial variation in the strength and direction of natural selection on flower colour (i.e., fluctuating selection) can maintain genetic variation [75,76,77].
Flower colour is often linked with other plant traits that may directly affect plant fitness. Therefore, variation in flower colour could be maintained through selection on the linked traits rather than by direct selection on the colour itself [39,40,42,78,79]. Indirect responses to selection on correlated characteristics through non-pollinator agents may also influence the evolution of flower traits [43].
Pollinator-mediated selection is frequently proposed to explain polymorphism, with different colour morphs likely reflecting selection driven by different pollinators with different colour preferences [7,11,21,30,80]. If the pollinators are able to discriminate between colour morphs (e.g., Iris lutescens [2]), they could potentially exert selection driving flower colour polymorphism, depending on their behavioural responses as well as preferences to such colour differences (innate preference for particular colour morph, or learning process), as was shown for bees [12,81,82,83] and for birds [8].
Our results present clear evidence for specific preferences of flower colour morph by flower visitors. Honeybees and Andrena bees avoided visiting red flowers (Table 1), in contrast to earlier findings [54]; but they visited the white and purple-blue flowers, as expected from their frequencies. The differences in honeybees’ visitations may reflect differences in the environmental conditions as well as in the co-flowering species in the different studies. Thus, bees can only partially contribute to the isolation of the red colour morph by specific visitation and pollination. Syrphid flies visited the different flower colour morphs as expected by their frequencies and thus also cannot contribute to the isolation of any colour morph (Table 1). Glaphyrid beetles, which were almost absent from our study, were found in the past to be its major pollinators, visiting mainly red A. coronaria flowers [53]. Even in the absence of preference at the pollinator species level, the constancy of bees to any flower morph during a single bout may contribute to higher gene flow within than between morphs. Moreover, as pollinator preferences are related to flower frequency, their preferences may vary among locations, years and seasons.
A. coronaria starts flowering early in January, when pollinator activity is still very low [74]. During this time, the red flowering morph is abundant and visited by Glaphyrid beetles [53]. However, at the beginning of the flowering season, red flowers are visited also by honeybees as a result of their abundance and the scarcity of other pollen sources [54]. If the beetles were present only in the south of Israel, this could have explained the distribution of pure red populations there, but they are common all over the country, where mixed coloured populations occur (A. Dafni Pers. Obs.).
The geographic distribution of flower colour morphs of A. coronaria in Israel indicates that the southernmost populations consist of pure red genotypes. This distribution pattern has raised the hypothesis for pleiotropic relations between the red flower colour genotype and the resistance to aridity [48,84]. Indeed, the red flowering populations in the arid south also have narrower leaves in comparison to the same morph in the north (Y. Tankus, pers comm.). It is logical to assume that the smaller leaves confer resistance to aridity more than anthocyanin’s quantity [28,29,44,45]. Since the red morph is also common in north Israel, where it has larger leaves, it is inferred that leaf size exhibits a phenotypic plasticity and the red colour and leaf size seem not to be genetically linked. Moreover, to the best of our knowledge, beyond the above-mentioned correlation, there is no genetic evidence that supports this hypothesis.
The resistance of the red genotypes of A. coronaria to heavy cattle grazing [57] might be an advantage over other flower colours. However, since most open sunny areas, which are the typical habitats of A. coronaria, are under medium to heavy grazing pressure, this cannot explain why some populations are pure red and some have all colour morphs. Roe deer graze on A. coronaria flowers [58], but at present, it is not part of the native fauna of Israel.
The distribution of A. coronaria colour morph in Israel may also be related to soil type. Pure white and purple-blue colour morphs grow mainly on chalk-free soils on limestone or basalt rocks, respectively, and the red morph is dominant on relatively dry and calcareous soils [48,52,84]. However, this explanation seems non-satisfactory, since monomorphic red populations are also found growing close to other polymorphic populations in similar habitats, and more detailed research is needed to clarify this aspect.
We do not have any explanations why the monomorphic populations are almost exclusively red and why other populations are polymorphic. This pattern fits the generalization of Narbona et al. [6] who reviewed flower colour polymorphism in the Mediterranean (but omitted the case of A. coronaria) and concluded that, “The fact that no case with only polymorphic population was found suggests that flower colour in species with flower polymorphism is subjected to different evolutionary processes throughout their distribution”.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Study Sites

Flower visitor activity was observed in 2011, 2012 and 2014 in six mixed colour morph populations in northern Israel (Table 3, Figure 5). Colour reflection of different colour morph flowers was conducted in Megiddo site. We focused on the three major flower colour morphs (red, white and purple-blue). Plant material for DNA extraction was collected from 10 sites located throughout the main distribution areas of A. coronaria in Israel (Table 3). Each study site was about 1000 m2.

4.2. Flower Reflectance and Its Perception

To measure the relative (%) reflectance of A. coronaria we randomly selected 15 flowers of A. coronaria, each flower from a different individual growing in Megiddo site. We used a portable spectrophotometer (AVASPEC-2048-USB2-UA; Avantes, Eerbeek, The Netherlands) equipped with a Xenon light source (AVALIGHT-XE; Avantes) to measure the relative (%) reflectance (300–700 nm in 5 nm steps). The spectrophotometer was calibrated with a white standard (WS-2; Avantes). We measured the relative (%) reflectance of the centre of fresh petals, and we used the spectral sensitivity functions of the honeybee (Apis mellifera), because they are considered representative of a wide taxonomic spectrum of hymenopterans, as found in our study; furthermore, they are considered consistent within the Apoidea (bees sensu lato) [85,86,87].
We plotted the mean relative (%) reflectance of the petals across the 300–700 nm range. We then converted the raw data of relative reflectance measurements of petals into individual loci in the bee colour hexagon [59] by using the honeybee receptor-sensitivity curves and a green vegetation background measured in situ (Figure 3). We assessed and quantified the colour and achromatic contrasts between the petals of different colour morphs by calculating (a) pairwise Euclidean distances between loci and (b) the mean Euclidean distance between the species centroids in the bee colour hexagon. The Euclidean distance between any two loci indicates the perceived colour difference or contrast between the stimuli, and threshold values of hexagon units for colour discrimination usually range between 0.062 [63,64] and 0.100 [65] for bees.

4.3. Number of Carpels

The number of carpels in red (N = 5), purple-blue (N = 5) and white (N = 5) flower morphs, from the Megiddo population, were counted. Differences in carpel number among flower colour morphs were tested using ANOVA.

4.4. Pollination Modes

4.4.1. Spontaneous Self-Pollination

To examine the possibility of spontaneous self-pollination (mechanical/autogamy), 60 random individuals, 20 from each colour morph (red, purple-blue and white) of flowers with flowering buds, growing in Beit Lehem Haglilit, were covered with fine nets (0.5 × 0.5 mm), which prevented the entry of insects and the movement of pollen by wind in our study. Flowers were covered on 25.1.10 and on 21.1.11 and were opened after 45 days to count fruit set.

4.4.2. Wind Pollination

To examine the possibility of wind pollination, we constructed 30 pollen traps from plastic straps with 28.27 mm2 holes and sticky tape underneath them. The airborne pollen traps were mounted among the flowers for 24 h periods (16.01.11, 26.02.11 and 6.03.11) in the Megiddo population. Pollen grains of A. coronaria were identified by their size and shape, discriminated from others, counted, and deposition rate per unit area was calculated.
To estimate the rate of wind pollination, insects were excluded using cages (40 × 40 ×75 cm) covered with standard mosquito net (1 × 1 mm), which prevented insect entrance but allowed air currents. (We found no thrips, which may transfer pollen, at this study site). Eleven cages were set up in Megiddo during the 2012 season, each with an average (± SD) of 3.2 ± 1.95 flowers per cage (all colours combined). Thus, we repeated this experiment in Beit Lehem Haglilit in the 2014 season, with twelve randomly selected plants of each colour morph. Six plants of each colour morph were covered just before the opening of the first flower and their colour was marked. Six other plants were left uncovered as a control for free pollination. Plants were caged on 29.1.14, and the number of flowers that have set fruits (number of achenes was not counted) was recorded on 2.3.14.
The experiments were conducted in one plot of 100 × 200 m, presumably exposed to the similar wind regime.

4.5. Flower Visitors

Initial observations on visitors’ identity (their relative visit frequencies to each colour morph) were conducted in February and March 2011 and 2012, with additional observations in 2014. In total, we performed 18 observation sessions on average (± SD) 124 ± 45 min in six populations (Table 1) over a total of 37 h. In each population, we performed three observations, each on a separated date.
To check any possible colour preference by flower visitors we followed individual visitors. We recorded the colour morph of each flower visited from the first time the insect landed on the flower until it left the observation area (about 1000 m2). For each observation session, we also counted the number of each colour morph along five random 100 m long transects in the same area. These observations were conducted only on bright windless days, when the temperatures were above 20 °C. We used Pearson’s χ2 to determine whether there is a significant difference between the expected visit frequencies (frequencies of flower morphs) and the observed visit frequencies, as presented in Table 1.

4.6. Genetic Structure of Populations and Colour Morphs

Gene diversity and genetic identity among ten populations and between their colour morphs were tested using amplified fragment length polymorphism (AFLP) technique. The AFLP method was carried out essentially as described by Vos et al. [88]. High-quality genomic DNA (~200 ng) was digested with a pair of restriction enzymes (EcoRI/MseI) at 37 °C for 4 h, then ligated to double stranded EcoRI (E -) and MseI (M -) adaptors. The resulting fragments were amplified with non-selective primers, where the ligated adaptors served as target sites for primer annealing. Four selective primer combinations were used for AFLP amplification, E-TG/M-CAC, E-AG/M-CTT, E-TC/M-ACC and E-AC/M-CTG (E and M representing the restriction site and its ligated adaptor sequence). The selective EcoRI (E) primers were labelled with florescent dye (6-Fam, Ned, Vic and Pet, respectively). PCR reactions were carried out in a total volume of 13 L. PCR amplification cycles started at an annealing temperature of 65 °C, after which the annealing temperature was lowered by 0.7 °C per cycle for 12 cycles (a touch-down phase of 13 cycles), followed by 23 cycles at an annealing temperature of 56 °C. Amplification products were visualized under a Florescence-Reader (Applied Biosystems). Fragment analyses and genotyping were determined manually from the chromatographs using Peak-Scanner software (Applied Biosystems). DNA of several samples (~10%) were amplified and run in duplicate to validate the integrity of amplifications. The similarities between duplicated fingerprints were higher than 98%.
Amplification products were scored as discrete character states (present/absent) and transformed into band frequencies. Diversity values were based on phenotype frequency (phenotypes defined by the band patterns produced by individual primer pairs). Data were analysed by Tools for Population Genetic Analyses (TFPGA) software version 1.3 [89] and by GenAlEx 6.4 [60]. These programs consider AFLP bands as diploid-dominant markers in which the estimated allele frequencies are based on the square root of the frequency of the null (recessive) genotype. Genetic identity was calculated according to Nei [90], implemented in TFPGA [89]. Population differentiation was tested by exact tests (TFPGA [89]) 1000 dememorization steps, 10 batches, 2000 permutations per batch [91]. Molecular analysis of variance (AMOVA) was conducted using GenAlEx [60].
Bayesian partitioning clustering was applied using the program STRUCTURE 2.3.3 [61,62]. The data set was partitioned into 23 probable groups considering each colour morph in every population as a different group. The STRUCTURE run was applied, with four replicates for each putative group, 100,000 burn-in iterations and 100,000 Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) reps. The inference of the probable number of clusters was extracted by the log likelihood for each putative number of populations (K), Ln P(D) = L(K), and by the delta K method [92], using the program Structure Harvester [93].
As there is only one species of Anemone growing in Israel, we could not find a suitable closely related outgroup for our analyses. However, using the exact same method, in parallel to these analyses, we analysed several other species (e.g., Pinus halepensis, Sternbergia clusiana) that produced completely different AFLP profiles. The different AFLP patterns of these species, and the presence of variation in the AFLP profiles of A. coronaria, in the current study, can be regarded as controls for the present procedure.

5. Conclusions

Our results show that the extreme differences in flower colours among the human-perceived red, white and purple-blue morphs of A. coronaria, can also be sensed by bees, and earlier data showed that large red flowers are attractive to beetles. We revealed partial preferences of bees and flies to colour morphs. In other words, pollinators’ behaviour can only partially explain the evolution or maintenance of flower colour polymorphism in A. coronaria. We found no spontaneous self-pollination but fruit set under only wind pollination was only a little but significantly less than under free pollination. We also found no evidence for genetic divergence among populations and flower morphs indicating the absence of any reproductive barrier between them. Thus, A. coronaria in Israel has one panmictic meta-population with no indication of divergence among flower colour morphs. Free gene flow, due to effective wind pollination, explains the absence of genetic divergence among populations and flower morphs.
To the best of our knowledge A. coronaria is the first and only described case of a wind pollinated panmictic species with extreme flower polymorphism, but the adaptive value of the various colour morphs is still unclear.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.N., R.B.-S., A.D. and N.J.V.; Methodology, G.N., R.B.-S., A.D. and N.J.V.; Data Curation, G.N., R.B.-S., A.D. and N.J.V. Formal Analysis, H.Z., G.N., R.B.-S, A.D. and N.J.V.; Investigation, H.T. and A.D.; Writing, A.D.; Writing review, G.N. and R.B.-S.; Genetic analysis, R.B.-S. and H.T.; Colour analysis, N.J.V.; Supervision, G.N., R.B.-S. and A.D.; Funding Acquisition, A.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially funded by Henk and Dorothy Schussheim Fund for Ecological Research in Mt Carmel.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Joseph, N.; Siril, E.A. Floral color polymorphism and reproductive success in annatto (Bixa orellana L.). Trop. Plant Biol. 2013, 6, 217–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Wang, H.; Conchou, L.; Bessière, J.M.; Cazals, G.; Schatz, B.; Imbert, E. Flower color polymorphism in Iris lutescens (Iridaceae), biochemical analyses in light of plant–insect interactions. Phytochemistry 2013, 94, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Wang, H.; Talavera, M.; Min, Y.; Flaven, E.; Imbert, E. Neutral processes contribute to patterns of spatial variation for flower colour in the Mediterranean Iris lutescens (Iridaceae). Ann. Bot. 2016, 117, 995–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Schemske, D.W.; Bierzychudek, P. Evolution of flower color in the desert annual Linanthus parryae, Wright revisited. Evolution 2001, 55, 1269–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Schemske, D.W.; Bierzychudek, P. Spatial differentiation for flower color in the desert annual Linanthus parryae, was right? Evolution 2007, 61, 2528–2543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Narbona, E.; Wang, H.; Ortiz, P.L.; Arista, M.; Imbert, E. Flower colour polymorphism in the Mediterranean Basin, occurrence, maintenance and implications for speciation. Plant Biol. 2017, 20, 8–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Malerba, R.; Nattero, J. Pollinator response to flower color polymorphism and floral display in a plant with a single-locus floral color polymorphism, consequences for plant reproduction. Ecol. Res. 2012, 27, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Streisfeld, M.A.; Kohn, J. Environment and pollinator-mediated selection on parapatric floral races of Mimulus aurantiacus. J. Evol. Biol. 2007, 20, 122–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hopkins, R.; Rausher, M.D. The cost of reinforcement, selection on flower color in allopatric populations of Phlox drummondii. Am. Nat. 2014, 183, 693–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Chittka, L.; Thomson, J.D.; Waser, N.M. Flower constancy, insect psychology, and plant evolution. Naturwissenschaften 1999, 86, 361–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Jones, K.N.; Reithel, J.S. Pollinator-mediated selection on a flower color polymorphism in experimental populations of Antirrhinum (Scrophulariaceae). Am. J. Bot. 2001, 88, 447–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Irwin, R.E.; Strauss, S.Y. Flower color microevolution in wild radish, evolutionary response to pollinator-mediated selection. Am. Nat. 2005, 165, 225–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Caruso, C.M.; Scott, S.L.; Wray, J.C.; Walsh, C.A. Pollinators, herbivores, and the maintenance of flower color variation, a case study with Lobelia siphilitica. Int. J. Plant Sci. 2010, 171, 1020–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Strauss, S.Y.; Irwin, R.E.; Lambrix, V.M. Optimal defence theory and flower petal colour predict variation in the secondary chemistry of wild radish. J. Ecol. 2004, 92, 132–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Arista, M.; Talavera, M.; Berjano, R.; Ortiz, P.L. Abiotic factors may explain the geographical distribution of flower colour morphs and the maintenance of colour polymorphism in the scarlet pimpernel. J. Ecol. 2013, 101, 1613–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Levin, D.A.; Watkins, L. Assortative mating in Phlox. Heredity 1984, 53, 595–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Cresswell, J.E.; Galen, C. Frequency-dependent selection and adaptive surfaces for floral character combinations, the pollination of Polemonium viscosum. Am. Nat. 1991, 138, 1342–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Smithson, A.; MacNair, M.R. Negative frequency-dependent selection by pollinators on artificial flowers without rewards. Evolution 1997, 51, 715–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gegear, R.J.; Laverty, T.M. The effects of variation among floral traits on the flower constancy of pollinators. In Cognitive Ecology of Pollination; Chittka, L., Thomson, J.D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2001; pp. 297–317. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gigord, L.D.B.; MacNair, M.R.; Smithson, A. Negative frequency-dependent selection maintains a dramatic flower color polymorphism in the rewardless orchid Dactylorhiza sambucina (L.) Soo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 6253–6255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Eckhart, V.M.; Rushing, N.S.; Hart, G.M.; Hansen, J.D. Frequency-dependent pollinator foraging in polymorphic Clarkia xantiana ssp. Xantiana populations, implications for flower colour evolution and pollinator interactions. Oikos 2006, 112, 412–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rausher, M.D.; Fry, J.D. Effects of a locus affecting floral pigmentation in Ipomoea purpurea on female fitness components. Genetics 1993, 134, 1237–1247. [Google Scholar]
  23. Subramaniam, B.; Rausher, M.D. Balancing selection on a floral polymorphism. Evolution 2000, 54, 691–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Levin, D.A.; Brack, E.T. Natural selection against white petals in Phlox. Evolution 1995, 49, 1017–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Carlson, J.E.; Holsinger, K.E. Natural selection on inflorescence color polymorphisms in wild Protea populations, the role of pollinators, seed predators and inter-trait correlations. Am. J. Bot. 2010, 97, 934–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Carlson, J.E.; Holsinger, K.E. Developmental plasticity in Protea as an evolutionary response to environmental clines in the Cape Floristic Region. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e52035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Bernhardt, P.; Edens-Meier, R.; Jocson, D.; Zweck, J.; Ren, Z.X.; Camilo, G.R.; Arduser, M. Comparative floral ecology of bicolor and concolor morphs of Viola pedata L. (Violaceae) following controlled burns. J. Pollinat. Ecol. 2016, 19, 57–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Warren, J.; Mackenzie, S. Why are all colour combinations not equally represented as flower-colour polymorphisms? New Phytol. 2001, 151, 237–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Steyn, W.J.; Wand, S.J.E.; Holcroft, D.M.; Jacobs, G. Anthocyanins in vegetative tissues, a proposed unified function in photoprotection. New Phytol. 2002, 155, 349–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Brown, B.A.; Clegg, M.T. The influence of flower color polymorphisms on genetic transmission in a natural population of the common morning glory, Ipomoea purpurea. Evolution 1984, 38, 796–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Frey, F.M. Opposing natural selection from herbivores and pathogens may maintain floral-color variation in Claytonia virginica (Portulacaceae). Evolution 2004, 58, 2426–2437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Raguso, R.A. Wake up and smell the roses, the ecology and evolution of floral scent. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2008, 39, 549–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Johnson, E.T.; Berhow, M.A.; Dowd, P.F. Colored and white sectors from star-patterned petunia flowers display differential resistance to corn earworm and cabbage looper larvae. J. Chem. Ecol. 2008, 34, 757–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Irwin, R.E.; Strauss, S.Y.; Storz, S.; Emerson, A.; Guibert, G. The role of herbivores in the maintenance of a flower color polymorphism in wild radish. Ecology 2003, 84, 1733–1743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Whitney, K.D.; Stanton, M.L. Insect seed predators as novel agents of selection on fruit color. Ecology 2004, 84, 2153–2160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Brody, A.K.; Mitchell, R.J. Effects of experimental manipulation of inflorescence size on pollination and pre-dispersal seed predation in the hummingbird-pollinated plant Ipomopsis aggregata. Oecologia 1997, 110, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Galen, C.; Cuba, J. Down the tube, pollinators, predators, and the evolution of flower shape in the alpine skypilot, Polemonium viscosum. Evolution 2001, 55, 1963–1971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Adler, L.S.; Bronstein, J.L. Attracting antagonists, does floral nectar increase leaf herbivory? Ecology 2004, 85, 1519–1526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Strauss, S.Y.; Whittall, J.B. Non-pollinating agents of selection on floral traits. In Ecology and Evolution of Flowers; Harder, L.D., Barrett, S.C.H., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006; pp. 120–138. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fineblum, W.L.; Rausher, M.D. Do floral pigmentation genes also influence resistance to enemies? The W locus in Ipomoea purpurea. Ecology 1997, 78, 1646–1654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Simms, E.L.; Bucher, M.A. Pleiotropic effects of flower-color intensity on herbivore performance on Ipomoea purpurea. Evolution 1996, 50, 957–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Armbruster, W.S. Can indirect selection and genetic context contribute to trait diversification? A transition-probability study of blossom-colour evolution in two genera. J. Evol. Biol. 2002, 15, 468–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Strauss, S.Y.; Irwin, R.E. Ecological and evolutionary consequences of multispecies plant–animal interactions. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2004, 35, 435–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bowman, R.N. Cryptic self-incompatibility and the breeding system of Clarkia unguiculata (Onagraceae). Am. J. Bot. 1987, 74, 471–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Grace, S.C.; Logan, B.A. Energy dissipation and radical scavenging by the plant phenylpropanoid pathway. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2000, 355, 1402–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Koes, R.E.; Quattrocchio, F.; Mol, J.N.M. The flavonoid biosynthetic pathway in plants, function and evolution. Bioessays 1994, 16, 123–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Coberly, L.C.; Rausher, M.D. Analysis of a chalcone synthase mutant in Ipomoea purpurea reveals a novel function for flavonoids, amelioration of heat stress. Mol. Ecol. 2003, 12, 1113–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Horovitz, A.; Galil, J.; Zohary, D. Biological flora of Israel, 6. Anemone coronaria L. Isr. J. Bot. 1975, 24, 26–41. [Google Scholar]
  49. Harvey, D.M. Phenotypic variation in flower colour within the Anemone coronaria cultivars. Ann. Bot. 1971, 35, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Horovitz, A. The local gene pool of Anemone coronria. Ornam. Plant Improv. Class. Mol. 1995, 420, 144–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Horovitz, A.; Zohary, D. Spontaneous variegation for perianth colour in wild Anemone coronaria. Heredity 1966, 21, 513–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Horovitz, A. The pollination syndrome of Anemone coronaria L.; an insect-biased mutualism. VI Int. Symp. Pollinat. 1990, 288, L283–L287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dafni, A.; Bernhardt, P.; Shmida, A.; Ivri, Y.; Greenbaum, S.; O’Toole, C.; Losito, L. Red bowl-shaped flowers, convergence for beetle pollination in the Mediterranean region. Isr. J. Bot. 1990, 39, 81–92. [Google Scholar]
  54. Keasar, T.; Shmida, A.; Zylbertal, A. Pollination Ecology of the Red Anemone coronaria (Ranunclaceae), Honeybees May Select for Early Flowering; Center for the Study of Rationality: Jerusalem, Israel, 2008; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Avi_Shmida/publication/23531070 (accessed on 6 February 2020).
  55. Keasar, T.; Harari, A.R.; Sabatinelli, G.; Keith, D.; Dafni, A.; Shavit, O.; Shmida, A. Red anemone guild flowers as focal places for mating and feeding by Levant glaphyrid beetles. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2010, 99, 808–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Keasar, T.; Kishinevsky, M.; Shmida, A.; Gerchman, Y.; Chinkov, N.; Koplovich, A.; Katzir, G. Plant-derived visual signals may protect beetle herbivores from bird predators. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013, 67, 1613–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Perevolotsky, A.; Schwartz-Tzachor, R.; Yonathan, R.; Ne’eman, G. Geophytes–herbivore interactions, reproduction and population dynamics of Anemone coronaria L. Plant Ecol. 2011, 212, 563–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wallach, A.D.; Inbar, M.; Shanas, U. Roe deer and decapitated Anemone flowers. Isr. J. Plant Sci. 2009, 57, 103–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Chittka, L. The colour hexagon, a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1992, 170, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Peakall, R.; Smouse, P.E. GENALEX 6, genetic analysis in excel. Population genetic software for teaching and research. Mol. Ecol. Notes 2006, 6, 288–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Pritchard, J.K.; Stephens, M.; Donnelly, P. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. Genetics 2000, 155, 945–959. [Google Scholar]
  62. Falush, D.; Stephens, M.; Pritchard, J.K. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data, dominant markers and null alleles. Mol. Ecol. Notes 2007, 7, 574–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Dyer, A.G.; Chittka, L. Biological significance of distinguishing between similar colours in spectrally variable illumination, bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) as a case study. J. Comp. Physiol. 2004, 190, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Dyer, A.G.; Chittka, L. Fine color discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften 2004, 91, 224–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Chittka, L.; Gumbert, A.; Kunze, J. Foraging dynamics of bumble bees, correlates of movements within and between plant species. Behav. Ecol. 1997, 8, 239–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lunau, K. Visual ecology of flies with particular reference to colour vision and colour preferences. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2014, 200, 497–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Martinez-Harms, J.; Vorobyev, M.; Schorn, J.; Shmida, A.; Keasar, T.; Homberg, U.; Schmeling, F.; Menzel, R. Evidence of red sensitive photoreceptors in Pygopleurus israelitus (Glaphyridae, Coleoptera) and its implications for beetle pollination in the southeast Mediterranean. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2012, 198, 451–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Regal, P.J. Pollination by wind and animals, ecology of geographic patterns. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1982, 13, 497–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Duan, Y.W.; Zhang, T.F.; He, Y.P.; Liu, J.Q. Insect and wind pollination of an alpine biennial Aconitum gymnandrum (Ranunculaceae). Plant Biol. 2009, 11, 796–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. De la Bandera, M.C.; Traveset, A. Breeding system and spatial variation in the pollination biology of the heterocarpic Thymelaea velutina (Thymelaeaceae). Plant Syst. Evol. 2006, 257, 9–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Dafni, A.; Marom-Levy, T.; Jürgens, A.; Dötterl, S.; Dorchin, A.; Shimrat, Y.; Kirkpatrick, E.B. Ambophily and “super generalism” in Ceratonia siliqua pollination. In Evolution of Plant–Pollinator Relationships; Patiny, S., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012; pp. 344–373. [Google Scholar]
  72. Cursach, J.; Rita, J. Reproductive biology of Ranunculus weyleri (Ranunculaceae), a narrowly endemic plant from the Balearic Islands with disjunct populations. Flora Morphol. Distrib. Funct. Ecol. Plants 2012, 207, 726–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Kaplan, S.M.; Mulcahy, D.L. Mode of pollination and floral sexuality in Thalictrum. Evolution 1971, 25, 659–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Dafni, A. Autumnal and winter pollination adaptations under Mediterranean conditions. Bocconea 1996, 5, 171–181. [Google Scholar]
  75. Hedrick, P.W. Genetic polymorphism in heterogeneous environments, a decade later. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1986, 17, 535–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Hedrick, P.W.; Ginevan, M.E.; Ewing, E.P. Genetic polymorphism in heterogeneous environments. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1976, 7, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Ellner, S.; Sasaki, A. Patterns of genetic polymorphism maintained by fluctuating selection with overlapping generations. Theor. Popul. Biol. 1996, 50, 31–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Rausher, M.D. Evolutionary transitions in floral color. Int. J. Plant Sci. 2008, 169, 7–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Dormont, L.; Delle-Vedove, R.; Bessière, J.M.; Key, H.M.; Schatz, B. Rare white flowered morphs increase the reproductive success of common purple morphs in a food deceptive orchid. New Phytol. 2010, 185, 300–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Suchet, C.; Dormont, L.; Schatz, B.; Giurfa, M.; Simon, V.; Raynaud, C.; Chave, J. Floral scent variation in two Antirrhinum majus subspecies influences the choice of naive bumblebees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011, 65, 1015–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Raine, N.E.; Chittka, L. The adaptive significance of sensory bias in a foraging context, floral colour preferences in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. PLoS ONE 2007, 2, e556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Kay, Q.O.N. Preferential pollination of yellow-flowered morphs of Raphanus raphanistrum by Pieris and Eristalis spp. Nature 1976, 261, 230–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Stanton, M.L. Reproductive biology of petal color variants of wild populations of Raphanus sativus L., I. pollinator response to color morphs. Am. J. Bot. 1987, 74, 176–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Horovitz, A. Edaphic factors and flower colour distribution in the Anemone coronaria (Ranunculaceae). Plant Syst. Evol. 1976, 126, 239–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Peitsch, D.; Fietz, A.; Hertel, H.; Souza, J.; Ventura, D.F.; Menzel, R. The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1992, 170, 23–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Chittka, L.; Kevan, P.G. Flower colour as advertisement. In Practical Pollination Biology; Dafni, A., Kevan, P.G., Husband, B.C., Eds.; Enviroquest Ltd.: Cambridge, ON, Canada, 2005; pp. 157–196. [Google Scholar]
  87. Dyer, A.G.; Spaethe, J.; Prack, S. Comparative psychophysics of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object detection. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2008, 194, 617–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Vos, P.; Hogers, R.; Bleeker, M.; Reijans, M.; Lee, T.V.D.; Hornes, M.; Frijters, A.; Pot, J.; Peleman, J.; Kuiper, M. AFLP, a new technique for DNA fingerprinting. Nucl. Acids Res. 1995, 3, 4407–4414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  89. Miller, M.P. Tools for Population Genetic Analyses (TFPGA), version 1.3; Department of Biological Sciences, Northern Arizona University: Flagstaff, AZ, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  90. Nei, M. Estimation of Average Heterozygosity and genetic distance from a small number of individuals. Genetics 1978, 89, 583–590. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  91. Raymond, M.; Rousset, F. An exact test for population differentiation. Evolution 1995, 49, 1280–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Evanno, G.; Regnaut, S.; Goudet, J. Detecting the number of clusters of individuals using the software STRUCTURE, a simulation study. Mol. Ecol. 2005, 14, 2611–2620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Earl, D.A. Structural harvester, a website and program for visualizing STRUCTURE output and implementing the Evanno method. Conserv. Genet. Resour. 2012, 4, 359–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Red, white and purple-blue flower morphs of Anemone coronaria in Israel, and some multi-coloured populations.
Figure 1. Red, white and purple-blue flower morphs of Anemone coronaria in Israel, and some multi-coloured populations.
Plants 09 00397 g001
Figure 2. The relative reflectance (%) curves of red, white and purple-blue A. coronaria flower colour morphs.
Figure 2. The relative reflectance (%) curves of red, white and purple-blue A. coronaria flower colour morphs.
Plants 09 00397 g002
Figure 3. The perception of red, white and purple-blue flower morphs of A. coronaria by honeybees demonstrated by their location in the bee colour hexagon [59]. Axes origin represents green leaves background.
Figure 3. The perception of red, white and purple-blue flower morphs of A. coronaria by honeybees demonstrated by their location in the bee colour hexagon [59]. Axes origin represents green leaves background.
Plants 09 00397 g003
Figure 4. Plot-bar of STRUCTURE Bayesian clustering of multilocus A. coronaria genotypes (462 polymorphic loci). K = a putative number of populations ranging from K = 1 to K = 23. Numbers under the bar plot are as following: 1. Mt. Meron—red, 2. Mt. Meron—white, 3. Mt. Meron – purple-blue, 4. Zippori—red, 5. Zippori—white, 6. Zippori—purple-blue, 7. Haifa University—red, 8. Nof Carmel—red, 9. Nof Carmel—purple-blue, 10. Alonim—red, 11. Alonim—white, 12. Alonim—purple-blue, 13. Beit Shearim—red, 14. Beit Shearim—white, 15. Balfouria—red, 16. Balfouria—white, 17. Balfouria—purple-blue, 18. Megiddo—red, 19. Megiddo—white, 20. Megiddo—purple-blue, 21. Purra—red, 22. Purra—red-peach, 23. Lahav—red.
Figure 4. Plot-bar of STRUCTURE Bayesian clustering of multilocus A. coronaria genotypes (462 polymorphic loci). K = a putative number of populations ranging from K = 1 to K = 23. Numbers under the bar plot are as following: 1. Mt. Meron—red, 2. Mt. Meron—white, 3. Mt. Meron – purple-blue, 4. Zippori—red, 5. Zippori—white, 6. Zippori—purple-blue, 7. Haifa University—red, 8. Nof Carmel—red, 9. Nof Carmel—purple-blue, 10. Alonim—red, 11. Alonim—white, 12. Alonim—purple-blue, 13. Beit Shearim—red, 14. Beit Shearim—white, 15. Balfouria—red, 16. Balfouria—white, 17. Balfouria—purple-blue, 18. Megiddo—red, 19. Megiddo—white, 20. Megiddo—purple-blue, 21. Purra—red, 22. Purra—red-peach, 23. Lahav—red.
Plants 09 00397 g004
Figure 5. Google map of Israel with indication of our study sites and their composition of floral colour morphs.
Figure 5. Google map of Israel with indication of our study sites and their composition of floral colour morphs.
Plants 09 00397 g005
Table 1. The number of landings of various insects on different colour morph flowers of A. coronaria, in 2014; the total number of flowers of each colour morph (pooled data from four populations in three observation sites. P value of Spearman’s χ2 test with df = 2.
Table 1. The number of landings of various insects on different colour morph flowers of A. coronaria, in 2014; the total number of flowers of each colour morph (pooled data from four populations in three observation sites. P value of Spearman’s χ2 test with df = 2.
VisitorWhite
Flowers
Purple-Blue FlowersRed FlowersTotalP
Honeybee5202520772<0.005
Andrena sp.7516091<0.005
Syrphid flies2470310.056
Beetles0011
Total visits6192751895
Number of flowers6943771151015
Table 2. The number of sampled plants (N), its average gene diversity (He—unbiased expected heterozygosity) and SE, and percent of polymorphism (P%) in each population (All), and each colour morph (Red, White, Purple-blue), as revealed by AFLP analysis (462 polymorphic loci).
Table 2. The number of sampled plants (N), its average gene diversity (He—unbiased expected heterozygosity) and SE, and percent of polymorphism (P%) in each population (All), and each colour morph (Red, White, Purple-blue), as revealed by AFLP analysis (462 polymorphic loci).
AllRedWhitePurple-Blue
PopulationNHeSEP(%)NHeSENHeSENHeSE
Mt. Meron280.1560.00787.7140.1390.00760.1330.00880.1910.008
Zippori410.1410.00789.2120.1090.006140.1530.007150.1140.007
Haifa University150.1150.00666.9150.1150.006------------
Nof Carmel190.1360.00777.980.0960.007------110.1530.007
Alonim190.1200.00667.8110.0960.00730.1090.00950.1540.008
Beit Shearim120.140.00763.6100.140.00720.0970.01------
Balfouria350.0940.00660.6150.0740.005120.0940.00780.1010.008
Megiddo340.1500.00693.7110.0770.006110.1770.008120.1780.007
Purra180.1080.00760.0140.0970.007------------
Lahav150.0840.00739.4140.0850.007------------
Total2360.1240.00270.71240.1020.006480.1360.007590.1460.007
Table 3. Study sites, their GPS (Global Positioning System), floral colour morphs (R-red, B-purple-blue, and W-white), and the analyses performed, visitor activity in 2011–12 and 2014, plant material collected for DNA analyses and colour analyses.
Table 3. Study sites, their GPS (Global Positioning System), floral colour morphs (R-red, B-purple-blue, and W-white), and the analyses performed, visitor activity in 2011–12 and 2014, plant material collected for DNA analyses and colour analyses.
Site NameGPS
N E
Colour
Morphs
Visitor Activity
2011-12
Visitor
Activity 2014
DNA
Analyses
Colour
Analysis
1. Alonei Abba32.73RBW
35.17
2. Alonim32.72RBW
35.15
3. Balfuria32.63RBW
35.29
4. Beit Lehem
Haglilit*
32.74RBW
35.19
5. Beit Shearim32.70RW
35.13
6. Haifa University32.75R
35.03
7. Lehavim32.37R
34.85
8. Megiddo32.59RBW
35.23
9. Mt. Meiron33.01RBW
35.40
10. Nahal Sanin32.64RBW
35.10
11. Nof Carmel32.75RBW
35.05
12. Pura31.50R
34.78
13. Tsippori32.76RBW
35.24
* Two populations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dafni, A.; Tzohari, H.; Ben-Shlomo, R.; Vereecken, N.J.; Ne’eman, G. Flower Colour Polymorphism, Pollination Modes, Breeding System and Gene Flow in Anemone coronaria. Plants 2020, 9, 397. https://doi.org/10.3390/plants9030397

AMA Style

Dafni A, Tzohari H, Ben-Shlomo R, Vereecken NJ, Ne’eman G. Flower Colour Polymorphism, Pollination Modes, Breeding System and Gene Flow in Anemone coronaria. Plants. 2020; 9(3):397. https://doi.org/10.3390/plants9030397

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dafni, Amots, Hagai Tzohari, Rachel Ben-Shlomo, Nicolas J. Vereecken, and Gidi Ne’eman. 2020. "Flower Colour Polymorphism, Pollination Modes, Breeding System and Gene Flow in Anemone coronaria" Plants 9, no. 3: 397. https://doi.org/10.3390/plants9030397

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop