Next Article in Journal
Power Differences and Dynamics in Multiparty Collaborative Systems: A Systematic Literature Review
Previous Article in Journal
Investigating Market Diffusion of Electric Vehicles with Experimental Design of Agent-Based Modeling Simulation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The “Fish Tank” Experiments: Metacognitive Awareness of Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) Significantly Increases Cognitive Complexity

1
Jeb E. Brooks School of Public Policy, Cornell Institute for Public Affairs, SC Johnson College of Business, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
2
Cabrera Research Lab, Ithaca, NY 14850, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Systems 2022, 10(2), 29; https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10020029
Submission received: 26 November 2021 / Revised: 14 January 2022 / Accepted: 19 January 2022 / Published: 4 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Complex Systems and Cybernetics)

Abstract

:
In the field of systems thinking, there are far too many opinioned frameworks and far too few empirical studies. This could be described as a “gap” in the research but it is more like a dearth in the research. More theory and empirical validation of theory are needed if the field and the phenomenon of systems thinking holds promise and not just popularity. This validation comes in the form of both basic (existential) and applied (efficacy) research studies. This article presents efficacy data for a set of empirical studies of DSRP Theory. According to Cabrera, Cabrera, and Midgley, DSRP Theory has equal or more empirical evidence supporting it than any existing systems theories (including frameworks, which are not theories). Four separate studies show highly statistically relevant findings for the effect of a short (less than one minute) treatment of D, S, R, and P. Subjects’ cognitive complexity and the systemic nature of their thinking increased in all four studies. These findings indicate that even a short treatment in DSRP is effective in increasing systems thinking skills. Based on these results, a longer, more in-depth treatment—such as a one hour or semester long training, such is the norm—would therefore likely garner transformative results and efficacy.

1. Introduction

There is currently a dearth1 of empirical research into what systems thinking is and how it can be improved. As a hypothetical example, a team of observers trained only in the current literature on systems thinking and shown one hundred instances of thinking would find it futile to determine empirically how many of those one hundred instances were instantiations of systems thinking. Nor would they be able to measure to what degree the instances were or were not systems thinking. In other words, we use the term systems thinking as if we know what it is and can reasonably measure it, when we cannot. Even more, many of the claims about systems thinking or definitions for it are not empirical to begin with, in that they have not or cannot be validated.
Cabrera [1] expanded on systems thinking theoretically by proposing [1,2,3,4,5,6] DSRP Theory, which details four empirical patterns of both mind and nature: identity–other Distinctions (D). part–whole Systems (S), action–reaction Relationships (R), and point–view Perspectives (P). Each pattern is composed of two elements. In its simplest form, DSRP Theory states:
“the ways information that which is Organized is/is not bounded Distinctions , arranged Systems , and interconnected Relationships from frames of reference Perspectives determines what actually exists Material Complexity ( Nature ) and what we think exists Cognitive Complexity ( Mind ) .”
But DSRP Theory entails more than is relayed by this simplified statement [1,7,8,9,10]. Cabrera, Cabrera, and Midgley [2], discussing DSRP Theory has launched a fourth wave in the field of systems thinking, pointed out that:
Since Cabrera’s first writings, we now have the benefit of over 20 years of hindsight on the possible start of a fourth wave (which is as long as the gap between the first and second waves, and twice as long as the gap between the second and third waves). During those years, we have seen considerable testing of Cabrera’s DSRP Theory, including: (1) a burgeoning amount of empirical evidence (at least as much as has been offered in the previous waves); (2) substantial private sector funding to develop tools for systems thinking; (3) substantial public funding for research; (4) a substantial peer review and publication history, sizeable citation histories, including several special issues dedicated to DSRP; (5) considerable public exposure and critique; (6) public adoption; (7) high attendance annual conferences; institutional recognition and support; and (9) as yet, few competitor theories (at least, none that have been explicated and communicated to the same degree).
Cabrera details DSRP Theory in a primer [11] and also elaborates on the the literature and evidence base for DSRP Theory ([1,3,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18]) as well as, specifically, the literature on: identity-other Distinction making (D) [5,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89]; part–whole Systems (S) [1,5,21,48,49,57,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105] action-reaction Relationships (R) [1,5,21,57,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,105,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120]; and, point-view Perspectives (P) [5,12,57,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136,137,138,139,140,141,142,143,144,145,146,147,148,149,150,151,152,153].
Cabrera’s 2021 review of research [12] builds upon two previous literature reviews [1,6], constitutes a proverbial “tip of the iceberg”, and is part of an accumulating body of evidence in support of the predictions made by DSRP Theory generally. Figure 1 shows the methodological distribution of this research and Figure 2 shows the distribution of these studies across DSRP pattern.

The Importance of Metacognition in Systems Thinking

Cabrera et al. (2021) [154] writes, “An important aspect of systems thinking is the act of metacognition. The process of deliberately structuring one’s thoughts using the four building blocks of cognition (D, S, R and P) requires awareness of, or thinking about, one’s own thinking, or metacognition” (p. 11, [154]). Systems thinking is synonymous with cognitive complexity. Thus, DSRP Theory further stipulates that awareness of the D, S, R, and P structures (i.e., “metacognition of DSRP”) can increase one’s effectiveness in thinking about systems, modeling systems, or in increasing cognitive fluidity, complexity and robustness. Table 1 shows the research matrix upon which our hypotheses, null hypotheses, research design, and findings are based.
Thus, the “fish tank studies” described herein is part of a research program that empirically tests the efficacy of DSRP in understanding Mind/Nature. Thus, this research addresses the questions: Is DSRP effective? Does metacognition of DSRP increase effectiveness in navigating cognitive complexity in order to understand system (ontological) complexity? This gets at the critically important question of “parallelism”—defined as the probability that our cognitive organizational rules align with nature’s organizational rules—which is central to the idea of the Systems Thinking/DSRP loop2 (Figure 3).
Metacognition is therefore intimately tied to systems thinking because it requires—at the very least—a recognition of mental models as existent. Beyond that, metacognition is required to build awareness and purposeful use of cognitive patterns that increase systemic thinking or cognitive complexity, rather than those structures that might cause to lessen it. Generally, research has shown that metacognitive awareness of a skill promotes and improves overall performance. As Stephen Fleming writes, “Insights into our own thoughts, or metacognition, is key to high achievement in all domains” [155]. This includes metacognitive awareness of the universal patterns of mind and nature: Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Empirical studies have shown that the DSRP patterns exist universally in the mind and in nature [3]. In this study, we aim to demonstrate that through isolating the Distinction, System, Relationship, and Perspective patterns using a short (<1 min) treatment, a significant effect can be made on each participant.
Metacognition, or the concept of “thinking about one’s thinking”, is not a new concept. Many credit John Flavell with the first use of the term metacognition in 1979 [156]. Flavell defined metacognition as, “metacognition refers to one’s knowledge concerning one’s own cognitive processes and products or anything related to them” (p. 232, [157]). His research focused on whether children were aware of their thinking and cognitive processes.
While Flavell coined the term, he was not the first to explore the idea of thinking about your thinking. Piaget [158] did his work in the early years of cognitive development research and wrote about “knowing the knowing and thinking the thinking” in The Psychology of Intelligence [158]. Aturk and Sahin [159] point out that the origin of thinking about your thinking could have happened much earlier.
“According to Georghiades (2004), being aware of one’s cognition was already been mentioned by Plato. Likewise, Aristotle pointed out that mind used a different power above and beyond seeing and hearing and thus laid the foundations for thinking about metacognition long before (Sandí-Ureña, 2008) [159].”
Thus, in this study we form a hypothesis that making someone aware (metacognitive) of systems thinking patterns (DSRP) would increase cognitive complexity (a.k.a., a quantitative measure of the emergent property of systems thinking). Therefore, the hypothesis of this study is that metacognitive awareness of each of the isolated patterns of DSRP (D, S, R, and P) will have a significant effect on the cognitive complexity/systems thinking of a participant’s thoughts on a simple task.
In what follows, we review the methods used for the four sub-studies (each isolating a pattern of DSRP), the results of these studies, and a discussion of these results. At the end, we summarize our findings.

2. Methods

Statistical analysis was performed using R (v3.6.3). Counts and percentages were used to summarize the distribution of categorical variables. The median and interquartile range were used to summarize the distribution of continuous variables. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare the distribution of raw counts, words, and characters between time points. Mixed Poisson and negative binomial regression were used to compare the distribution of counts, words, and characters between time points after adjusting for age, gender, race, and ethnicity. Hypothesis testing was performed at 5% level of significance.
The samples used in these studies were broken into four non-duplicative groups (N = 350 per study, N = 1400 across all four) who represented a normal distribution in the US population based on gender, education level, race, ethnicity, region (rural/urban/suburban), and age, balanced to match the census (general population).
In the study, the participants were shown a generic but detailed image of a fish tank (Figure 4).
For the first part (“Pre”) the participants were shown the above image on a screen and instructed to “Describe what you see in the image”. After they had written their answers, depending on the pattern they were randomly assigned to, they were taken to another screen.
For the Distinction study, after the participants filled in their answers in the PreD section, they were asked to read a “Distinction-making-prime” shown in Table 2.
Then participants were shown the same fish tank image again and asked, “Describe what you see in the image when applying the Distinction Rule you just learned (text copied below the image)”. This was called the Post-Distinction-making-prime (or “PostD”).
For the Systems study, after the participants filled in their answers in the PreS section, they were asked to read a “Systems-prime” shown in Table 3.
Then participants were shown the same fish tank image again and asked, “Describe what you see in the image when applying the Systems Rule you just learned (text copied below the image)”. This was called the Post-Systems-prime (or “PostS”).
For the Relationship study, after the participants filled in their answers in the PreR section, they were asked to read a “Relationship-prime” shown in Table 4.
Then participants were shown the same fish tank image again and asked, “Describe what you see in the image when applying the action–reaction Relationships Rule you just learned (text copied below the image)”. This was called the Post-Relationships-prime (or “PostR”).
For the Perspectives study, after the participants filled in their answers in the PreP section, they were asked to read a “Perspectives-prime” shown in Table 5.
Then participants were shown the same fish tank image again and asked, “Describe what you see in the image when applying the point-view Perspectives Rule you just learned (text copied below the image)”. This was called the Post-Perspectives-prime (or “PostP”).

3. Results

We measured the average time to read the treatment which is shown in Table 6. All read-times were less than one minute and together totaled 165.61 s or 2.76 min. To measure the shift in responses from PreX to PostX, a number of strategies were applied to eight different measures. We utilized counts of characters, words, and syllables of the raw data. Word clouds were used in both a qualitative and quantitative manner. Unique words were analyzed in the same way as the raw data. We also performed a textual analysis of word types and their synonyms. Lastly, we did a statistical analysis of the variance between the PreX and PostX conditions.
The word clouds shown in this paper are quantitative data organized visually. The size of each term signifies the frequency of use of the term. Importantly, according to research by Lewis and Frank [160] word length is a valid indicator of complexity of ideas. Indeed, Lewis and Frank showed that the length of a word in characters is correlated with conceptual complexity. Lewis and Frank write:
Hypothesis 1.
At the pragmatic timescale, we asked whether participants would be biased to assign a relatively long novel word to a conceptually more complex referent.
Hypothesis 2.
At the language evolution timescale, we asked whether languages tended to encoded conceptually more complex meanings with longer forms.
“We found support for both hypotheses”.
They showed experimentally that “visual complexity is highly correlated with an implicit measure—reaction time—and this measure predicts the bias to assign an object a long or a short word” (p. 35 [160]). Their experimental research also shows [3] that “explicit measures of conceptual complexity in English are highly correlated with word length in English, and the corpus analysis reveals a correlation between English complexity norms and word lengths in a diverse set of languages” (p. 35 [160]).

3.1. Distinctions

The quantitative data for the Distinctions (D) study is shown visually in the comparison of word clouds. Below is the PreD and PostD word cloud comparisons (Table 7).
The word clouds in Table 7 explicate the impact of the “Distinction-making prime”. The PostD word cloud is more detailed and more descriptive than the unprimed PreD word cloud. The larger a word is, the more times it is used. In the PostD word cloud adjectives and colors are more prevalent than in the PreD word cloud. PostD also has more unique words overall. The same patterns shown visually in the word clouds are in the quantitative data as well. The PostD responses have significantly more words overall and those words are more complex. Table 8 shows the differences.
Table 9 shows the correlations between words for Pre and Post D. Two plots show the increased interconnectivity and complexity between words before and after the treatment for Distinction.
Overall, the PostD responses were more specific than the unprimed PreD responses. This is shown in the word counts and the actual words themselves. It also shows their occurrences and percentage of total occurrences. The top 10 words in the unprimed PreD section is shown in Table 10.
After being primed with the Distinction pattern of mind, the participants used more descriptive adjectives, in particular, colors. After a very short (<1 min read) the participants increased the specificity of their distinctions. What was once just a “fish” became a “blue fish”, “yellow fish”, and “orange fish”. Table 11 lays out the top 40 words for the PreD and the PostD responses.
Results in Table 12 and Table 13 show that the distribution of number of concepts (i.e., the number of individual entries the subject made, “raw data”) was not significantly different before and after treatment (p = 0.062 using Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Data (N = 383) were summarized using median (IQR). Statistical analysis was performed using Wilcoxon-signed rank test. However, the median number of words used after treatment (Mdn = 6, IQR 3–10) was significantly higher than the median number of words used before treatment (Mdn = 4, IQR 3–7, p < 0.001 ***). Similarly, the median number of characters used after treatment (M = 29, IQR 14–53) was significantly higher than the median number of characters used before treatment (Mdn = 12, IQR 13–36, p < 0.001 ***).
Figure 5 shows the difference in the use of any given word (x-axis) between Post and Pre. Data were filtered to only include words mentioned more than five times, such that positive numbers indicate higher word counts post treatment. A positive number indicates that the word was used more post treatment while a negative indicates the less post treatment. For example, the word blue was used 93 more times post-treatment indicating more refined distinction making.
Results in Table 14 show that the expected average count was not significantly different before and after treatment (IRR = 1.05, p = 0.201). However, the expected number of words was higher by 43% post-treatment than before treatment (IRR = 1.43, p < 0.001 ***). Similarly, the expected average number of characters was higher by 41% post-treatment than pre-treatment (IRR = 1.41, p < 0.001 ***). Education and Age were not statistically significant factors in pre-post differences, indicating universality. Male gender was associated with lower expected number of words (IRR = 0.67, p < 0.001 ***) and characters (IRR = 0.69, p < 0.001 ***).

3.2. Systems

The quantitative data for the Systems (S) study is shown visually in the comparison of word clouds. Below is the PreS and PostS word cloud comparisons (Table 15).
The word clouds in Table 15 demonstrate the impact of the Systems prime. The PostS word cloud is more detailed and more descriptive than the unprimed PreS word cloud. The larger a word is, the more times it is used. Certain terminologies—such as ecosystem, system, whole, zoom, and part—are much more prevalent in the PostS and nonexistent in PreS. PostS also has more unique words overall. The same patterns shown visually in the word clouds are in the quantitative data as well. The responses in the PostS have significantly more words overall and those words are more complex. Table 16 shows the quantitative data analysis.
Overall, the PostS responses were more “systemic” than the unprimed PreS responses. This is shown in the words themselves including: system (38), part (23), whole (16), contain (12), zoom (12), group (8), habitat (6), together (6), community (4), environment (4), organisms (4), organized (2), entirety (2), biosystem (1), gestalt (1), microscopic (1), neighborhood (1), population (1). These terms make up 7.44% of the total words in the PostS data.
After being primed with the systems pattern of mind, the participants used more systemic language. They were more focused on part–whole aspects of the fish tank image. After a very short (<1 min read) the participants increased their focus on systems. Table 17 lays out the top 40 words for the PreS and the PostS responses. It also shows their occurrences and percentage of total occurrences.
Results in Figure 6 and Table 18 show that the distribution of concepts was significantly different before and after treatment (p =< 0.001 *** using Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Data was summarized using median (IQR). Statistical analysis was performed using Wilcoxon-signed rank test. The distribution of the number of words and characters was not significantly different before and after treatment.
Figure 7 shows the difference in the use of any given word (x-axis) between Post and Pre. Data were filtered to only include words mentioned more than five times, such that positive numbers indicate higher word counts post treatment. A positive number indicates that the word was used more post treatment while a negative indicates the less post treatment. For example, the word system was used 34 more times and the word ecosystem was used 23 more times post-treatment indicating that subjects identified systemic concepts with part–whole structure.
Data were analyzed (Table 19) using Poisson regression for concepts and using negative binomial regression for words and characters. Results showed that the expected number of concepts was lower by 13% after treatment than before treatment (IRR = 0.87, p < 0.001 ***). However, the expected number of words was higher by 12% post-treatment than before treatment (IRR = 1.12, p = 0.015 **). The expected number of characters was higher by 8% post-treatment than pre-treatment (IRR = 1.06, p = 0.091) although the result was statistically significant at the 0.1 level only. Male gender and younger age were associated with an overall lower number of words and characters. Ethnicity and education were not significantly different before and after intervention.

3.3. Relationships

The data for the Relationships (R) study is applied visually in the comparison of word clouds. Below is the PreR and PostR word cloud comparisons (Table 20).
The above word clouds demonstrate the impact of the “Relationships prime”. The PostR word cloud is more detailed and more descriptive than the unprimed PreR word cloud. The larger a word is, the more times it is used. Smaller words indicate more overall detail and more words used among each participant. Relational words—like relationship, and, to, between—are shown in the PostR and nonexistent in the PreR data. PostR also has more unique words overall. The same patterns shown visually in the word clouds are in the quantitative data as well. The responses in the PostR have significantly more words overall and those words are more relational. Table 21 shows the quantitative data analysis.
Overall, the PostR responses were more interrelational than the unprimed PreR responses. This is shown in the actual words used by the participants. This includes “connector” words such as: and (78), in (67), of (61), to (61), relationship (41), are (32), for (24), with (20), different (16), between (16). Relational words were 2.96 times more common, -ing words were 1.40 times more common, and verbs were 6.38 times more common in the PostR data than in PreR.
Table 22 lays out the top 40 words for the PreR and the PostR responses. It also shows their occurrences and percentage of total occurrences. The PostR condition includes many more connector words than the PreR.
Data (N = 382) were summarized using median (IQR). Statistical analysis was performed using Wilcoxon-signed rank test. Results in Figure 8, Figure 9 and Figure 10 and Table 23 show that the distribution of concepts was significantly different before and after treatment (p = < 0.001 *** using Wilcoxon signed-rank test) with a lower average number of concepts observed after treatment. The distribution of the number of words used after treatment (M = 4, IQR 2–9) was significantly different from that observed before treatment (M = 4, IQR 3–7, p = 0.003 *). The distribution of the number of characters used after treatment (M = 23, IQR 10–51) was significantly higher than the median number of words used before treatment (M = 23, IQR 13–38, p = 0.015 *).
Figure 11 shows the difference in the use of any given word (x-axis) between Post and Pre. Data were filtered to only include words mentioned more than five times, such that positive numbers indicate higher word counts post treatment. A positive number indicates that the word was used more post treatment while a negative indicates the less post treatment. For example, the word relationship was used 29 more times and the word swimming was used 15 more times post-treatment indicating that subjects identified relational concepts more often.
Data were analyzed (Table 24) using Poisson regression for concepts and using negative binomial regression for words and characters. Results showed that the expected number of concepts was lower by 30% after treatment than before treatment (IRR = 0.69, p < 0.001 ***). However, the expected number of words was higher by 21% post-treatment than before treatment (IRR = 1.21, p < 0.001 ***). Similarly, the expected number of characters was higher by 14% post-treatment than pre-treatment (IRR = 1.14, p = 0.004 **). Male gender, and younger age were associated with an overall lower number of words and characters. Ethnicity and education were not significantly different before and after intervention.

3.4. Perspectives

The data for the Perspectives (P) study is shown visually in the comparison of word clouds. Below is the PreP and PostP word cloud comparisons (Table 25).
The word clouds show the significant impact of the Perspectives prime. The PostP word cloud is more detailed and more descriptive than the unprimed PreP word cloud. The larger a word is, the more times it is used. There are perspectival words—such as perspectives and see—in the PostP data that are nonexistent in the PreP data. PostP also has more unique words overall. The same patterns shown visually in the word clouds are in the quantitative data as well. The responses in the PostP have significantly more words overall and those words are more perspectival. Table 26 shows the quantitative data analysis.
Table 27 lays out the top 40 words for the PreP and the PostP responses. It also shows their occurrences and percentage of total occurrences. The PostP condition includes many more unique words than the PreP. Perspectival words made up significantly more of the PostP total than in the PreP condition including words like: see (41), perspective (25), and cage (18).
Data (N = 362) were summarized using median (IQR). Statistical analysis was performed using Wilcoxon-signed rank test. Results in Figure 12, Figure 13 and Figure 14 and Table 28 show that the distribution of concepts was significantly different before and after treatment (p = 0.002 ** using Wilcoxon signed-rank test) with a lower average number of concepts observed after treatment. The median number of words used after treatment (Mdn = 6, IQR 3–10) was significantly higher than the median number of words used before treatment (Mdn = 5, IQR 3–8, p = 0.064) although the result was statistically significant at the 0.1 level. The median number of characters used after treatment (Mdn = 32, IQR 19–59) was significantly higher than the median number of words used before treatment (Mdn = 28, IQR 18–45, p = 0.013 **).
Figure 15 shows the difference in the use of any given word (x-axis) between Post and Pre. Data were filtered to only include words mentioned more than five times, such that positive numbers indicate higher word counts post treatment. A positive number indicates that the word was used more post treatment while a negative indicates the less post treatment. For example, the word perspective was used 20 more times and the word cage was used 18 more times post-treatment indicating that subjects took perspective more often.
Data were filtered to words mentioned > five times; positive numbers indicate higher count post treatment. Results showed (Table 29) that the expected number of concepts was significantly lower after treatment than before treatment (IRR = 0.85, p < 0.001 ***). However, the expected number of words was higher by 17% post-treatment than before treatment (IRR = 1.17, p < 0.001 ***). Similarly, the expected number of characters was higher by 19% post-treatment than pre-treatment (IRR = 1.19, p < 0.001 ***). Male gender, education, age, and ethnicity were not significantly different before and after intervention.

4. Discussion

Subjects were asked to describe a common scene before and after a simple treatment. The results are statistically unambiguous. However, one should consider some of the alternative explanations or mediating variables. There are a number of such things to consider.
For example, the post-treatment condition is the second time subjects were asked to describe the same scene. Thus, one might expect them—due to familiarity/repetition—to describe the scene with greater complexity. However, this explanation does not account for the increases in unique words, number of characters of unique words, number of syllables for unique words, and total unique words occurrence. As a mediating variable, familiarity/repetition also does not account for the correlation we see in the top words used Pre and Post. In other words, we see more and better post treatment. Familiarity/repetition might account for some aspect of the more, but not the better results. Another way to explain this is that in the post-treatment results, where we saw more and better (both quantitatively and qualitatively), we also saw more of the specific form of thinking: more perspectives, more distinctions, more part–whole structures, more interrelationships. Repetition/familiarity alone would not produce results of this specific nature.
One might also conclude that intentionality or a “social-desirability bias” (a form of response bias) played a role in the post-treatment results. For example, it may be that subjects, in their desire to please the researchers or be seen as “good at the task” simply did more post-treatment than pre-treatment. To assume such a mediating factor would be to grossly misunderstand the application of such bias. If we are surveying an individual, for example, on their drug use, one might reasonably expect an under-reporting due to such a bias. However, in this study, we are testing whether being made aware of a cognitive pattern/structure can effectively increase one’s cognitive complexity and the systemic nature of their thinking. In this case, intentionality is precisely what we are seeking to learn the effect of (a.k.a., metacognition). We are seeking to determine whether being made aware of a cognitive pattern—and therefore intentional in one’s thinking—produces a positive effect in the result of one’s thinking.
Finally, with regard to mediating factors, consider a hypothetical alternative study where the study design is the same but for one change to the treatment. In this hypothetical example, consider two variants:
1.
The treatment is something entirely random such as: Look for orange things such as Cheetos, oranges, basketballs, pumpkins or things that remind you of Cheetos, oranges, basketballs, pumpkins, etc.
2.
The treatment is something more purposefully cognitive and structural such as: consider the polarizing arguments that could be made about the scene.
Now consider what we might find in our two hypothetical studies and how those findings would differ from those of this study. In the first study (#1 above), one would hypothesize that more subjects would focus on and find more orange things. They would perhaps set their intention to discover more orange things in order to please the researchers. Overall, we would see an increase in orange things seen and described. In the second study (#2 above), one would hypothesize that more subjects would focus on and describe more polarizing aspects of the scene, such as the consideration of animal cruelty vs. pet-loving or agency vs. ownership or saltwater vs. freshwater. They would perhaps set their intention to discover more polarized examples in order to please the researchers. Overall, we would see an increase in the polarized examples described.
In the first hypothetical study, we would get relatively inconsequential results due to the random nature of the treatment. In the second hypothetical study, we would get relatively negative results due to the undesirable nature of the treatment (few of us are seeking ways to increase polarized-thinking). The point of these hypothetical examples is to tease out what is critically important in the fish tank studies described herein: that there is something quite remarkable about the cognitive structures of the treatments. These particular DSRP structures—when used purposefully and metacognitively—are neither random nor do they produce undesirable results, but are instead targeted and produce the desirable results of increased cognitive complexity, sophistication, robustness, and systemic thinking.
As just one of eight dimensions of difference between Pre and Post, Figure 16 shows the stark aggregate change from PreDSRP to PostDSRP (in terms of number of words).
Table 30 shows a summary of p-values across all four D, S, R, and P studies.
Table 31 summarizes the aggregate differences along eight dimensions across all four D, S, R, and P studies showing a stark difference in the rows and columns of increases (+s). The percentages are the result of Post minus Pre aggregates.

4.1. Distinctions

In Table 30, p-value results for Distinctions (D) indicate that there was statistically significant difference in individual subjects in the number of concepts they answered at the 0.1 level. Additionally, there was a highly statistically significant differences in both the number of words and characters. In addition, overall counts (in Table 31) in eight different categories of response data increased. This leads us to conclude that the very short treatment had a significant effect on the participant’s cognitive complexity. Figure 17 graphically represents the difference between the PreD and PostD data.
These findings indicate highly statistically significant and statistically significant increases in the degree to which people made more detailed distinctions and more distinctions from just a <1 min treatment. The implications this research has on metacognition and its relationship to cognitive complexity is substantial.

4.2. Systems

In Table 30, p-value results for Systems (S) indicate that there was a highly statistically significant difference in individual subjects in the number of concepts they answered, but not in the number of words and characters. This is most likely because listing additional parts would not require additional words and characters (e.g., complexity). For example, listing that a fish had a part “fin” increases the number of concepts but not necessarily words and characters. At the same time, overall counts (in Table 31) in eight different categories of response data increased. This leads us to conclude that the very short treatment had a significant effect on the participant’s cognitive complexity.
Figure 18 shows the graphical representation of the difference between the PreS and PostS data.
These findings indicate highly statistically significant and statistically significant increases in the degree to which people made more systemic answers from just a <1 min treatment. Again, the implications this research has on metacognition and its relationship to cognitive complexity is substantial.

4.3. Relationships

Relationship (R) results in Table 30, show p-value results that indicate that there was highly statistically significant difference in individual subjects in the number of concepts, words, and characters. In addition, overall counts (in Table 31) in eight different categories of response data increased. This leads us to conclude that the very short treatment had a significant effect on the participant’s cognitive complexity. Figure 19 shows the graphical representation of the difference between the PreR and PostR data.
These findings indicate highly statistically significant increases in the degree to which people made more interrelational answers from just a <1 min treatment. Again, the implications this research has on metacognition and its relationship to cognitive complexity is substantial.

4.4. Perspectives

Perspective (P) results in Table 30, show p-value results that indicate that there was statistically significant difference in individual subjects in the number of concepts and characters but not words. In addition, overall counts (in Table 31) in eight different categories of response data increased. This leads us to conclude that the very short treatment had a significant effect on the participant’s cognitive complexity. Figure 20 shows the graphical representation of the difference between the PreP and PostP data.
These findings indicate statistically significant increases in the degree to which people made take perspectives from just a <1 min treatment. Again, the implications this research has on metacognition of DSRP and its relationship to systems thinking and cognitive complexity are substantial.
Although it was not the primary focus of this study, there were no statistically significant differences Pre and Post for D, S, R, or P treatments based on either ethnicity or education. This indicates that DSRP is not effected by education or ethnicity, which is interesting given that most “educational” treatments would differ based on ethnicity (e.g., systemic racism in test scores) and education level (e.g., systemic racism in educational attainment)3. Whereas, there was a significant difference in S and R for younger individuals, suggesting that DSRP abilities may increase with life experience. There were also significant decreases in D, S, and R for males. Although further research is needed to determine the full scope and meaning of these results, these data may suggest that DSRP is egalitarian.

5. Conclusions

As a whole, these findings indicate both highly statistically significant and statistically significant increases cognitive complexity from a <1 min treatment. The PostD, PostS, PostR, and PostP studies substantially differ from the PreD, PreS, PreR, and PreP studies, respectively. We can conclude that:
1.
people trained in Distinction-making will have more detailed and specific thoughts, whereas;
2.
people trained in Systems-organizing will create more hierarchical structures and scale their thoughts up and down past the visual/conceptual question;
3.
people trained in Relationship building will create and identify more and better interrelationships, and;
4.
people trained in seeing Perspectives will see the stimulus from multiple points of view.
The implications this research has on metacognition and its relationship to cognitive complexity are substantial, especially when considering the very short and relatively shallow treatment. With a more substantive treatment, such as 1 h or 1 semester training (i.e., the training norm) the effects may be truly transformative. Future studies might vary the depth and length of the treatment or focus the treatment on sub-aspects of the D, S, R, or P patterns or elements, or on combinations thereof. In addition, similar studies could be undertaken in more specialized domains with demographically or psychographically specialized samples, rather than the general sample and content domain chosen for this study for the purpose of generalizability.
The plethora of different analyses of these data in this collection of studies clearly demonstrate that a short treatment of D, S, R, or P has both highly statistically significant and statistically significant effect on a participant’s cognitive and conceptual complexity. The “Fish Tank” experiments show that less than one minute of reading bulleted text can change a person’s thinking significantly. Further, the Fish Tank experiments show that the awareness of the universal patterns of mind (DSRP) improves the quality and quantity of a person’s observations.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed equally in methodology, software, validation, formal analysis, investigation, resources, data curation, writing—original draft preparation, writing—review and editing, visualization, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition. Development of DSRP Theory, D.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study, due to no collection of personal or identifying data.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy, human subjects, and ethical considerations.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
DSRPDSRP Theory (Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, Perspectives)
Didentity–other Distinctions
Spart–whole Systems
Raction–reaction Relationships
Ppoint-view Perspectives
STMISystems Thinking and Metacognition Inventory
IQRInterquartile Range
GLMMGeneralized Linear Mixed Modeling
RDSRelate-Distinguish-Systematize Jig

Notes

1
We would use the words “gap in the research here” but a gap implies something missing between two existing things. Whereas a dearth is a scarcity or lack of something altogether.
2
It should be noted that the ST/DSRP Loop is the mirror opposite of confirmation bias. Confirmation bias reverses this loop, by fitting reality to one’s mental models, whereas DSRP Systems Thinking fits mental models to real-world observables and feedback. Parallelism is therefore the degree to which one’s cognitive paradigm, style, or mindset, aligns with nature’s. One purpose of this research program is to determine the degree to which DSRP Theory accomplishes this parallelism.
3
In other words, Black, Hispanic and Native Americans statistically tend to be disadvantaged when it comes to educational attainment and get lower test scores overall than White or Asians Americans [161,162].

References

  1. Cabrera, D. Systems Thinking; Cornell University: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  2. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Midgley, G. (Eds.) The Four Waves of Systems Thinking. In Routledge Handbook of Systems Thinking; Routledge: London, UK, 2022; under process. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E.; Hernandez, O.; Sukhbaatar, U.; Fuqua, K.; Benitez Collante, A.E.; Lemaiyan, E.; Warugongo, N.; Sekyere, A.; et al. The Origin of Ideas: Empirical Studies in Cognitive Complexity; Odyssean Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  4. Cabrera, D. Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, Perspectives: The simple rules of complex conceptual systems. In Proceedings of the 52nd Annual Conference of the International Society for the Systems ScInternational Society for the Systems Sciences, Madison, WI, USA, 13–18 July 2008; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L. Systems Thinking Made Simple: New Hope for Solving Wicked Problems in a Complex World; Odyssean Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Lobdell, C. Systems Thinking. J. Eval. Program Plan. 2008, 31, 299–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L. Complexity and Systems Thinking Models in Education: Applications for Leaders. In Learning, Design, and Technology: An International Compendium of Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy; Spector, M.J., Lockee, B.B., Childress, M.D., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L. What Is Systems Thinking? In Learning, Design, and Technology: An International Compendium of Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy; Spector, M.J., Lockee, B.B., Childress, M.D., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019; pp. 1–28. [Google Scholar]
  9. Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, D. The Simple Rules of Complex Networks: Heuristics for Structural Predictions. In The Routledge Handbook for Systems Thinking; Cabrera, L., Cabrera, D., Midgley, G., Eds.; Routledge Press: London, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L. Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP): A Theory of Thinking and of Things. J. Eval. Program Plan. 2008, 31, 311–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L. DSRP Theory: A Primer. Systems 2022, 10, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. A Literature Review of the Universal Patterns and Atomic Elements of Complex Cognition. In The Handbook of Systems Thinking; Cabrera, D., Cabrera, L., Midgley, G., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cabrera, L.; Sokolow, J.; Cabrera, D. Developing and Validating a Measurement of Systems Thinking: The Systems Thinking and Metacognitive Inventory (STMI). In The Routledge Handbook for Systems Thinking; Cabrera, L., Cabrera, D., Midgley, G., Eds.; Routledge Press: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. Perspectives Organize Information in Mind and Nature: Empirical Findings of Point-View Perspective (P) in Cognitive and Material Complexity. Systems, 2022; under process. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. Distinctions Organize Information in Mind and Nature: Empirical Findings of Identity-Other Distinctions (D) in Cognitive and Material Complexity. Systems, 2022; under process. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. Relationships Organize Information in Mind and Nature: Empirical Findings of Action-Reaction Relationships (R) in Cognitive and Material Complexity. Systems, 2022; under process. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. Systems Organize Information in Mind and Nature: Empirical Findings of Part-Whole Systems (S) in Cognitive and Material Complexity. Systems, 2022; under process. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Midgley, G. (Eds.) Routledge Handbook of Systems Thinking; Routledge: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Spencer-Brown, G. Laws of Form; Allen & Unwin: London, UK, 1969. [Google Scholar]
  20. Krauss, L. Why Is There Something Instead of Nothing? Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  21. Euler, L. Solutio problematis ad geometriam situs pertinentis. Comment. Acad. Sci. Petropol. 1735, 8, 128–140. [Google Scholar]
  22. Kolata, G. Studying Learning In The Womb. Science 1984, 225, 302–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Partanen, E.; Kujala, T.; Näätänen, R.; Liitola, A.; Sambeth, A.; Huotilainen, M. Learning-induced neural plasticity of speech processing before birth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 15145–15150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Quinn, P.C.; Brown, C.R.; Streppa, M.L. Perceptual organization of complex visual configurations by young infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 1997, 20, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Newman, R.S.; Jusczyk, P.W. The cocktail party effect in infants. Percept. Psychophys. 1996, 58, 1145–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Gauthier, I.; Tarr, M.J. Becoming a “Greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision Res. 1997, 37, 1673–1682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Aubin, T.; Jouventin, P. Cocktail-party effect in king penguin colonies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B-Biol. Sci. 1998, 265, 1665–1673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Fry, S.N.; Wehner, R. Honey bees store landmarks in an egocentric frame of reference. J. Comp. Physiol. A-Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2002, 187, 1009–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Badre, D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bukach, C.M.; Gauthier, I.; Tarr, M.J.; Kadlec, H.; Barth, S.; Ryan, E.; Turpin, J.; Bub, D.N. Does acquisition of Greeble expertise in prosopagnosia rule out a domain-general deficit? Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 289–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Drews, G. Contributions of Theodor Wilhelm Engelmann on phototaxis, chemotaxis, and photosynthesis. Photosynth. Res. 2005, 83, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sengupta, A.; Kruppa, T.; Löwen, H. Chemotactic predator-prey dynamics. Phys. Rev. E 2010, 83, 031914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Berleman, J.E.; Kirby, J.R. Deciphering the hunting strategy of a bacterial wolfpack. FEMS Microbiol. Rev. 2009, 33, 942–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Pradel, E.; Zhang, Y.; Pujol, N.; Matsuyama, T.; Bargmann, C.I.; Ewbank, J.J. Detection and avoidance of a natural product from the pathogenic bacterium Serratia marcescens by Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 2295–2300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Rajalingham, R.; DiCarlo, J.J. Reversible Inactivation of Different Millimeter-Scale Regions of Primate IT Results in Different Patterns of Core Object Recognition Deficits. Neuron 2019, 102, 493–505.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Clark, T. National boundaries, border zones, and marketing strategy: A conceptual framework and theoretical model of secondary boundary effects. J. Mark. 1994, 58, 67–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Coye, D. The Sneakers/Tennis Shoes Boundary. American Speech 1986, 61, 366–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Powers, E.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, D. No Word Is an Island: Distinguishing “Nerd” and “Geek”. Cognitive Case Study; Cornell University: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  39. Peterson, M.A.; Skow-Grant, E. Memory and Learning in Figure–Ground Perception. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  40. Abdullah, M.H.; Klypin, A.; Wilson, G. Cosmological Constraints on Ω m and σ 8 from Cluster Abundances Using the GalWCat19 Optical-spectroscopic SDSS Catalog. Astrophys. J. 2020, 901, 90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. De Luca Picione, R.; Valsiner, J. Psychological Functions of Semiotic Borders in Sense-Making: Liminality of Narrative Processes. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2017, 13, 532–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Glanville, R. The Self and the Other: The Purpose of Distinction. In Cybernetics and Systems ’90, Proceedings of the European Meeting on Cybernetics and Systems Research, Singapore, 17–20 April 1999; World Scientific: Singapore, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  43. Durand, R.; Calori, R. Sameness, Otherness? Enriching Organizational Change Theories With Philosophical Considerations On The Same And The Other. AMRO 2006, 31, 93–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Gillette, J.M. Boundary Lines of Social Phenomena. Am. J. Sociol. 1925, 30, 585–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tajfel, H.; Wilkes, A.L. Classification and quantitative judgement. Br. J. Psychol. 1963, 54, 101–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Davies, C. Sexual Taboos and Social Boundaries. Am. J. Sociol. 1982, 87, 1032–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Langer, E.J.; Bashner, R.S.; Chanowitz, B. Decreasing prejudice by increasing discrimination. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1985, 49, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Perdue, C.W.; Dovidio, J.F.; Gurtman, M.B.; Tyler, R.B. Us and them: Social categorization and the process of intergroup bias. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 59, 475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Leudar, I.; Marsland, V. On membership categorization: ‘us’, ‘them’ and ‘doing violence’ in political discourse. Discourse Soc. 2004, 15, 243–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Young, J. On Insiders (Emic) and Outsiders (Etic): Views of Self, and Othering. Syst. Pract. Action Res. 2005, 18, 151–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Midgley, G.; Pinzón, L.A. Systemic mediation: Moral reasoning and boundaries of concern: Systemic mediation. Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 2013, 30, 607–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bentley, S.V.; Greenaway, K.H.; Haslam, S.A. Cognition in context: Social inclusion attenuates the psychological boundary between self and other. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 73, 42–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. King, S.L.; Connor, R.C.; Krützen, M.; Allen, S.J. Cooperation-based concept formation in male bottlenose dolphins. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 2373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Stafstrom, J.A.; Menda, G.; Nitzany, E.I.; Hebets, E.A.; Hoy, R.R. Ogre-Faced, Net-Casting Spiders Use Auditory Cues to Detect Airborne Prey. Curr. Biol. 2020, 30, 5033–5039.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Rubin, N. Figure and ground in the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 857–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Baylis, G.C.; Driver, J. Shape-coding in IT cells generalizes over contrast and mirror reversal, but not figure-ground reversal. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 937–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bateson, G. Form Substance and Difference. Essent. Readings Biosemiotics 1970, 501, 9. [Google Scholar]
  58. Darwin, C. On the Origin of Species: By Means of Natural Selection, or The Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life; John Murray: London, UK, 1859. [Google Scholar]
  59. Von Bertalanffy, L. The history and status of general systems theory. Acad. Manag. J. 1972, 15, 407–426. [Google Scholar]
  60. Rittel, H.W.J.; Webber, M.M. Dilemmas in a general theory of planning. Policy Sci. 1973, 4, 155–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Marchal, J.H. On the Concept of a System. Philos. Sci. 1975, 42, 448–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Goguen, J.A.; Varela, F.J. Systems And Distinctions: Duality And Complementarity. Int. J. Gen. Syst. 1979, 5, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ivan, M.; Kondo, K.; Yang, H.; Kim, W.; Valiando, J.; Ohh, M.; Salic, A.; Asara, J.M.; Lane, W.S.; Kaelin, W.G., Jr. HIFa Targeted for VHL-Mediated Destruction by Proline Hydroxylation: Implications for O Sensing. Science 2001, 292, 464–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Chen, P.; Fan, D.; Zhang, Y.; Selloni, A.; Carter, E.A.; Arnold, C.B.; Dankworth, D.C.; Rucker, S.P.; Chelikowsky, J.R.; Yao, N. Breaking a dative bond with mechanical forces. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 5635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wadhams, G.H.; Armitage, J.P. Making sense of it all: Bacterial chemotaxis. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 2004, 5, 1024–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Janetopoulos, C.; Firtel, R.A. Directional sensing during chemotaxis. FEBS Lett. 2008, 582, 2075–2085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Ikeya, N.; Woodward, J.R. Cellular autofluorescence is magnetic field sensitive. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2018043118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Förster, D.; Helmbrecht, T.O.; Mearns, D.S.; Jordan, L.; Mokayes, N.; Baier, H. Retinotectal circuitry of larval zebrafish is adapted to detection and pursuit of prey. Elife 2020, 9, e58596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. De Corte, B.J.; Navarro, V.M.; Wasserman, E.A. Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, R1264–R1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Schnell, A.K.; Loconsole, M.; Garcia-Pelegrin, E.; Wilkins, C.; Clayton, N.S. Jays are sensitive to cognitive illusions. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021, 8, 202358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Giannoni-Guzmán, M.A.; Rivera, E.; Aleman-Rios, J.; Melendez Moreno, A.M.; Ramos, M.P.; Pérez-Claudio, E.; Loubriel, D.; Moore, D.; Giray, T.; Agosto-Rivera, J.L. The Role of Colony Temperature in the Entrainment of Circadian Rhythms of Honey Bee Foragers. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 2021, 114, 596–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Mareschal, D.; Quinn, P.C. Categorization in infancy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001, 5, 443–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ashby, F.G.; Ell, S.W.; Waldron, E.M. Procedural learning in perceptual categorization. Mem. Cogn. 2003, 31, 1114–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Sloutsky, V.M. The role of similarity in the development of categorization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Lewis, M.; Balamurugan, A.; Zheng, B.; Lupyan, G. Characterizing Variability in Shared Meaning through Millions of Sketches. Proc. Annu. Meet. Cognit. Sci. Soc. 2021, 43. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lewandowsky, S.; Roberts, L.; Yang, L.X. Knowledge partitioning in categorization: Boundary conditions. Mem. Cogn. 2006, 34, 1676–1688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Sperry, R.W. Cerebral Organization and Behavior: The split brain behaves in many respects like two separate brains, providing new research possibilities. Science 1961, 133, 1749–1757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Lupyan, G. The conceptual grouping effect: Categories matter (and named categories matter more). Cognition 2008, 108, 566–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Van Dijk, J.; Kerkhofs, R.; van Rooij, I.; Haselager, P. Special Section: Can There Be Such a Thing as Embodied Embedded Cognitive Neuroscience? Theory Psychol. 2008, 18, 297–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Mahon, B.Z.; Caramazza, A. Concepts and categories: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 27–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Cira, N.J.; Benusiglio, A.; Prakash, M. Vapour-mediated sensing and motility in two-component droplets. Nature 2015, 519, 446–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Tweedy, L.; Thomason, P.A.; Paschke, P.I.; Martin, K.; Machesky, L.M.; Zagnoni, M.; Insall, R.H. Seeing around corners: Cells solve mazes and respond at a distance using attractant breakdown. Science 2020, 369, eaay9792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Blackiston, D.; Lederer, E.; Kriegman, S.; Garnier, S.; Bongard, J.; Levin, M. A cellular platform for the development of synthetic living machines. Sci. Robot. 2021, 6, eabf1571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Tarrant, M.; Calitri, R.; Weston, D. Social identification structures the effects of perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 973–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Takaoka, A.; Maeda, T.; Hori, Y.; Fujita, K. Do dogs follow behavioral cues from an unreliable human? Anim. Cogn. 2015, 18, 475–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Havy, M.; Waxman, S.R. Naming influences 9-month-olds’ identification of discrete categories along a perceptual continuum. Cognition 2016, 156, 41–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Mischel, W.; Ebbesen, E.B. Attention in delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1970, 16, 329–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Schnell, A.K.; Boeckle, M.; Rivera, M.; Clayton, N.S.; Hanlon, R.T. Cuttlefish exert self-control in a delay of gratification task. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2021, 288, 20203161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Boisseau, R.P.; Vogel, D.; Dussutour, A. Habituation in non-neural organisms: Evidence from slime moulds. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2016, 283, 20160446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Powers, E. A Unifying Theory of Systems Thinking with Psychosocial Applications. Syst. Res. 2015, 32, 534–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Simon, H.A. The Architecture of Complexity. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 1962, 106, 467–482. [Google Scholar]
  92. Anderson, J.R. The adaptive nature of human categorization. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Di Pellegrino, G. Categorization in single neurons. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001, 5, 186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Muehlhaus, J.; Heim, S.; Altenbach, F.; Chatterjee, A.; Habel, U.; Sass, K. Deeper insights into semantic relations: An fMRI study of part-whole and functional associations. Brain Lang. 2014, 129, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Montoro, P.R.; Luna, D.; Ortells, J.J. Subliminal Gestalt grouping: Evidence of perceptual grouping by proximity and similarity in absence of conscious perception. Conscious. Cogn. 2014, 25, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Baron-Cohen, S.; Ashwin, E.; Ashwin, C.; Tavassoli, T.; Chakrabarti, B. Talent in autism: Hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 1377–1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Lewin, K. Dynamic Theory of Personality; Read Books Ltd.: Redditch, UK, 1935. [Google Scholar]
  98. Mooney, R.L. Perception, language, and the part-whole problem. In Psychology of Reading; Garrard Press: New York, NY, USA, 1951. [Google Scholar]
  99. Ackoff, R.L. Towards a System of Systems Concepts. Manag. Sci. 1971, 17, 661–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Solomon, K.O.; Medin, D.L.; Lynch, E. Concepts do more than categorize. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 3, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Tversky, B.; Hemenway, K. Objects, parts, and categories. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1984, 113, 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Glushko, R.J.; Maglio, P.P.; Matlock, T.; Barsalou, L.W. Categorization in the wild. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Liberman, Z.; Woodward, A.L.; Kinzler, K.D. The Origins of Social Categorization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2017, 21, 556–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Fisher, M.; Keil, F.C. The Binary Bias: A Systematic Distortion in the Integration of Information. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 1846–1858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Kominsky, J.F.; Strickland, B.; Wertz, A.E.; Elsner, C.; Wynn, K.; Keil, F.C. Categories and Constraints in Causal Perception. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 28, 1649–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Weily, J. Review of Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine. Psychol. Bull. 1951, 46, 236–237. [Google Scholar]
  107. Wiener, N. Cybernetics or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
  108. Clement, C.A.; Falmagne, R.J. Logical reasoning, world knowledge, and mental imagery: Interconnections in cognitive processes. Mem. Cogn. 1986, 24, 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Gopnik, A.; Glymour, C.; Sobel, D.M.; Schulz, L.E.; Kushnir, T.; Danks, D. A theory of causal learning in children: Causal maps and Bayes nets. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 3–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Schulz, L.E.; Gopnik, A. Causal learning across domains. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 40, 162–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Greene, A.J. Making Connections. Sci. Am. Mind. 2010, 21, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Piao, Y.; Yao, G.; Jiang, H.; Huang, S.; Huang, F.; Tang, Y.; Liu, Y.; Chen, Q. Do pit vipers assess their venom? Defensive tactics of Deinagkistrodon acutus shift with changed venom reserve. Toxicon 2021, 199, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Chersi, F.; Ferro, M.; Pezzulo, G.; Pirrelli, V. Topological self-organization and prediction learning support both action and lexical chains in the brain. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2014, 6, 476–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  114. Ferry, A.L.; Hespos, S.J.; Gentner, D. Prelinguistic Relational Concepts: Investigating Analogical Processing in Infants. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 1386–1405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  115. Harris, P.L.; German, T.; Mills, P. Children’s use of counterfactual thinking in causal reasoning. Cognition 1996, 61, 233–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Mascalzoni, E.; Regolin, L.; Vallortigara, G.; Simion, F. The cradle of causal reasoning: Newborns preference for physical causality. Dev. Sci. 2013, 16, 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Rolfs, M.; Dambacher, M.; Cavanagh, P. Visual adaptation of the perception of causality. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, 250–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Dhamala, M. What is the nature of causality in the brain?—Inherently probabilistic: Comment on “Foundational perspectives on causality in large-scale brain networks” by M. Mannino and S.L. Bressler. Phys. Life Rev. 2015, 15, 139–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Sanefuji, W.; Haryu, E. Preschoolers’ Development of Theory of Mind: The Contribution of Understanding Psychological Causality in Stories. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  120. Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. An Ecology of Studies on Action-Reaction Relationships: Research into Universals of Systems Thinking and Cognitive Complexity in Mind and Nature. J. Syst. Think. 2021, 10, 21. [Google Scholar]
  121. Marvin, R.S.; Greenberg, M.T.; Mossler, D.G. The Early Development of Conceptual Perspective Taking: Distinguishing among Multiple Perspectives. Child Dev. 1976, 47, 511–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Premack, D.; Woodruff, G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1978, 1, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Endler, J.A. Bowerbirds, art and aesthetics: Are bowerbirds artists and do they have an aesthetic sense? Commun. Integr. Biol. 2012, 5, 281–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Thomas, B. Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors. Nat. Commun. 2016, 7, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  125. Chamovitz, D. What a Plant Knows: A Field Guide to the Senses. Plant Prot. Q. 2012, 27, 116. [Google Scholar]
  126. Montesinos-Navarro, A.; Storer, I.; Perez-Barrales, R. Benefits for nurse and facilitated plants emerge when interactions are considered along the entire life-span. Perspect. Plant Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2019, 41, 125483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Baron-Cohen, S.; Leslie, A.M.; Frith, U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 1985, 21, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Vallar, G.; Lobel, E.; Galati, G.; Berthoz, A.; Pizzamiglio, L.; Le Bihan, D. A fronto-parietal system for computing the egocentric spatial frame of reference in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 1999, 124, 281–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Dyson, F. Disturbing the Universe; Basic Books Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  130. Zwick, A.; Álvarez, G.A.; Kurizki, G. Maximizing Information on the Environment by Dynamically Controlled Qubit Probes. Phys. Rev. Applied 2016, 5, 014007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Álvarez, G.A. Email To Cabrera: “RE: Question about Your Article on Qubit Probes”; 2021. Available online: https://tinyurl.com/4876tv64 (accessed on 15 November 2021).
  132. Ruby, P.; Decety, J. How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 988–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Russell, J.; Alexis, D.; Clayton, N. Episodic future thinking in 3- to 5-year-old children: The ability to think of what will be needed from a different point of view. Cognition 2010, 114, 56–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Rakoczy, H.; Wandt, R.; Thomas, S.; Nowak, J.; Kunzmann, U. Theory of mind and wisdom: The development of different forms of perspective-taking in late adulthood. Br. J. Psychol. 2018, 109, 6–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  135. Mafessoni, F.; Lachmann, M. The complexity of understanding others as the evolutionary origin of empathy and emotional contagion. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 5794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. In Behavioral Decision Making; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  137. Schober, M.F. Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition 1993, 47, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Bateson, G. Perspective Taking: Imagining how another would feel versus imagining how you would feel. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 23, 751–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Knowles, M.L. Social rejection increases perspective taking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 55, 126–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Cavallo, A.; Ansuini, C.; Capozzi, F.; Tversky, B.; Becchio, C. When Far Becomes Near. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 28, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Neale, M.A.; Bazerman, M.H. The Role of Perspective-Taking Ability in Negotiating under Different Forms of Arbitration. ILR Rev. 1983, 36, 378–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Galinsky, A.D.; Moskowitz, G.B. Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 78, 708–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Takaku, S.; Weiner, B.; Ohbuchi, K.I. A cross-cultural examination of the effects of apology and perspective taking on forgiveness. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 20, 144–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  144. Parker, S.K.; Axtell, C.M. Seeing Another Viewpoint: Antecedents and Outcomes of Employee Perspective Taking. Acad. Manag. J. 2001, 44, 1085–1100. [Google Scholar]
  145. Epley, N.; Keysar, B.; Van Boven, L.; Gilovich, T. Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 87, 327–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  146. Davis, M.H.; Soderlund, T.; Cole, J.; Gadol, E.; Kute, M.; Myers, M.; Weihing, J. Cognitions associated with attempts to empathize: How do we imagine the perspective of another? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 30, 1625–1635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Harwood, M.D.; Farrar, M.J. Conflicting emotions: The connection between affective perspective taking and theory of mind. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 24, 401–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Tversky, B.; Hard, B.M. Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition 2009, 110, 124–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Wang, C.S.; Tai, K.; Ku, G.; Galinsky, A.D.; Urgesi, C. Perspective-Taking Increases Willingness to Engage in Intergroup Contact. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e85681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Ben-Ami Bartal, I.; Decety, J.; Mason, P. Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science 2011, 334, 1427–1430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  151. Satoh, S.; Bshary, R.; Shibasaki, M.; Inaba, S.; Sogawa, S.; Hotta, T.; Awata, S.; Kohda, M. Prosocial and antisocial choices in a monogamous cichlid with biparental care. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 1775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Gagliano, M.; Renton, M.; Depczynski, M.; Mancuso, S. Experience teaches plants to learn faster and forget slower in environments where it matters. Oecologia 2014, 175, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Gagliano, M. Inside the Vegetal Mind: On the Cognitive Abilities of Plants. In Memory and Learning in Plants; Baluska, F., Gagliano, M., Witzany, G., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 215–220. [Google Scholar]
  154. Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, D. Systems Thinking & Metacognition Inventory (STMI): A Validated Test of Systems Thinking. In The Routledge Handbook for Systems Thinking; Cabrera, L., Cabrera, D., Midgley, G., Eds.; Routledge Press: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  155. Fleming, S.M. Metacognition Is the Forgotten Secret to Success Insight into Our Own Thoughts, or Metacognition, Is Key to High Achievement in All Domains; Scientific American Mind: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  156. Flavell, J.H. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. Am. Psychol. 1979, 34, 906–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  157. Flavell, J.H. Metacognitive Aspects of Problem Solving. In The Nature of Intelligence; Resnick, L.B., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1976; pp. 231–236. [Google Scholar]
  158. Piaget, J. The Psychology of Intelligence; Routledge: London, UK, 1950. [Google Scholar]
  159. Akturk, A.O.; Sahin, I. Literature Review on Metacognition and its Measurement. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 15, 3731–3736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  160. Lewis, M.L.; Frank, M.C. The length of words reflects their conceptual complexity. Cognition 2016, 153, 182–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  161. De Brey, C.; Musu, L.; McFarland, J. Status and Trends in the Education of Racial and Ethnic Groups 2018; National Center for Education Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2018.
  162. Aud, S.; Fox, M.A.; KewalRamani, A. Status and Trends in the Education of Racial and Ethnic Groups; National Center for Education Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2010.
Figure 1. Knowledge-Method Matching Matrix (KMMM) analysis of empirical findings in DSRP across the disciplines.
Figure 1. Knowledge-Method Matching Matrix (KMMM) analysis of empirical findings in DSRP across the disciplines.
Systems 10 00029 g001
Figure 2. K-MMM analysis of empirical studies by DSRP pattern.
Figure 2. K-MMM analysis of empirical studies by DSRP pattern.
Systems 10 00029 g002
Figure 3. The ST/DSRP loop [11].
Figure 3. The ST/DSRP loop [11].
Systems 10 00029 g003
Figure 4. Fish tank image used in the experiments.
Figure 4. Fish tank image used in the experiments.
Systems 10 00029 g004
Figure 5. Difference in word count before and after D treatment.
Figure 5. Difference in word count before and after D treatment.
Systems 10 00029 g005
Figure 6. Distribution of Systems (S) concepts (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 6. Distribution of Systems (S) concepts (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g006
Figure 7. Difference in word count before and after S treatment.
Figure 7. Difference in word count before and after S treatment.
Systems 10 00029 g007
Figure 8. Distribution of concepts for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 8. Distribution of concepts for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g008
Figure 9. Distribution of words for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 9. Distribution of words for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g009
Figure 10. Distribution of characters for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 10. Distribution of characters for PreR and PostR (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g010
Figure 11. Difference in word count before and after R treatment.
Figure 11. Difference in word count before and after R treatment.
Systems 10 00029 g011
Figure 12. Distribution of concepts for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 12. Distribution of concepts for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g012
Figure 13. Distribution of words for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 13. Distribution of words for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g013
Figure 14. Distribution of characters for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Figure 14. Distribution of characters for PreP and PostP (vertical lines represent median).
Systems 10 00029 g014
Figure 15. Difference in word count before and after P treatment.
Figure 15. Difference in word count before and after P treatment.
Systems 10 00029 g015
Figure 16. Aggregate DSRP Pre/Post graphical comparison.
Figure 16. Aggregate DSRP Pre/Post graphical comparison.
Systems 10 00029 g016
Figure 17. Comparison of PreD and PostD data.
Figure 17. Comparison of PreD and PostD data.
Systems 10 00029 g017
Figure 18. Comparison of PreS and PostS data.
Figure 18. Comparison of PreS and PostS data.
Systems 10 00029 g018
Figure 19. Comparison of PreR and PostR data.
Figure 19. Comparison of PreR and PostR data.
Systems 10 00029 g019
Figure 20. Comparison of PreP and PostP data.
Figure 20. Comparison of PreP and PostP data.
Systems 10 00029 g020
Table 1. Four dimensions of research program.
Table 1. Four dimensions of research program.
Existential
(Basic Research)
Efficacy
(Applied Research)
Mind
(cognitive complexity)
Does DSRP Exist in Mind?
(i.e., Does DSRP exist as universal, material, observable cognitive phenomena?)
Is Metacognitive Awareness of DSRP Effective?
(i.e., Does it increase ability to align cognitive complexity to real-world complexity? (a.k.a., parallelism)
Nature
(ontological complexity)
Does DSRP Exist in Nature?
(i.e., Does DSRP exist as universal, material, observable phenomena?)
EMPIRICAL BASIS
Table 2. Distinction treatment.
Table 2. Distinction treatment.
Things to consider from the identity–other Distinction Rule (D):
  • Distinctions are all around us, it’s how we name, identify and differentiate things, ideas, or objects from one another.
  • The identity–other structure of distinctions means that any object or idea is both an identity and an other (e.g., “us” vs. “them”).
  • The distinctions you make can be general and/or specific (e.g., “a cup” vs. “a red porcelain cup”).
  • Often a single distinction can become many more distinctions when looks closer at its meaning (e.g., “birds” can be further distinguished to be owls, eagles, seagulls).
Table 3. Systems treatment.
Table 3. Systems treatment.
Things to consider from the part–whole Systems Rule (S):
  • Systems are all around us, it’s how ideas or objects are organized, grouped or nested with one another.
  • The part–whole structure of systems means that any object or idea is both a part and a whole simultaneously (e.g., a planet is comprised of land and water and is also part of the solar system).
  • In any whole system, you want to identify the relevant parts to better understand that system.
  • The systems rule tells us that we can “zoom in” to see more parts and “zoom out” to see more wholes (e.g., zoom in to see the land and water parts of a planet, zoom out to see that planet as part of the solar system).
Table 4. Relationships treatment.
Table 4. Relationships treatment.
Things to consider from the action–reaction Relationships Rule (R):
  • The Relationship rule reminds us to identify and examine the relationships among all the parts of a system. In any system, you want to see not only the nodes—but also the relevant relationships among them to better understand that system.
  • The action–reaction structure of relationships means that any object or idea is an action or reaction (e.g., Person A can act upon Person B or react to Person B).
  • The R rule encourages not only to recognize that a relationship exists but to distinguish that relationship to better understand it (i.e., by naming it, for example the relationship between “mom” and “dad” is “marriage”).
  • The R rule encourages not only to recognize that a relationship exists but also to zoom into that relationship to see its constituent parts (e.g., the relationship between a farmer and consumer is a vast supply chain made up of many parts; the synaptic relationship between = neurons is made up of electrochemical components).
Table 5. Perspectives treatment.
Table 5. Perspectives treatment.
Things to consider from the point-view Perspectives Rule (P):
  • The Perspectives rule reminds us to examine systems from multiple perspectives to better understand any system.
  • The point–view structure of Perspectives means that any object or idea can be a point and/or a view (e.g., A person (point) can see another person (view); or different states (point) see the parts of marriage (view) differently).
  • The Perspectives Rule encourages us to take both perspectives “with eyes” (e.g., people, stakeholders, groups, countries, animals) but also non-human perspectives (e.g., economic, political, historical, structural, strengths, weaknesses, color, etc.).
  • When you change the way you look at things (Perspective), the things you look at change (e.g., the Southern perspective on the Civil War includes different things than the Northern perspective on the Civil War).
  • Perspectives can be used as a frame on a system that can either limit/narrow or expand/widen what you see (e.g., looking only at a system from an economic-impact perspective limits what is included while taking a holistic perspective broadens the view).
Table 6. Treatment read-time averages.
Table 6. Treatment read-time averages.
D-treatment28.11 s
S-treatment35.19 s
R-treatment51.91 s
P-treatment50:40 s
Total165.61 s
Table 7. Word cloud of response before and after distinction prompt.
Table 7. Word cloud of response before and after distinction prompt.
PreDPostD
Systems 10 00029 i001 Systems 10 00029 i002
Table 8. PreD and PostD aggregate response data.
Table 8. PreD and PostD aggregate response data.
PreDPostDDifference
Number of characters (including spaces)17,69124,308+27.22%
Number of characters (without spaces)10,29114,752+30.24%
Number of words (including repeated words)20983071+31.68%
Number of syllables (including repeated words)32464558+28.78%
Unique words251453+44.59%
Number of characters (no spaces) for unique words14182626+46.00%
Number of syllables for unique words492901+45.39%
Total unique words occurrence18322680+31.64%
Table 9. Correlation between top words used Pre and Post.
Table 9. Correlation between top words used Pre and Post.
PreDPostD
Systems 10 00029 i003 Systems 10 00029 i004
Table 10. Comparison of top 10 words.
Table 10. Comparison of top 10 words.
PreD Top 10 WordsPostD Top 10 Words
FishFish
WaterBlue
AquariumYellow
Fish tankWater
RockRock
PlantPlant
ColorOrange
BlueAquarium
TankColor
CoralGreen
Table 11. PreD and PostD top 40 terms used.
Table 11. PreD and PostD top 40 terms used.
PreD (Total 1848)PostD (Total 2695)
RankWordOccurs%WordOccurs%
1fish41022.19%fish54220.11%
2water1246.71%blue1455.38%
3aquarium1186.39%yellow1154.27%
4fish tank924.98%water1144.23%
5rock854.60%rock983.64%
6plant844.55%plant802.97%
7color653.52%orange742.75%
8blue522.81%aquarium662.45%
9tank432.33%color562.08%
10coral412.22%green471.74%
11yellow271.46%goldfish421.56%
12stone241.30%tank401.48%
13see221.19%fish tank401.48%
14beautiful201.08%coral351.30%
15tropical170.92%different311.15%
16orange170.92%small271.00%
17gravel160.87%white220.82%
18goldfish150.81%beautiful200.74%
19pebbles150.81%stone190.71%
20different130.70%pebbles190.71%
21lots120.65%gravel170.63%
22grass110.60%swimming170.63%
23swimming110.60%with170.63%
24some110.60%see170.63%
25seaweed100.54%many160.59%
26decorations100.54%vase160.59%
27very90.49%clear150.56%
28filter90.49%that130.48%
29tree80.43%broken120.45%
30green80.43%very120.45%
31clean80.43%red120.45%
32fake80.43%tropical110.41%
33pipe70.38%seaweed110.41%
34many70.38%vs100.37%
35vase70.38%grey100.37%
36nice70.38%colored90.33%
37sand70.38%light90.33%
38life60.32%large90.33%
39like60.32%multiple80.30%
40colored60.32%pipe80.30%
Table 12. Distribution of words and characters (vertical lines represent median) for Distinctions (D).
Table 12. Distribution of words and characters (vertical lines represent median) for Distinctions (D).
Systems 10 00029 i005 Systems 10 00029 i006
Table 13. Comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after D-treatment.
Table 13. Comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after D-treatment.
Pre PostP.overall
No. concepts3.00 [1.00; 4.00]3.00 [1.00; 4.00]0.062
No. words4.00 [3.00; 7.00]6.00 [3.00; 10.0]<0.001
No. characters21.0 [13.0; 36.0]29.0 [14.0; 53.0]<0.001
Table 14. D mixed generalized linear regression.
Table 14. D mixed generalized linear regression.
No. ConceptsNo. WordsNo. Characters
PredictorsIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate Ratiosp
(Intercept)2.69 (1.91–3.77)<0.0013.07 (2.01–4.69)<0.00117.31 (11.43–26.21)<0.001
Time (Post vs. Pre)1.05 (0.97–1.14)0.2011.43 (1.34–1.53)<0.0011.41 (1.31–1.51)<0.001
Age (1 level increase)0.98 (0.96–1.01)0.1751.00 (0.97–1.04)0.8781.01 (0.97–1.04)0.749
Education (1 level increase)0.00 (0.96–1.04)0.9131.00 (0.95–1.05)0.9721.00 (0.95–1.04)0.907
Ethnicity (Not Latino and/or Hispanic)0.97 (0.81–1.16)0.7501.04 (0.83–1.30)0.7341.03 (0.83–1.28)0.803
Gender (Male)0.89 (0.79–1.00)0.0530.67 (0.58–0.78)<0.0010.69 (0.60–0.80)<0.001
Marginal R 2 /Conditional R 2 0.013/0.3650.107/0.6640.095/0.621
Table 15. Word cloud of response before and after system prompt.
Table 15. Word cloud of response before and after system prompt.
PreSPostS
Systems 10 00029 i007 Systems 10 00029 i008
Table 16. PreS and PostS aggregate response data.
Table 16. PreS and PostS aggregate response data.
PreSPostSDifference
Number of characters (including spaces)17,06119,367+11.91%
Number of characters (without spaces)10,31811,350+9.09%
Number of words (including repeated words)20922410+13.20%
Number of syllables (including repeated words)32073654+12.23%
Unique words243416+41.59%
Number of characters (no spaces) for unique words12262414+49.21%
Number of syllables for unique words472828+43.00%
Total unique words occurrence19112009+4.88%
Table 17. PreS and PostS top 40 terms used.
Table 17. PreS and PostS top 40 terms used.
PreS (Total 1911)PostS (Total 2009)
RankWordOccurs%WordOccurs%
1fish41619.78%fish30512.66%
2water1366.47%water1456.02%
3aquarium1316.23%aquarium923.82%
4rock944.47%plant923.82%
5plant864.09%rock682.82%
6fish tank773.66%fish tank562.32%
7blue612.90%tank441.83%
8coral572.71%and401.66%
9color411.95%blue391.62%
10tank391.85%system381.58%
11yellow351.66%color271.12%
12gravel311.47%ecosystem271.12%
13orange311.47%coral241.00%
14and281.33%gravel241.00%
15stone221.05%part230.95%
16with221.05%see190.79%
17seaweed170.81%stone180.75%
18green160.76%with180.75%
19swimming160.76%swimming160.66%
20pebbles150.71%vase160.66%
21tropical150.71%whole160.66%
22vase150.71%yellow150.62%
23decorations140.67%pebbles130.54%
24white140.67%sea130.54%
25broken130.62%contain120.50%
26filter130.62%green120.50%
27reef120.57%orange120.50%
28see110.52%this120.50%
29different100.48%zoom120.50%
30light100.48%decorations110.46%
31saltwater100.48%school110.46%
32tree100.48%different100.41%
33lots90.43%on100.41%
34on90.43%bubbles90.37%
35sand90.43%light90.37%
36sea90.43%living90.37%
37bottom80.38%also80.33%
38many80.38%glass80.33%
39small80.38%group80.33%
40grass70.33%life80.33%
Table 18. S comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after treatment.
Table 18. S comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after treatment.
PrePostP.overall
No. concepts3.00 [1.00; 5.00]3.00 [1.00; 4.00]<0.001
No. words4.00 [3.00; 7.00]4.00 [2.00; 7.00]0.13
No. characters23.0 [14.0; 35.8]22.0 [13.0; 39.0]0.13
Table 19. S mixed generalized linear regression.
Table 19. S mixed generalized linear regression.
No. ConceptsNo. WordsNo. Characters
PredictorsIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate Ratiosp
Time (Post vs. Pre)0.87 (0.81–0.95)<0.0011.12 (1.02–1.22)0.0151.08 (0.99–1.17)0.091
Age (1 level increase)1.02 (0.99–1.04)0.2591.06 (1.03–1.10)<0.0011.06 (1.03–1.10)<0.001
Education (1 level increase)0.99 (0.95–1.03)0.5791.00 (0.96–1.05)0.8471.01 (0.97–1.06)0.622
Ethnicity (Not Latino and/or Hispanic)1.06 (0.88–1.28)0.5380.99 (0.79–1.25)0.9590.98 (0.79–1.22)0.847
Gender (Male)0.93 (0.82–1.05)0.2530.85 (0.73–0.99)0.0390.85 (0.73–0.98)0.027
Marginal R 2 /Conditional R 2 0.017/0.3990.039/0.4830.040/0.470
Table 20. Word cloud of response before and after relationships prompt.
Table 20. Word cloud of response before and after relationships prompt.
PreRPostR
Systems 10 00029 i009 Systems 10 00029 i010
Table 21. PreR and PostR aggregate response data.
Table 21. PreR and PostR aggregate response data.
PreRPostRDifference
Number of characters (including spaces)18,44321,965+16.03%
Number of characters (without spaces)11,27113,132+14.17%
Number of words (including repeated words)22482814+20.11%
Number of syllables (including repeated words)35322814+20.11%
Unique words279466+40.13%
Number of characters (no spaces) for unique words15782684+41.21%
Number of syllables for unique words537926+42.01%
Total unique words occurrence21382553+16.26%
Table 22. PreR and PostR top 40 terms used.
Table 22. PreR and PostR top 40 terms used.
PreR (Total 2138)PostR (Total 2553)
RankWordOccurs%WordOccurs%
1fish44019.78%fish40414.36%
2water1516.79%water1545.47%
3aquarium1275.71%and782.77%
4rock1165.21%in672.38%
5plant994.45%plant622.38%
6blue652.92%of612.17%
7fish tank642.88%to612.17%
8coral552.47%aquarium561.99%
9color431.93%rock491.74%
10tank411.80%blue411.46%
11yellow401.80%relationship411.46%
12gravel351.57%tank401.42%
13orange331.48%are321.14%
14of311.39%is301.07%
15in241.08%swimming281.00%
16and200.90%color260.92%
17filter200.90%for240.85%
18pebbles200.90%yellow230.82%
19vase190.85%coral210.75%
20see170.76%with200.71%
21tropical170.76%good190.68%
22goldfish160.72%other190.68%
23seaweed160.72%ecosystem170.60%
24with160.72%different160.57%
25decorations130.58%environment160.57%
26swimming130.58%fish tank160.57%
27different120.54%that160.57%
28reef120.54%between150.53%
29broken110.49%green150.53%
30green110.49%need150.53%
31fake100.45%be140.50%
32life100.45%filter140.50%
33saltwater100.45%goldfish140.50%
34decoration90.40%on140.50%
35is90.40%each130.46%
36small90.40%orange130.46%
37aquatic80.36%living120.43%
38are80.36%can110.39%
39pipe80.36%oxygen110.39%
40red80.36%school110.39%
Table 23. R comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after treatment.
Table 23. R comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after treatment.
PrePostP.overall
No. concepts3.00 [2.00; 5.00]2.00 [1.00; 3.00]<0.001
No. words4.00 [3.00; 7.00]4.00 [2.00; 9.00]0.003
No. characters23.0 [13.0; 38.0]23.0 [10.2; 50.8]0.015
Table 24. R mixed generalized linear regression.
Table 24. R mixed generalized linear regression.
No. ConceptsNo. WordsNo. Characters
PredictorsIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate Ratiosp
Time (Post vs. Pre)0.69 (0.63–0.75)<0.0011.21 (1.11–1.32)<0.0011.14 (1.04–1.24)0.004
Age (1 level increase)1.00 (0.97–1.03)0.9481.03 (1.00–1.07)0.0501.04 (1.00–1.07)0.031
Education (1 level increase)0.99 (0.95–1.02)0.5111.01 (0.96–1.06)0.6681.01 (0.96–1.06)0.752
Ethnicity (Not Latino and/or Hispanic)1.00 (0.85–1.17)0.9621.06 (0.85–1.32)0.5971.04 (0.84–1.28)0.728
Gender (Male)0.93 (0.83–1.05)0.2510.75 (0.65–0.88)<0.0010.77 (0.66–0.89)<0.001
Marginal R2 / Conditional R20.076/0.3700.051/0.5170.041/0.481
Table 25. Word cloud of response before and after perspective prompt.
Table 25. Word cloud of response before and after perspective prompt.
PrePPostP
Systems 10 00029 i011 Systems 10 00029 i012
Table 26. PreP and PostP aggregate response data.
Table 26. PreP and PostP aggregate response data.
PrePPostPDifference
Number of characters (including spaces)19,75822,371+11.68%
Number of characters (without spaces)12,33613,794+10.57%
Number of words (including repeated words)25132915+13.79%
Number of syllables (including repeated words)39674483+11.51%
Unique words276497+44.47%
Number of characters (no spaces) for unique words15982914+45.16%
Number of syllables for unique words533991+46.22%
Total unique words occurrence20892322+10.03%
Table 27. PreP and PostP top 40 terms used.
Table 27. PreP and PostP top 40 terms used.
PreP (Total 2089)PostP (Total 2322)
RankWordOccurs%WordOccurs%
1fish40319.29%fish35015.07%
2aquarium1426.80%water1064.57%
3water1386.61%aquarium903.88%
4rock974.64%color863.70%
5color944.50%rock572.45%
6plant884.21%blue482.07%
7blue683.26%plant451.94%
8fish tank602.87%tank421.81%
9coral512.44%see411.77%
10tank452.15%fish tank381.64%
11yellow371.77%swimming291.25%
12orange281.28%coral261.12%
13tropical281.34%perspective251.08%
14gravel271.29%with230.99%
15many241.15%different220.95%
16see231.10%yellow210.90%
17swimming211.01%cage180.78%
18beautiful180.86%small180.78%
19pebbles170.81%beautiful170.73%
20saltwater160.77%be160.69%
21stone160.77%clean150.65%
22different150.72%ecosystem150.65%
23goldfish150.72%there150.65%
24seaweed150.72%decoration140.60%
25decoration130.62%green140.60%
26filter130.62%gravel130.56%
27green130.62%life130.56%
28small120.57%tropical130.56%
29reef110.53%filter120.52%
30vase110.53%lots120.52%
31greenery100.48%many120.52%
32sand90.43%other120.52%
33some90.43%sea120.52%
34life80.38%large110.47%
35sea80.38%orange110.47%
36aquatic70.34%saltwater110.47%
37bottom70.34%that110.47%
38clean70.34%environment100.43%
39light70.34%habitat100.43%
40nice70.34%home100.43%
Table 28. Comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after P treatment.
Table 28. Comparison of raw counts, words, and characters before and after P treatment.
PrePostP.overall
No. concepts
Median [IQR]3.00 [1.00; 5.00]3.00 [1.00; 4.00]0.002
Mean (SD)3.51 (2.36)2.99 (2.15)0.002
3.00 [1.00; 4.00]3.00 [1.00; 4.00]0.062
No. words5.00 [3.00; 8.00]6.00 [3.00; 10.0]0.064
No. characters28.0 [18.0; 45.0]32.0 [19.0; 58.8]0.013
Table 29. P mixed generalized linear regression.
Table 29. P mixed generalized linear regression.
No. ConceptsNo. WordsNo. Characters
PredictorsIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate RatiospIncidence Rate Ratiosp
(Intercept)4.12 (3.25–5.21)<0.0014.88 (3.68–6.46)<0.00125.69 (19.66–33.57)<0.001
Time (Post vs. Pre)0.85 (0.79–0.93)<0.0011.17 (1.09–1.26)<0.0011.19 (1.11–1.27)<0.001
Age (1 level increase)0.97 (0.95–1.00)0.0511.01 (0.98–1.04)0.5311.02 (0.99–1.05)0.253
Education (1 level increase)1.01 (0.97–1.05)0.6881.02 (0.97–1.07)0.4291.02 (0.97–1.06)0.439
Ethnicity (Not Latino and/or Hispanic)0.86 (0.71–1.04)0.1281.11 (0.88–1.40)0.3571.12 (0.90–1.40)0.293
Gender (Male)0.91 (0.80–1.03)0.1191.00 (0.86–1.15)0.9821.01 (0.88–1.16)0.837
Marginal R 2 /Conditional R 2 0.032/0.3940.015/0.5730.021/0.562
Table 30. Summary p-values for D, S, R, and P Pre and Post treatments.
Table 30. Summary p-values for D, S, R, and P Pre and Post treatments.
p
Identity–Other Distinctions (D)
No. concepts0.062
No. words<0.001
No. characters<0.001
Part–Whole Systems (S)
No. concepts<0.001
No. words0.13
No. characters0.13
Action–Reaction Relationships (R)
No. concepts<0.001
No. words0.003
No. characters0.015
Point–View Perspectives (P)
No. concepts0.002
No. words0.064
No. characters0.013
Table 31. Difference for Pre and Post D, S, R, and P.
Table 31. Difference for Pre and Post D, S, R, and P.
DSRP
Number of characters (including spaces)+27.22%+11.91%+16.03%+11.68%
Number of characters (without spaces)+30.24%+09.09%+14.17%+10.57%
Number of words (including repeated words)+31.68%+13.20%+20.11%+13.79%
Number of syllables (including repeated words)+28.78%+12.23%+20.11%+11.51%
Unique words+44.59%+41.59%+40.13%+44.47%
Number of characters (no spaces) for unique words+46.00%+49.21%+41.21%+45.16%
Number of syllables for unique words+45.39%+43.00%+42.01%+46.22%
Total unique words occurrence+31.64%+04.88%+16.26%+10.03%
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cabrera, D.; Cabrera, L.; Cabrera, E. The “Fish Tank” Experiments: Metacognitive Awareness of Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) Significantly Increases Cognitive Complexity. Systems 2022, 10, 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10020029

AMA Style

Cabrera D, Cabrera L, Cabrera E. The “Fish Tank” Experiments: Metacognitive Awareness of Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) Significantly Increases Cognitive Complexity. Systems. 2022; 10(2):29. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10020029

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cabrera, Derek, Laura Cabrera, and Elena Cabrera. 2022. "The “Fish Tank” Experiments: Metacognitive Awareness of Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) Significantly Increases Cognitive Complexity" Systems 10, no. 2: 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10020029

APA Style

Cabrera, D., Cabrera, L., & Cabrera, E. (2022). The “Fish Tank” Experiments: Metacognitive Awareness of Distinctions, Systems, Relationships, and Perspectives (DSRP) Significantly Increases Cognitive Complexity. Systems, 10(2), 29. https://doi.org/10.3390/systems10020029

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop