Next Article in Journal
The Role of Aquaculture in Shaping the Morphology of Babylonia areolata: A Comparative Study of Cultured and Wild Populations
Previous Article in Journal
Bird Richness and Abundance in Urban Areas: Simulation-Based Conservation Strategies for an Italian Town
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Exploring Brain Size Asymmetry and Its Relationship with Predation Risk Among Chinese Anurans

1
Key Laboratory of Southwest China Wildlife Resources Conservation (Ministry of Education), China West Normal University, Nanchong 637009, China
2
School of Ecology and Nature Conservation, Beijing Forestry University, Beijing 100083, China
3
Key Laboratory of Artificial Propagation and Utilization in Anurans of Nanchong City, China West Normal University, Nanchong 637009, China
4
College of Panda, China West Normal University, Nanchong 637009, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biology 2025, 14(1), 38; https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14010038
Submission received: 30 September 2024 / Revised: 15 December 2024 / Accepted: 19 December 2024 / Published: 7 January 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Zoology)

Simple Summary

Brain size asymmetry has been implicated in specific cognitive tasks in a broad range of animals. This study aimed to ascertain the asymmetry of brain size among Chinese anurans and the effect of predation risk (i.e., snake density) on the index of brain size asymmetry. The results showed that a significant difference was observed between the sizes of the left and right parts of the brain and brain regions, displaying directional size asymmetry of the total brain and brain regions when one side is consistently larger than the other. For all 99 species of anurans, there was a non-significant difference between the left and right hemisphere size of the whole brain and brain regions.

Abstract

Brain size asymmetry differs considerably across species, including humans, vertebrates, and invertebrates. The subtle structural, functional, or size differences between the two brain sides are associated with processing specific cognitive tasks. To evaluate the differences between the sizes of the left and right sides of the whole brain and brain regions and the effect of predation risk (i.e., snake density) on brain size asymmetry among Chinese anurans, we compared the differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain and brain regions among anuran species and analyzed the correlations between the predation risk and size asymmetry index of the brain and brain regions. We found that when one side of the brain was consistently larger than the other, there was a significant difference between the sizes of the left and right sides of the brain and brain regions, displaying directional asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions. We also found that total brain size was positively correlated with the size asymmetry index of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta when the left hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions were larger than the right ones. Meanwhile, the index of telencephalon size asymmetry was positively correlated with predation risk when the right hemispheres of the brain and brain regions were larger than the left ones. However, there were non-significant differences between the sizes of the left and right sides of the brain and brain regions across 99 species of anurans. Our findings suggest that an increased predation risk linked to sociality is likely to drive an increase in right telencephalon size.

1. Introduction

Biologists have confirmed that a fundamental property of biological systems is their asymmetrical organization during the evolutionary process [1,2]. Symmetry, such as bilateral symmetry, is often associated with harmony and a form of predictable rigidity [3]. The degree of symmetry in bilateral organ structures is used as an indicator of changes in environmental conditions [4]. Bilateral symmetry, also known as “fluctuating asymmetry (FA)”, sometimes reflects developmental stability in organisms [5]. Fluctuating asymmetry is often considered an indicator of the genome’s ability to successfully buffer development and achieve a normal phenotype under stressful environments [6]. Conversely, directional asymmetry is another type of bilateral asymmetry in which one side is consistently larger than the other [7,8] that is also seen as a potential measure of developmental stability [9]. There is evidence that the degree of directional asymmetry in the organ size of animals is associated with developmental stability [10,11,12].
Comparative analyses of brain size evolution have primarily focused on the relationships between brain size and ecological and life history aspects in organisms [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35]. The human brain exhibits a considerable number of morphological and functional left–right differences associated with environmental factors [36,37,38]. For example, the degree of functional lateralization is positively associated with the level of cognitive ability in humans [37,38]. Similarly, in non-human vertebrates and lower animals, normal variation and specialization produce asymmetry in the whole brain and brain structures [39,40,41]. The degree and direction of brain hemispheric asymmetry differ significantly among species [42]. Previous studies of the relationship between brain size and brain asymmetry have confirmed that larger-brained species tend to have more right-lateralized hemispheres in human subjects [43,44].
Brain asymmetry has been conserved or independently acquired during the evolutionary process, suggesting some cognitive advantage. For instance, many regional cortical asymmetries exist in chimpanzees, rhesus macaques, olive baboons, and vervet monkeys [45,46,47,48,49], indicating that brain asymmetry is likely associated with primate-specialized functions, such as communication and tool use [50]. The most dramatic illustration of the effects of environmental factors on brain asymmetry is light stimulation on the asymmetric development of the visual pathway in birds [51,52,53]. In particular, the bird embryos are turned in the egg so that light passing through the shell can only stimulate the right eye in the final days of development. Indeed, eye preference for avoiding predator risk is dependent on the early exposure of embryos to light in zebrafish [54]. Additionally, the effect of environmental temperature on sex hormones can explain gender differences observed in lateralized behaviors in fish [55]. There is evidence that sex hormones modulate seasonal variations in habenular structural asymmetry in frogs [56]. However, it remains unclear whether brain size asymmetry is associated with environmental factors (e.g., predator risk) across anuran species.
Anurans provide an opportunity to examine the relationship between brain size asymmetry and predation risk. Snakes are major predators of anurans in China [57]. Anuran species have developed various morphological and behavioral traits specialized for coping with different levels of predation risk [21,23,58,59]. If specialized behaviors associated with predators show evidence of brain lateralization in anurans, we would expect that detecting predators using a left visual hemifield should show a right hemisphere bias, thus displaying brain size asymmetry. Indeed, there is evidence for left eye–right hemisphere specialization for predator response in three species of toads [22]. However, there is no evidence that right hemispheric specialization for responding to predators is associated with asymmetry in the size of the hemispheres. Neural pathways and subcellular aspects of brain functions may differ between the hemispheres without being manifested as hemispheric differences in brain size. In this study, our aims were to investigate the effects of predator risk on the size asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions among species of Chinese anurans. First, we tested whether there were left–right differences in the sizes of total brain and brain regions among these species. Second, if left–right differences in the sizes of the whole brain and brain regions existed for some species, we tested the relationships between the index of directional asymmetry of the brain and total brain size. Finally, we examined the relationships between the index of size asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions and predator risk among anurans when one side was larger than the other.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Field Samples

A total of 459 individuals from 99 Chinese anuran species were collected from April to July between 2017 and 2020 in western and southwestern China (Figure 1; Table S1). We used a 12 V flashlight to search for all mature individuals along an area with an average length of 1.3 km (range: 0.4–4.6 km) and a width of 5 m and then captured and counted all individuals in their breeding season. Two people walked the transect for 2–3 h during the sampling process. For some transects, we collected all individuals in breeding ponds along the sampling strip transect. A number of individuals of each species were caught in the sampling transect, as shown in Table S1. They were sexed on the basis of the nuptial pad in adult males and eggs readily visible through the skin of the abdomen in females. All individuals were taken to the laboratory and kept individually in rectangular tanks (0.5 m × 0.4 m × 0.4 m). We used benzocaine to anesthetize and euthanize them through single-pithing. We measured the body size (snout–vent length: SVL) of each individual to the nearest 0.01 mm with a caliper and preserved them in 4% phosphate-buffered formalin. After two months, we dissected the brains of all the preserved individuals. The reported experiments comply with the current laws of China concerning animal experimentation, and permission to collect and sacrifice individuals was received from the Ethical Committee for Animal Experiments of China West Normal University (CWNU2019D002).

2.2. Predation Risk

The level of predation risk was determined based on the snake density at breeding sites, focusing on one breeding population per species in the breeding season from April to July between 2017 and 2020. For three consecutive nights around the full moon, we walked along transects with an average length of 1.3 km (range: 0.4–4.6 km) and a width of 5 m for each anuran population with a 12 V flashlight and recorded the number of snakes encountered. Specifically, we used a snake clip to capture the snakes found in the transect for two to three hours and then photographed and identified their classification status (Table S1). Subsequently, we released them to the sites where they were captured. Most of the snake species identified were known predators of anurans, including Cyclophiops major, Trimeresurus stejnegeri, Protobothrops mucrosquamatus, Rhabdophis tigrinus, Lycodon ruhstrati, Ovophis tonkinensis, Dinodon rufozonatum, Elaphe taeniura, and Gloydius strauchii. Nightly searches for a month for one population for each of 10 anuran species showed that searching for three consecutive nights around the full moon was sufficiently representative. There is evidence that the mean snake density is strongly correlated between the two approaches [21]. Therefore, we used the density of snakes for three consecutive nights to estimate the predation risk for other anurans. Finally, we calculated the mean number of snake individuals per km² of transect area as an indicator of predation risk.

2.3. Brain Measurements

All dissections and digital imaging were performed by two individuals (Yu JP and Mai CL). We obtained 459 intact brains of anurans in this study. Measurements were taken blindly by ID number without knowledge of species identity. We selected specific brain regions (olfactory bulbs, telencephalon, and optic tectum) for asymmetry analysis because they demonstrated significant differences between the left and right hemispheres. We obtained the left and right volumes of the whole brain and three main brain regions (i.e., olfactory bulbs, telencephalon, and optic tectum) for 99 anuran species (Table S2). The total brain size was calculated using the sum of the left and right volumes of the whole brain. Digital images of the dorsal, ventral, left, and right sides of the brain were captured using a Motic Images 3.1 digital camera mounted on a Moticam 2006 light microscope (Canon, Beijing, China) at 400× magnification. For dorsal and ventral views, we ensured that the brain was horizontally and symmetrically positioned so that neither hemisphere appeared larger than the other. For paired structures, we measured the length, width, and height of the left and right hemispheres using landmark-based measurements (tpsDig) version 1.37 software (Figure 2). The whole brain and brain regions were defined as the greatest distances enclosed by the landmarks used. Volumetric estimates of the whole brain and brain regions were based on the following ellipsoid model: volume = (L × W × H) × π/(6 × 1.43). Measurements were taken three times on random specimens to assess repeatability. We found very high intra-measurer repeatability within each species in the dataset (all R > 0.96). The potential subtle impacts of microevolution on brain size were minimized by sampling each species from a single locality [20]. The index of asymmetry for the total brain and brain regions was calculated using the following ellipsoid model: asymmetry = (left − right)/0.5 × (left + right) [60,61,62,63,64].

2.4. Phylogeny Reconstruction

To reconstruct the phylogeny of the 99 anuran species, we obtained sequences of six mitochondrial ribosomal genes and three nuclear genes from GenBank. The mitochondrial ribosomal genes included cytochrome b (CYTB), 12S ribosomal RNA (12S), 16S ribosomal RNA (16S), cytochrome oxidase I (COI), NADH dehydrogenase subunit 2 (ND2), and NADH dehydrogenase subunit 4 (ND4). The nuclear genes included recombination activating gene 1 (RAG1), tyrosinase (TYR), and ras homolog family member D (RHOD). We aligned the sequences using the multisequence alignment tool MUSCLE in MEGA v.6.0.6 [65]. The best nucleotide substitution model, which was determined using jModelTest v.2.1.7 [66] based on the Akaike information criterion, was GTR + Γ + I for all genes, except RHOD, for which HKY + Γ + I was favored. GTR + Γ + I was used as the best substitution model for all genes.
To construct the phylogeny, we used unlinked substitution models, a relaxed uncorrelated log-normal clock, and a Yule speciation process with BEAUTi and BEAST v.1.8.3 [67]. Due to the lack of fossil dates for anurans, we omitted time calibration. We then used the BEAST implementation in the CIPRES Science Gateway (http://www.phylo.org) (accessed on 28 June 2024) to run the Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) simulation for 100 million generations, sampling every 10,000th tree. For all tree statistics, the effective sample size values exceed 200 in Tracer v.1.6.0 [68], indicating satisfactory convergence of the Bayesian chain and adequate model mixing. Finally, we used TreeAnnotator v.1.8.3 [67] to generate maximum clade credibility trees with mean node heights and a 10% burn-in (Figure 3).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted on log10-transformed data in R version 4.0.1 [69]. We accounted for the non-independence of data due to shared ancestry using phylogenetic generalized least-squares (PGLS) models [70] based on the phylogeny described. To address varied sample sizes between species that might affect the accuracy of species means, we performed the PGLS analyses using the R package nlme, incorporating Pagel’s [71] phylogenetic correlation structure (corPagel), as implemented in the ape package [72], and weighting the model by the number of individuals measured. The PGLS models can estimate the phylogenetic scaling parameter λ based on maximum likelihood [73]. We assessed the phylogenetic effect by comparing our model with estimated λ to models with λ set to 0 (phylogenetic independence) or 1 (complete phylogenetic dependence) using likelihood ratio tests [70]. p-values for these tests were reported as superscripts following the λ values.
We first tested differences in the sizes of the left and right parts of the whole brain and brain morphological regions using a phylogenetic t-test when one side was larger than the other. Next, we built PGLS models, treating the index of asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions as the dependent variable and whole brain size as the independent variable. This approach tested the prediction of the Ringo hypothesis that there were significant associations between the index of brain size asymmetry and the total brain size while controlling for the potential confounding effect of body size. In addition, we analyzed the associations between the index of brain size asymmetry and predation risk. Finally, we examined the differences between the left and right parts of the whole brain and brain regions at intraspecific or inter-specific levels for 99 species. All numeric variables were standardized before analysis to make parameter estimates comparable, and model assumptions were checked and met. Sample sizes varied between models because not all variables were available for all species.

3. Results

We first analyzed differences in the size of the whole brain among 99 species when controlling for the SVL effect and found significant differences in the relative size of the whole brain among species of anurans (ANCOVA: F99,458 = 4.242, p < 0.001). We then analyzed differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain and brain regions in species in which the left hemisphere was larger than the right one. We found a significant difference between the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions (Table 1). A PGLS analysis revealed that the index of size asymmetry of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta was positively correlated with total brain size after accounting for the effect of SVL (Table 2; Figure 4). Inconsistent with the prediction of the Ringo hypothesis, the asymmetry index of the total brain size tended to be correlated with the total brain size. Conversely, we observed significant differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain and brain regions in species in which the right hemisphere was larger than the left one (Table 3). Meanwhile, there were no correlations between the index of size asymmetry of the total brain and the three brain regions and the total brain size (Table S3). Additionally, we found no significant correlations between the index of asymmetry of the total brain and the brain regions and predation risk when the left hemisphere was larger than the right one (Table S4). By contrast, there was a positive correlation between the index of asymmetry of telencephalon size and the predation risk in species in which the right hemisphere was larger than the left one (Table 4; Figure 5).
We combined data from all 99 anuran species and analyzed differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain and brain regions. We found non-significant differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain (Table S5). Additionally, the sizes of three main brain regions (i.e., olfactory bulbs, telencephalon, and optic tectum) showed non-significant differences between the left and right hemispheres (Table S5).
Due to the non-significant left–right differences in the whole brain and brain regions across all 99 species, we decided not to test the relationships between the degree of asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions and brain size for all species, as well as the relationships between the index of size asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions and the predation risk.

4. Discussion

We first examined the differences between the left and right hemisphere sizes of the whole brain and brain regions and assessed the associations between the index of asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions with the total brain size or predation risk across anuran species. We found positive correlations between the index of size asymmetry of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta and the total brain size in species in which the left brain hemisphere was larger than the right one. We also found a positive correlation between the index of asymmetry of the telencephalon size and predation risk when the right hemisphere was larger than the left one. Our study confirmed non-significant differences between the sizes of the left and right sides of the whole brain and three main brain regions among 99 anuran species.
Most biological systems exhibit varied degrees of asymmetry in the size of morphological organs [74,75,76]. This morphological asymmetry in animals includes both directional and fluctuating asymmetry, which reflects developmental stability under different environmental conditions [6,7,8,77,78,79,80,81]. In humans and many other animals, the two brain hemispheres show significant differences in anatomy and function. Although previous studies on frog brain asymmetry have suggested that sex hormones may influence habenular structural asymmetry [56], no systematic research addresses the relationships between anuran brain asymmetry and environmental factors. In this study, we first observed that anuran species displayed directional asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions, with one side of the brain being consistently larger than the other, reflecting developmental stability. However, when combining data from all 99 species to analyze the differences between the left and right hemispheres, we found non-significant differences between the sizes of the left and right sides of the whole brain and three main brain regions among anuran species.
The Ringo hypothesis predicts that larger-brained species exhibit more pronounced laterality compared to smaller-brained species [80]. Interhemispheric conduction delay, which depends on the length of the fiber tracts connecting the two brain hemispheres, is a key factor shaping the evolution of hemispheric asymmetry in mammals [42]. Consequently, longer fiber tracts lead to greater interhemispheric conduction delays, requiring faster reactions to environmental demands and influencing evolutionary pressures controlled by unilateral neural networks. Hence, larger-brained species tend to exhibit more right-lateralized individuals [43,44]. However, inconsistent with the prediction of the Ringo hypothesis [80], the evidence of size asymmetry in the small brains of insects shows that brain size is not an important factor in shaping the evolution of brain size asymmetry [81,82,83]. We found that larger-brained species did not display a larger index of asymmetry in the whole brain compared to smaller-brained species when the left brain hemisphere was larger than the right one. This pattern suggests that the evolution of brain size asymmetry is not influenced by total brain size. However, we found a higher index of size asymmetry of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta in larger-brained species, suggesting that an interhemispheric conduction delay in total brain size might drive the evolution of hemispheric asymmetry of the two brain regions [80].
The lateralized specialization of brain size is believed to originate from a combination of developmental, evolutionary, genetic, and environmental factors [47,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102]. In particular, the way the preferential use of the left and right eyes affects visual discrimination learning and detour behavior in varied vertebrates, including chicks, fish, anurans, and sheep [85]. Similarly, ultrasonic calls emitted by young mice, which are designed to elicit maternal care, are preferentially processed by the left hemisphere of the mother’s brain [103]. The left hemisphere has also been implicated in generating or perceiving calls in primates and mice, suggesting a conserved role in individual communication [102]. Meanwhile, brain lateralization is associated with learning and memory retrieval, such as the differentiation between short- and long-term memory [104,105,106]. For instance, honeybees favor the right antenna for short-term memory recall, while the left antenna is preferred for long-term memory [106]. Similarly, mice with impaired hippocampal asymmetry exhibit decreased performance in spatial learning and working memory [104]. Moreover, brain lateralization also influences stimuli perception and motor responses [106]. For example, toads are more likely to attack conspecifics on their left side and prey on their right side [107], while poeciliid fish show a consistent rightward turning bias when navigating an opaque barrier but a leftward bias when evading a simulated predator [54]. In our study, 56 species with left-brain lateralization appear to be related to stimuli perception, and future behavioral experiments need to confirm this consistent conclusion.
There is evidence that the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions generally develop with a high degree of symmetry at both the anatomical and functional levels in most animals [1,2,3]. However, subtle structural differences between the two sides can influence the processing of specific cognitive tasks [92]. Hemispheric specialization for particular cognitive functions likely reflects variations in the neural circuits of each hemisphere [3]. For example, light-induced visual lateralization has been shown to enhance chicks’ performance in dual tasks involving predator monitoring [108]. Similarly, the bias in front paw use is inversely correlated with the side of the face displaying supernumerary whiskers in mice, a phenomenon that has been suggested to result from competition for cortical space between motor and somatosensory areas [1]. Our findings for the directional size asymmetry of the whole brain and brain regions across anuran species suggest that specific cognitive tasks are closely tied to the neural circuits of each brain hemisphere. Similar findings have been reported for asymmetry in amphibia [109,110,111].
Although interhemispheric conduction delay is an important factor in shaping the evolution of brain hemispheric asymmetry, environmental factors must also be considered in this context. Previous studies have demonstrated that sociality plays a crucial role in both the evolution of brain size [112] and population-level brain hemispheric asymmetry [112,113,114,115]. Consequently, inter-individual interactions produce evolutionarily stable strategies of lateralization, depending on environmental conditions [116], which is supported by empirical evidence from insects [117] and fish [118]. For instance, social honeybees display brain hemispheric asymmetry on both behavioral and electrophysiological levels, while non-social bees do not [117]. For anuran species, sociality linked to anti-predator risk cannot play an important role in shaping the evolution of brain hemispheric asymmetry in breeding season [20,119,120]. In addition, the digit ratio, which is an indicator of brain laterality, is associated with behaviors in the gecko Ptyodactylus guttatus [8]. Indeed, we found that there were non-significant differences between the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions across 99 species. However, we found that the index of asymmetry of telencephalon size was correlated positively with predation risk in species in which the right hemisphere was larger than the left one, suggesting that an increasing predation risk linked to sociality possibly promoted the enlarged right telencephalon.

5. Conclusions

This study indicated that anurans exhibit significant differences between the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions across various anuran species when one side of the brain is larger than the other. Meanwhile, the index of size asymmetry of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta is positively correlated with total brain size in species in which the left brain hemisphere is larger than the right one, suggesting that interhemispheric conduction delays shaped the evolution of the hemispheric asymmetry of the olfactory bulb and optic tecta. Moreover, the positive and significant correlation between the index of asymmetry of the telencephalon size and predation risk in species in which the right brain hemisphere was larger than the left one suggests that the increased predation risk promoted the increased right telencephalon.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biology14010038/s1, Table S1: Species, line length (m), line wide (m), line square (m2) and snake species (individuals) in anuran species. Table S2: Species, body size, size of left and right brain hemispheres of total brain and three main brain regions and predation risk in anuran species; Table S3: the relationships between asymmetry index of total brain and three brain regions and the total brain size in species where the left hemisphere is smaller than the right one; Table S4: the relationships between the asymmetry index of different brain regions and predation risk in species where the left hemisphere is larger than the right hemisphere; Table S5: Difference in sizes of the left and right hemisphere sizes in total brain and three brain regions among 99 anuran species.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.L.; methodology, L.C. and C.C.; software, C.C. and Y.J.; validation, C.C. and Y.J.; formal analysis, Y.W.; data curation, C.C. and Y.W.; writing—original draft preparation, C.C. and W.L.; writing—review and editing, W.L.; visualization, L.C. and Y.J.; supervision, W.L.; funding acquisition, W.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32370456) and the Key Project of Natural Sciences Foundation of Sichuan Province (22NSFSC0011).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Chine West Normal University (CWNU2019D002).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank L. Jin, C. Mai, J. Yu, X. Zhu, and Y. Yang for their assistance in data collection during the experiment.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Barneoud, P.; der Loos, H.V. Direction of handedness linked to hereditary asymmetry of a sensory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1993, 90, 3246–3250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G.; Andrew, R.J. Divided Brains: The Biology and Behaviour of Brain Asymmetry; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Duboc, V.; Dufourcq, P.; Blader, P.; Roussigné, M. Asymmetry of the brain: Development and implications. Annu. Rev. Genet. 2015, 49, 647–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Møller, A.P.; Swaddle, J.P. Asymmetry, Developmental Stability and Evolution; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  5. Polak, M.; Triners, R. The science of symmetry in biology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1994, 9, 122–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Waddington, C.H. Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature 1942, 150, 563–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Van Valen, L. A study of fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution 1962, 16, 125–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Sion, G.; Tal, R.; Meiri, S. Asymmetric behavior in Ptyodactylus guttatus: Can a digit ratio reflect brain laterality? Symmetry 2020, 12, 1490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Graham, J.H.; Emlen, J.M.; Freeman, D.C.; Leamy, L.J.; Kieser, J.A. Directional asymmetry and the measurement of developmental instability. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 1998, 64, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Graham, J.H.; Roe, K.E.; West, T.B. Effects of lead and benzene on the developmental stability of Drosophila melanogaster. Ecotoxicology 1993, 2, 185–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Palmer, A.R. From symmetry to asymmetry: Phylogenetic patterns of asymmetry variation in animals and their evolutionary significance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1996, 93, 14279–14286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Hettyey, A.; Laurila, A.; Herczeg, G.; Jonsson, K.I.; Kovacs, T.; Merilä, J. Does testis weight dechne towards the Subarctic? A case study on the common frog Rana temporaria. Naturwissenschaften 2005, 92, 188–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Kotrschal, K.; Van Staaden, M.J.; Huber, R. Fish brains: Evolution and environmental relationships. Rev. Fish Biol. Fish. 1998, 8, 373–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Marino, L. A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans and anthropoid primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 1998, 51, 230–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Day, L.B.; Westcott, D.A.; Olster, D.H. Evolution of bower complexity and cerebellum size in bowerbirds. Brain Behav. Evol. 2005, 66, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Aviles, J.M.; Garamszegi, L.Z. Egg rejection and brain size among potential hosts of the common cuckoo. Ethology 2007, 113, 562–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gonda, A.; Herczeg, G.; Merilä, J. Evolutionary ecology of intraspecific brain size variation: A review. Ecol. Evol. 2013, 3, 2751–2764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. van Woerden, J.T.; Willems, E.P.; van Schaik, C.P.; Isler, K. Large brains buffer energetic effects of seasonal habitats in catarrhine primates. Evolution 2011, 66, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. De Schotten, M.T.; Dell’Acqua, F.; Forkel, S.J.; Simmons, A.; Francesco, V. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 1245–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Liao, W.B.; Jiang, Y.; Jin, L.; Lüpold, S. How hibernation in frogs drives brain and reproductive evolution in opposing directions. eLife 2023, 12, RP88236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Liao, W.B.; Jiang, Y.; Li, D.Y.; Jin, L.; Zhong, M.J.; Qi, Y.; Lüpold, S.; Kotrschal, A. Cognition contra camouflage: How the brain mediates predator-driven crypsis evolution. Sci. Adv. 2022, 8, eabq1878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Jiang, Y.; Yuan, L.J.; Liao, W.B. Chemical defense mediates population-driven on anuran brain size evolution. Sci. China Life Sci. 2024, 67, 1076–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Jiang, Y.; Luan, X.F.; Liao, W.B. Anuran brain size predicts food availability-driven population density. Sci. China Life Sci. 2023, 66, 415–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jiang, Y.; Chen, C.; Liao, W.B. Anuran interorbital space variation: The role of ecological and behavioral factors. Integr. Zool. 2022, 17, 777–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Liu, Y.T.; Wu, Z.J.; Liao, W.B. Large-brained birds display lower extra-pair paternity. Integr. Zool. 2023, 18, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Yu, X.; Zhong, M.J.; Li, D.Y.; Jin, L.; Liao, W.B.; Kotrschal, A. Large-brained frogs mature later and live longer. Evolution 2018, 72, 1174–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Huang, Y.; Mai, C.L.; Liao, W.B.; Kotrschal, A. Body mass variation is negatively associated with brain size: Evidence for the fat-brain trade-off in anurans. Evolution 2020, 74, 1551–1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Mai, C.L.; Liao, W.B.; Kotrschal, A.; Lüpold, S. Relative brain size is predicted by the intensity of intrasexual competition in frogs. Am. Nat. 2020, 196, 169–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Bialas, J.T.; Dylewski, L.; Tobolka, M. Brain size mediates the choice of breeding strategy in the red-backed shrike Lanius collurio. Integr. Zool. 2024, 19, 683–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Yatsuk, A.A.; Triseleva, T.A.; Narchuk, E.P.; Matyukhin, A.V.; Safonkin, A.F. Morphology of the wings and attachment apparatus in the evolution of the family Hippoboscidae (Diptera). Integr. Zool. 2023, 19, 941–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Taylor, P.J.; Nengovhela, A.; Denys, C.; Scott, G.R.; Ivy, C.M. Adaptation in brain structure and respiratory and olfactory structures across environmental gradients in African and North American muroid rodents. Integr. Zool. 2024, 19, 165–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Yang, C.C.; Cai, Y. The importance of brain size for the evolution of nest complexity in birds. Integr. Zool. 2023, 18, 1105–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Chen, C.; Shao, W.J.; Zhu, X.; Yang, Y.J.; Jiang, Y.; Liao, W.B. Brain size predicts foraging and escaping abilities in the paddy frogs. Integr. Zool. 2023, 18, 958–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Li, S.B.; Liu, Y.X.; Du, X.L.; Li, G.P.; Liao, W.B. Nest complexity correlates with larger brain size but smaller body mass across bird species. Integr. Zool. 2024, 19, 496–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jin, L.; Jiang, Y.; Han, L.X.; Luan, X.F.; Liu, X.; Liao, W.B. Big-brained alien birds tend to occur climatic niche shifts through enhanced behavioral innovation. Integr. Zool. 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Corballis, M.C. How many lateralities? Laterality 2021, 26, 307–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ocklenburg, S.; Güntürkün, O. The Lateralized Brain: The Neuroscience and Evolution of Hemispheric Asymmetries; Academic Press: Amsterdam, The Netherland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  38. McManus, C. Half a century of handedness research: Myths, truths; fictions, facts; backwards, but mostly forwards. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2019, 3, 2398212818820513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Knebel, D.; Rigosi, E. Temporal and structural neural asymmetries in insects. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2021, 48, 72–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Miletto-Petrazzini, M.E.; Sovrano, V.A.; Vallortigara, G.; Messina, A. Brain and behavioral asymmetry: A lesson from fish. Front. Neuroanat. 2020, 14, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G. Brain and behavioural asymmetries in nonhuman species. Laterality 2021, 26, 4–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Güntürkün, O.; Ströckens, F.; Ocklenburg, S. Brain lateralization: A comparative perspective. Physiol. Rev. 2020, 100, 1019–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Thiebaut de Schotten, M.; Beckmann, C.F. Asymmetry of brain structure and function: 40 years after Sperry’s Nobel Prize. Brain Struct. Funct. 2022, 227, 421–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Yang, L.; Zhao, C.X.; Xiong, Y.R.; Zhong, S.Y.; Wu, D.; Peng, S.L.; Thiebaut de Schotten, M.; Gong, G.L. Callosal fiber length scales with brain size according to functional lateralization, evolution, and development. J. Neurosci. 2022, 42, 3599–3610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Bishop, D.V.M. What does lack of language lateralization signify? Evidence of fluctuating asymmetry rather than hemispheric equipoise on non-lateralized tasks. Roy. Soc. Open Sci. 2024, 11, 240495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Xia, J.; Wang, F.; Wu, Z.; Wang, L.; Zhang, C.; Shen, D.; Li, G. Mapping hemispheric asymmetries of the macaque cerebral cortex during early brain development. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019, 41, 95–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Becker, Y.; Claidière, N.; Margiotoudi, K.; Marie, D.; Roth, M.; Nazarian, B.; Anton, J.L.; Coulon, O.; Meguerditchian, A. Broca’s cerebral asymmetry refects gestural communication’s lateralisation in monkeys (Papio anubis). eLife 2022, 11, e70521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Marie, D.; Roth, M.; Lacoste, R.; Nazarian, B.; Bertello, A.; Anton, J.L.; Hopkins, W.D.; Margiotoudi, K.; Love, S.A.; Meguerditchian, A. Left brain asymmetry of the planum temporale in a nonhominid primate: Redefning the origin of brain specialization for language. Cereb. Cortex 2017, 28, 1808–1815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Gómez-Robles, A.; Hopkins, W.D.; Schapiro, S.J.; Sherwood, C.C. The heritability of chimpanzee and human brain asymmetry. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2016, 283, 20161319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Becker, Y.; Meguerditchian, A. Structural brain asymmetries for language: A comparative approach across primates. Symmetry 2022, 14, 876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Rogers, L.J.; Sink, H.S. Transient asymmetry in the projections of the rostral thalamus to the visual hyperstriatum of the chicken, and reversal of its direction by light exposure. Exp. Brain Res. 1988, 70, 378–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Rogers, L.J. Light experience and asymmetry of brain function in chickens. Nature 1982, 297, 223–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Manns, M.; Gunturkun, O. Monocular deprivation alters the direction of functional and morphological asymmetries in the pigeon’s (Columba livia) visual system. Behav. Neurosci. 1999, 113, 1257–1266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Bisazza, A.; Facchin, L.; Pignatti, R.; Vallortigara, G. Lateralization of detour behaviour in poeciliid fish: The effect of species, gender and sexual motivation. Behav. Brain Res. 1998, 91, 157–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Budaev, S.; Andrew, R.J. Patterns of early embryonic light exposure determine behavioural asymmetries in zebrafish: A habenular hypothesis. Behav. Brain Res. 2009, 200, 91–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Kemali, M.; Guglielmotti, V.; Fiorino, L. The asymmetry of the habenular nuclei of female and male frogs in spring and in winter. Brain Res. 1990, 517, 251–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Zamora-camacho, F.J. Sex and habitat differences in size and coloration of an amphibian’s poison glands match differential predator pressures. Integr. Zool. 2022, 17, 764–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Peng, Z.W.; Zhang, L.X.; Lu, X. Global gaps in age data based on skeletochronology for amphibians. Integr. Zool. 2022, 17, 752–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Lippolis, G.; Bisazza, A.; Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G. Lateralisation of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality 2002, 7, 163–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Steinmetz, H. Structure, functional and cerebral asymmetry: In vivo morphometry of the planum temporale. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1996, 20, 587–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sion, G. Foot-preference underlies bite-scar asymmetry in the gecko Ptyodactylus guttatus. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2017, 23, 129–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Jennings, D.J.; Gammell, M.P. Fluctuating asymmetry of fallow deer antlers is associated with weapon damage but not tactical behaviour during fights. Symmetry 2023, 15, 829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Versace, E.; Vallortigara, G. Forelimb preferences in human beings and other species: Multiple models for testing hypotheses on lateralization. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Werner, Y.L.; Rothenstein, D.; Sivan, N. Directional asymmetry in reptiles (Sauria: Gekkonidae: Ptyodactylus) and its possible role, with implications for biometrical methodology. J. Zool. 1991, 225, 647–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Tamura, K.; Stecher, G.; Peterson, D.; Filipski, A.; Kumar, S. MEGA6: Molecular evolutionary genetics analysis version 6.0. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2013, 30, 2725–2729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Darriba, D.; Taboada, G.L.; Doallo, R.; Posada, D. jModelTest 2: More models, new heuristics and parallel computing. Nat. Methods 2012, 9, 772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Drummond, A.J.; Suchard, M.A.; Xie, D.; Rambaut, A. Bayesian phylogenetics with BEAUti and the BEAST 1.7. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2012, 29, 1969–1973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Rambaut, A.; Drummond, A. Tracer v1.6. 2014. Available online: http://beast.bio.ed.ac.uk/software/tracer/ (accessed on 28 June 2024).
  69. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Development Core Team: Vienna, Austria, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  70. Freckleton, R.P.; Harvey, P.H.; Pagel, M. Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. Am. Nat. 2002, 160, 712–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Pagel, M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 1999, 401, 877–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Paradis, E. Analysis of Phylogenetics and Evolution with R; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  73. Pagel, M. Detecting correlated evolution on phylogenies: A general method for the comparative analysis of discrete characters. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1994, 255, 37–45. [Google Scholar]
  74. Geschwind, N.; Galaburda, A.M. Cerebral lateralization. Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology. Arch. Neurol. 1985, 42, 428–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Calfayan, L.M.; Cavia, R.; Fraschina, J.; Guidobono, J.S.; Gorosito, I.L.; Busch, M. Environmental drivers of long-term variations in the abundance of the red hocicudo mouse (Oxymycterus rufus) in Pampas agroecosystems. Integr. Zool. 2024, 19, 37–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Massei, G.; Jacob, J.; Hinds, L.A. Developing fertility control for rodents: A framework for researchers and practitioners. Integr. Zool. 2024, 19, 87–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Zhou, Q.X.; Song, C.; Wang, P.; Zhao, Z.; Li, Y.; Zhan, S. Generating dual active species by triple-atom-sites through peroxymonosulfate activation for treating micropollutants in complex water. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2023, 120, e2300085120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Zhou, Q.X.; Xue, W.; Cui, X.; Wang, P.; Zuo, S.; Mo, F.; Li, C.; Liu, G.; Ouyang, S.; Zhan, S.; et al. Oxygen bridging Fe, Co dual-metal dimers boost reversible oxygen electrocatalysis for rechargeable Zn-air batteries. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2024, 121, e2404013121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zhan, H.; Zhou, R.; Wang, P.; Zhou, Q.X. Selective hydroxyl generation for efficient pollutant degradation by electronic structure modulation at Fe sites. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2023, 120, e2305378120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ringo, J.L.; Doty, R.W.; Demeter, S.; Simard, P.Y. Time is of the essence: A conjecture that hemispheric specialization arises from interhemispheric conduction delay. Cereb. Cortex 1994, 4, 331–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Cante-Bazán, E.; Luría-Manzano, R. Seasonal variation of the relationship between body condition and fluctuating asymmetry in two sympatric ranid frogs. Herpetol. J. 2024, 34, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Frasnelli, E.; Vallortigara, G.; Rogers, L.J. Left–right asymmetries of behaviour and nervous system in invertebrates. Neurosci. Bio-Behav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1273–1291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Vallortigara, G.; Rogers, L.J. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: Advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 575–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Bryant, K.L.; Preuss, T.M. A comparative perspective on the human temporal lobe. In Digital Endocasts; Bruner, E., Ogihara, N., Tanabe, H., Eds.; Springer: Tokyo, Japan, 2018; pp. 239–258. [Google Scholar]
  85. Vallortigara, G. Comparative neuropsychology of the dual brain: A stroll through animals’ left and right perceptual worlds. Brain Lang. 2000, 73, 189–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Toga, A.W.; Thompson, P.M. Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat. Rev. 2003, 4, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Roe, J.M.; Vidal-Pineiro, D.; Amlien, I.K.; Pan, M.Y.; Sneve, M.H.; de Schotten, M.T.; Friedrich, P.; Sha, Z.Q.; Francks, C.; Eilertsen, E.M.; et al. Tracing the development and lifespan change of population-level structural asymmetry in the cerebral cortex. eLife 2023, 12, e84685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Chen, Y.; Arnatkeviciute, A.; McTavish, E.; Pang, J.C.; Chopra, S.; Suo, C.; Fornito, A.; Aquino, K.M.; Initiative, A.D.N. The individuality of shape asymmetries of the human cerebral cortex. eLife 2022, 11, e75056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Wan, B.; Bayrak, S.; Xu, T.; Schaare, H.L.; Bethlehem, R.A.I.; Bernhardt, B.C.; Valk, S.L. Heritability and cross-species comparisons of human cortical functional organization asymmetry. eLife 2022, 11, e77215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Lopez-Persem, A.; Roumazeilles, L.; Folloni, D.; Marche, K.; Fouragnan, E.F.; Khalighinejad, N.; Rushworth, M.F.; Sallet, J. Diferential functional connectivity underlying asymmetric reward-related activity in human and nonhuman primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 28452–28462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Jepma, M.; Roy, M.; Ramlakhan, K.; van Velzen, M.; Dahan, A. Different brain systems support learning from received and avoided pain during human pain-avoidance learning. eLife 2022, 11, e74149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Parkes, L.; Kim, J.Z.; Stiso, J.; Calkins, M.E.; Cieslak, M.; Gur, R.E.; Gur, R.C.; Moore, T.M.; Ouellet, M.; Roalf, D.R.; et al. Asymmetric signaling across the hierarchy of cytoarchitecture within the human connectome. Sci. Adv. 2022, 8, eadd2185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Zhao, Y.; Hong, Y.Y.; Li, Y.B.; Qi, F.J.; Qing, H.T.; Su, H.; Yin, J. Physically intelligent autonomous soft robotic maze escaper. Sci. Adv. 2023, 9, eadi3254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Merrick, C.M.; Dixon, T.C.; Breska, A.; Lin, J.; Chang, E.F.; King-Stephens, D.; Laxer, K.D.; Weber, P.B.; Carmena, J.; Knight, R.T.; et al. Left hemisphere dominance for bilateral kinematic encoding in the human brain. eLife 2022, 11, e69977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Berretz, G.; Packheiser, J.; Wolf, O.T.; Ocklenburg, S. Acute stress increases left hemispheric activity measured via changes in frontal alpha asymmetries. iScience 2022, 25, 103841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Costalunga, G.; Kobylkov, D.; Rosa-Salva, O.; Morandi-Raikova, A.; Vallortigara, G.; Mayer, U. Responses in the left and right entopallium are differently affected by light stimulation in embryo. iScience 2024, 27, 109268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Pedigo, B.D.; Powell, M.; Bridgeford, E.W.; Winding, M.; Priebe, C.E.; Vogelstein, J.T.; Ostojic, S. Generative network modeling reveals quantitative definitions of bilateral symmetry exhibited by a whole insect brain connectome. eLife 2023, 12, e83739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Veréb, D.; Mijalkov, M.; Canal-Garcia, A.; Chang, Y.W.; Gomez-Ruiz, E.; Gerboles, B.Z.; Kivipelto, M.; Svenningsson, P.; Zetterberg, H.; Volpe, G.; et al. Age-related differences in the functional topography of the locus coeruleus and their implications for cognitive and affective functions. eLife 2023, 12, RP87188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Ringers, C.; Bialonski, S.; Ege, M.; Solovev, A.; Hansen, J.N.; Jeong, I.; Friedrich, B.M.; Jurisch-Yaksi, N.; Goldstein, R.E. Novel analytical tools reveal that local synchronization of cilia coincides with tissue-scale metachronal waves in zebrafish multiciliated epithelia. eLife 2023, 12, e77701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Neubauer, S.; Gunz, P.; Scott, N.A.; Hublin, J.J.; Mitteroecker, P. Evolution of brain lateralization: A shared hominid pattern of endocranial asymmetry is much more variable in humans than in great apes. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaax9935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Peng, X.L.; Liu, Q.; Hubbard, C.S.; Wang, D.H.; Zhu, W.Z.; Fox, M.D.; Liu, H.S. Robust dynamic brain coactivation states estimated in individuals. Sci. Adv. 2023, 9, eabq8566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Ocklenburg, S.; Strockens, F.; Gunturkun, O. Lateralisation of conspecifific vocalisation in non-human vertebrates. Laterality 2013, 18, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Goto, K.; Kurashima, R.; Gokan, H.; Inoue, N.; Ito, I.; Watanabe, S. Left-right asymmetry defect in the hippocampal circuitry impairs spatial learning and working memory in IV mice. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e15468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Pascual, A.; Huang, K.L.; Neveu, J.; Preat, T. Neuroanatomy: Brain asymmetry and long-term memory. Nature 2004, 427, 605–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G. From antenna to antenna: Lateral shift of olfactory memory recall by honeybees. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e2340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Rogers, L.J. Hand and paw preferences in relation to the lateralized brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 943–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Vallortigara, G.; Rogers, L.J.; Bisazza, A.; Lippolis, G.; Robins, A. Complementary right and left hemififield use for predatory and agonistic behaviour in toads. Neuroreport 1998, 9, 3341–3344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Rogers, L.J.; Zucca, P.; Vallortigara, G. Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 271, S420–S422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Robins, A.; Rogers, L.J. Lateralized prey-catching responses in the cane toad, Bufo marinus: Analysis of complex visual stimuli. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 567–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Robins, A.; Rogers, L.J. Lateralized motor behaviour in the righting responses of the cane toad (Rhinella marina). Laterality 2022, 27, 129–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Robins, A.; Lippolis, G.; Bisazza, A.; Vallortigara, G.; Rogers, L.J. Lateralized agonistic responses and hindlimb use in toads. Anim. Behav. 1998, 56, 875–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Powell, J.L.; Lewis, P.A.; Dunbar, R.I.M.; García-Fiñana, M.; Roberts, N. Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 3554–3562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Rogers, L.J.; Rigosi, E.; Frasnelli, E.; Vallortigara, G. A right antenna for social behaviour in honeybees. Sci. Rep. 2013, 3, 2045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Ghirlanda, S.; Frasnelli, E.; Vallortigara, G. Intraspecific competition and coordination in the evolution of lateralization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 861–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Ghirlanda, S.; Vallortigara, G. The evolution of brain lateralization: Game theoretical analysis of population structure. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 271, 853–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Frasnelli, E.; Vallortigara, G. Individual-level and population-level lateralization: Two sides of the same coin. Symmetry 2018, 10, 739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Anfora, G.; Frasnelli, E.; Maccagnani, B.; Rogers, L.J.; Vallortigara, G. Behavioural and electrophysiological lateralization in a social (Apis mellifera) but not in a non-social (Osmia cornuta) species of bee. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 206, 236–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Bisazza, A.; Cantalupo, C.; Capocchiano, M.; Vallortigara, G. Population lateralisation and social behaviour: A study with 16 species of fish. Laterality 2000, 5, 269–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Zhao, L.F.; Qin, Y.Y.; Jin, Y.J.; Wang, J.C.; Liang, W. Snake-like bird hisses induce anti-predator responses in a frog. BMC Ecol. Evol. 2024, 24, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Liao, W.B. Adaptation in Brain Size in Anurans; Science Press: Beijing, China, 2024. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Topographic map showing the locations of the 99 species of anurans in China.
Figure 1. Topographic map showing the locations of the 99 species of anurans in China.
Biology 14 00038 g001
Figure 2. Ventral (A), right lateral (B), and left lateral (C) views of brain regions in an anuran species. Length, width, and height measures for each of the three main brain regions (olfactory bulb, telencephalon, and optic tectum) are shown.
Figure 2. Ventral (A), right lateral (B), and left lateral (C) views of brain regions in an anuran species. Length, width, and height measures for each of the three main brain regions (olfactory bulb, telencephalon, and optic tectum) are shown.
Biology 14 00038 g002
Figure 3. Phylogenetic tree of the 99 anurans species based on the three nuclear genes (RAG1, RHOD, and TYR) and the three mitochondrial genes (CYTB, 12S, and 16S), using TreeAnnotator v.1.8.3 in the comparative analysis.
Figure 3. Phylogenetic tree of the 99 anurans species based on the three nuclear genes (RAG1, RHOD, and TYR) and the three mitochondrial genes (CYTB, 12S, and 16S), using TreeAnnotator v.1.8.3 in the comparative analysis.
Biology 14 00038 g003
Figure 4. Relationships between asymmetry index of olfactory bulb (A) and optic tecta (B) and total brain size in species in which the left hemisphere was larger than the right one.
Figure 4. Relationships between asymmetry index of olfactory bulb (A) and optic tecta (B) and total brain size in species in which the left hemisphere was larger than the right one.
Biology 14 00038 g004
Figure 5. Relationship between the asymmetry index of telencephalon and predation risk in species in which the left hemisphere is smaller than the right one.
Figure 5. Relationship between the asymmetry index of telencephalon and predation risk in species in which the left hemisphere is smaller than the right one.
Biology 14 00038 g005
Table 1. Differences in sizes of the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions (mean ± SD) in anuran species in which the left hemisphere was larger than the right one using phylogenetic paired t-test.
Table 1. Differences in sizes of the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions (mean ± SD) in anuran species in which the left hemisphere was larger than the right one using phylogenetic paired t-test.
Brain RegionsLeft Hemisphere (mm3)Right Hemisphere (mm3)tdfp
Olfactory bulb2.745 ± 0.3872.692 ± 0.39310.63654<0.001
Telencephalon3.678 ± 0.3383.641 ± 0.3488.80253<0.001
Optic tecta3.500 ± 0.3953.467 ± 0.4052.202220.038
Total brain4.351 ± 0.3624.315 ± 0.36810.28056<0.001
Table 2. PGLS models assessing the relationships between asymmetry index of the total brain and three main brain regions among anuran species in which the left hemisphere is larger than the right one. Snout–vent length (SVL) was added as a covariate. Phylogenetic scaling parameters (superscripts following λ denote p-values of likelihood ratio tests against models with λ = 0 and 1, respectively).
Table 2. PGLS models assessing the relationships between asymmetry index of the total brain and three main brain regions among anuran species in which the left hemisphere is larger than the right one. Snout–vent length (SVL) was added as a covariate. Phylogenetic scaling parameters (superscripts following λ denote p-values of likelihood ratio tests against models with λ = 0 and 1, respectively).
Dependent VariableBrain RegionsIndependent VariableλR2βtp
Asymmetry indexOlfactory bulbTotal brain<0.001(<0.001, 0.915)0.0900.0382.3490.022
SVL −0.054−1.6360.108
TelencephalonTotal size<0.0001(<0.001, 0.348)0.1280.0191.8010.077
SVL −0.017−0.8300.410
Optic tectaTotal brain0.460(<0.001, 0.952)0.1400.0352.3780.027
SVL −0.059−1.9750.061
Total brainTotal size0.036(<0.001, 0.396)0.1050.0131.9090.061
SVL −0.013−0.9440.349
Table 3. Results of phylogenetic paired t-tests for the sizes of the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions (mean ± SD) in anuran species in which the left hemisphere was smaller than the right one.
Table 3. Results of phylogenetic paired t-tests for the sizes of the left and right hemispheres of the whole brain and brain regions (mean ± SD) in anuran species in which the left hemisphere was smaller than the right one.
Brain RegionsLeft Hemisphere (mm3)Right Hemisphere (mm3)tdfp
Olfactory bulb2.684 ± 0.4162.739 ± 0.405−5.55639<0.001
Telencephalon3.759 ± 0.2943.796 ± 0.292−2.517400.016
Optic tecta3.323 ± 0.3433.373 ± 0.339−6.32271<0.001
Total brain4.365 ± 0.3414.397 ± 0.341−4.07737<0.001
Table 4. PGLS models assessing the relationship between asymmetry index of the whole brain and three brain regions and predation risk in species in which the left hemisphere is smaller than the right one. Snout–vent length (SVL) was added as a covariate. Phylogenetic scaling parameters (superscripts following λ denote p-values of likelihood ratio tests against models with λ = 0 and 1, respectively).
Table 4. PGLS models assessing the relationship between asymmetry index of the whole brain and three brain regions and predation risk in species in which the left hemisphere is smaller than the right one. Snout–vent length (SVL) was added as a covariate. Phylogenetic scaling parameters (superscripts following λ denote p-values of likelihood ratio tests against models with λ = 0 and 1, respectively).
Dependent VariableBrain RegionsIndependent VariableλR2βtp
Asymmetry indexOlfactory bulbPredation risk<0.001(<0.001, 0.332)0.070−0.022−1.7850.082
SVL −0.049−2.1710.036
TelencephalonPredation risk1(0.032, <0.001)0.0770.0082.2890.027
SVL 0.0161.6000.118
Optic tectaPredation risk0.060(<0.001, 0.434)0.0170.0020.3350.739
SVL −0.014−1.2970.199
Total brainPredation risk0.262(0.034, 0.676)−0.0150.0031.0800.287
SVL 0.0061.0190.315
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, C.; Jiang, Y.; Wu, Y.; Cao, L.; Liao, W. Exploring Brain Size Asymmetry and Its Relationship with Predation Risk Among Chinese Anurans. Biology 2025, 14, 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14010038

AMA Style

Chen C, Jiang Y, Wu Y, Cao L, Liao W. Exploring Brain Size Asymmetry and Its Relationship with Predation Risk Among Chinese Anurans. Biology. 2025; 14(1):38. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14010038

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Chuan, Ying Jiang, Yiming Wu, Lingsen Cao, and Wenbo Liao. 2025. "Exploring Brain Size Asymmetry and Its Relationship with Predation Risk Among Chinese Anurans" Biology 14, no. 1: 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14010038

APA Style

Chen, C., Jiang, Y., Wu, Y., Cao, L., & Liao, W. (2025). Exploring Brain Size Asymmetry and Its Relationship with Predation Risk Among Chinese Anurans. Biology, 14(1), 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology14010038

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop