Overturning Children’s Misconceptions about Ruler Measurement: The Power of Disconfirming Evidence
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Structural Alignment
3. Disconfirming Evidence
4. Current Study
5. Experiment 1—Disconfirming Evidence and Structural Alignment (DE + SA) vs. Business-As-Usual (BAU)
5.1. Method
5.1.1. Participants
5.1.2. Testing Procedures and Materials
Pre-Test, Post-Test, and Follow-Up Test
Training
“This is a ruler. And each of these [Experimenter points to the unit chips] is one unit. They are all the same length. I also have sticks that are different colors and sizes [Experimenter points to the sticks]. We are going to play a game by measuring these sticks with our ruler and units.”
5.2. Results
5.2.1. Performance before and after Training
5.2.2. Exploratory Analysis—The Role of the Pre-Test Strategy
5.3. Discussion
6. Experiment 2—Disconfirming Evidence (DE) versus Structural Alignment (SA)
6.1. Method
6.1.1. Participants
6.1.2. Materials and Procedure
6.2. Results
6.2.1. Performance before and after Training
6.2.2. Exploratory Analysis—The Role of the Pre-Test Strategy
6.3. Discussion
7. Experiment 3—Disconfirming Evidence Alone (DE) vs. Disconfirming Evidence + Structural Alignment (DE + SA)
7.1. Methods
7.1.1. Participants
7.1.2. Materials and Procedure
7.2. Results
7.2.1. Performance before and after Training
7.2.2. Exploratory Analysis—The Role of the Pre-Test Strategy
8. Learning Trajectory during Training
9. Effect of Condition across All Experiments
10. General Discussion
11. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Power Analysis. For this model, we had an R2 value of 0.35, which translates into a Cohen’s f2 of 0.54; we set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 28 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 0.87 for this model. These results suggest that our model was sufficiently powered to detect our key fixed effects of interest in Experiment 1. Note that this power analysis was a conservative approximation based on validated post hoc power analyses for linear, rather than logistic, regression models. |
2 | For this model we had an R2 value of 0.90, which translates into an extremely high Cohen’s f2 of 9.16. We set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 28 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 1.00 for this model. These results suggest that our model was sufficiently powered to detect our fixed effects. The extreme nature of these data—the main effects of condition, strategy, and session are so powerful—meant that the three-way interaction term itself did not add additional descriptive power to the model. |
3 | For this model, we had a delta R2 value of 0.31 for fixed effects, which translates into a Cohen’s f2 of 0.45. We set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 28 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 0.79 for this model. These results suggest that our model was sufficiently powered to detect our fixed effects. |
4 | For this model, we had an R2 value of 0.79, which translates into a Cohen’s f2 of 3.76. We set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 28 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 1.00 for this model. These results suggest that our model was sufficiently powered to detect the reported fixed effects. |
5 | A post hoc analysis that compared the performance on each of the four shifted training conditions from Experiments 1, 2, and 3 did not show any significant differences between conditions (p = 0.549 at post-test and p = .229 at follow-up), suggesting that learning was not significantly affected by whether or not the stick was moved back to the zero-point on the ruler. |
6 | For this model, we had a delta R2 value of 0.25 for fixed effects, which translates into a Cohen’s f2 of 0.33. We set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 32 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 0.70 for this model. These results suggest that our model was slightly underpowered to detect the fixed effects size in our observed data. For this reason, any null findings, such as the main effect of condition, should not be overinterpreted. |
7 | For this model, we had an R2 value of 0.40, which translates into a Cohen’s f2 of 0.67. We set the alpha level to 0.05 and N = 32 participants and estimated an overall power (1 − β) of 0.94 for this model. These results suggest that our model was sufficiently powered to detect the reported fixed effects. |
8 | This analysis is only appropriate for Experiment 3, which directly compared two shifted-object training conditions. In both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, the children’s performance on unshifted problems was nearly perfect during training from the very first trial, as their misconceptions were never challenged by disconfirming evidence and both the hatch mark counting and read-off strategies produced a correct answer in the unshifted condition. |
References
- Barrett, Jeffrey, Douglas Clements, Julie Sarama, Corinne Cullen, Jeanne McCool, Carla Witkowski, and Donald Klanderman. 2012. Evaluating and Improving a Learning Trajectory for Linear Measurement in Elementary Grades 2 and 3: A Longitudinal Study. Mathematical Thinking and Learning 14: 28–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blume, Glendon W., Enrique Galindo, and Charles Walcott. 2007. Performance in measurement and geometry as seen through the lens of principles and standards for school mathematics. In Results and Interpretations of the Mathematics Assessments of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Edited by Peter Kloosterman and Frank K. Lester. Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter, Thomas, Mary Lindquist, Cynthia Brown, Vance Kouba, Arthur Edward, and Jane Swafford. 1988. Results of the Fourth NAEP Assessment of Mathematics: Trends and Conclusions. Arithmetic Teacher 36: 38–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charles, Randall, William Crown, and Francis Fennell. 2008. Mathematics. Edited by Michigan. Glenview: Pearson Education Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Christie, Stella, and Dedre Gentner. 2010. Where hypotheses come from: Learning new relations by structural alignment. Journal of Cognition and Development 11: 356–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Common Core State Standards Initiative. 2010. Common Core State Standards for Mathematics. Washington, DC: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and the Council of Chief State School Officers. Available online: http://www.corestandards.org/assets/CCSSI_Math%20Standards.pdf (accessed on 1 January 2024).
- Congdon, Eliza L., Marina Vasilyeva, Kelly S. Mix, and Susan C. Levine. 2018a. From intuitive spatial measurement to understanding of units. In Visualizing Mathematics. Cham: Springer, pp. 25–46. [Google Scholar]
- Congdon, Eliza L., Mee Kyoung Kwon, and Susan C. Levine. 2018b. Learning to measure through action and gesture: Children’s prior knowledge matters. Cognition 180: 182–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cook, Susan W., Rebecca G. Duffy, and Kristin M. Fenn. 2013. Consolidation and Transfer of Learning after Observing Hand Gesture. Child Development 84: 1863–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cook, Susan W., Zachary Mitchell, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2008. Gesturing Makes Learning Last. Cognition 106: 1047–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gentner, Dedre, Susan C. Levine, Simran Dhillon, Raedy Ping, Catherine Bradley, Alison Polterman, and Gena Honke. 2016. Rapid learning in children via analogical comparison. Cognitive Science 40: 224–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gopnik, Alison, and Andrew N. Meltzoff. 1997. Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grenell, Andrew, Janis R. Butts, Susan C. Levine, and Emily R. Fyfe. Forthcoming. Children’s confidence on mathematical equivalence and fraction problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. in press.
- Grenell, Andrew, Laura J. Nelson, Bailey Gardner, and Emily R. Fyfe. 2022. Children’s confidence using incorrect strategies on mathematical equivalence problems. Cognitive Development 62: 101167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huntley-Fenner, Gavin. 2001. Why count stuff? Young preschoolers do not use number for measurement in continuous dimensions. Developmental Science 4: 456–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalish, Charles W., and Catherine A. Lawson. 2007. Negative Evidence and Inductive Generalization. Thinking & Reasoning 13: 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kamii, Constance, and Franklin Clark. 1997. Measurement of length: The need for a better approach to teaching. School Science and Mathematics 97: 116–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klayman, Joshua, and Young-Won Ha. 1987. Confirmation, Disconfirmation, and Information in Hypothesis Testing. Psychological Review 94: 211–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kurtz, Kenneth J., Olga Boukrina, and Dedre Gentner. 2013. Comparison promotes learning and transfer of relational categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39: 1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwon, Mee-Kyoung, Susan C. Levine, Kristen Ratliff, and Cassandra Snyder. 2011. The importance of alignable differences in teaching linear measurement. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society 33: 33. [Google Scholar]
- Larson, Nancy. 2004. Saxon Math. Austin: Saxon Publishers, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer, Richard, Michael Jenkins, and Helen Osana. 1998. Longitudinal study of children’s reasoning about space and geometry. In Designing Learning Environments for Developing Understanding of Geometry and Space. Edited by Richard Lehrer and Daniel Chazan. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 137–67. [Google Scholar]
- Lemaire, Patrick, and Robert S. Siegler. 1995. Four aspects of strategic change: Contributions to children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124: 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levine, Susan C., Susan Goldin-Meadow, Mindy T. Carlson, and Nancy Hemani-Lopez. 2018. Mental transformation skill in young children: The role of concrete and abstract motor training. Cognitive Science 42: 1207–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lindquist, Mary M., and Vance L. Kouba. 1989. Measurement. In Results from the Fourth Mathematics Assessment of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Edited by Mary M. Lindquist. New York: Teachers College Press, pp. 35–43. [Google Scholar]
- Lubienski, Sarah T. 2003. Is our teaching measuring up: Race-, SES-, and gender-related gaps in measurement achievement. In Learning and Teaching Measurement: 2003 Yearbook. Edited by Douglas H. Clements. Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 282–92. [Google Scholar]
- Lubienski, Sarah T., and Margaret D. Crockett. 2007. NAEP findings regarding race and ethnicity: Mathematics achievement, student affect, and school-home experiences. In Results and Interpretations of the 2003 Mathematics Assessment of the National Assessment of Educational Progress. Edited by Peter Kloosterman. Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 227–60. [Google Scholar]
- Martin, W. Gary, and Marilyn E. Strutchens. 2000. Geometry and measurement. In Results from the Seventh Mathematics Assessment of the NAEP. Edited by Edward A. Silver and Patricia Ann Kenney. Reston: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 193–34. [Google Scholar]
- National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. 2000. Principles and Standards for School Mathematics. Reston: NCTM. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, Laura J., and Emily R. Fyfe. 2019. Metacognitive monitoring and help-seeking decisions on mathematical equivalence problems. Metacognition and Learning 14: 167–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Orona, Cassandra, Valerie Carter, and Hope Kindall. 2017. Understanding standard units of measure. Teaching Children Mathematics 23: 500–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Provasnik, Stephen, Lindsey Malley, Maria Stephens, Katherine Landeros, Robert Perkins, and Judy H. Tang. 2012. Highlights From TIMSS 2011: Mathematics and Science Achievement of U.S. Fourth- and Eighth-Grade Students in an International Context; (NCES 2013-009). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
- Ramscar, Michael, Melody Dye, Hanna Muenke Popick, and Fiona O’Donnell-McCarthy. 2011. The enigma of number: Why children find the meanings of even small number words hard to learn and how we can help them do better. PLoS ONE 6: e22501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ren, Kexin, Ying Lin, and Elizabeth A. Gunderson. 2019. The role of inhibitory control in strategy change: The case of linear measurement. Developmental Psychology 55: 1389–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rescorla, Robert, and Allan R. Wagner. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. Edited by Abraham H Black and William F. Prokasy. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
- Richland, Lindsey, and Janice Hansen. 2013. Reducing Cognitive Load in Learning by Analogy. International Journal of Psychological Studies 5: 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schulz, Laura E. 2012. The origins of inquiry: Inductive inference and exploration in early childhood. Trends in Cognitive Science 16: 382–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schulz, Laura E., Alison Gopnik, and Clark Glymour. 2007. Preschool Children Learn about Causal Structure from Conditional Interventions. Developmental Science 10: 322–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shipley, Elizabeth F., and Barbara Shepperson. 1990. Countable entities: Developmental changes. Cognition 34: 109–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegler, Robert S. 2006. Microgenetic analyses of learning. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Volume 2:Cognition, Perception, and Language, 6th ed. Edited by William Damon, Richard M. Lerner, Deanna Kuhn and Robert S. Siegler. Hoboken: Wiley, pp. 464–510. [Google Scholar]
- Siegler, Robert S., and Svetina Matija. 2002. A microgenetic/cross-sectional study of matrix completion: Comparing short-term and long-term change. Child Development 73: 793–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, Robert S., and Svetina Matija. 2006. What leads children to adopt new strategies? Amicrogenetic/cross sectional study of class inclusion. Child Development 77: 997–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, John P., Lorraine M. Males, Leslie C. Dietiker, KoSze Lee, and Aaron Mosier. 2013. Curricular Treatments of Length Measurement in the United States: Do They Address Known Learning Challenges? Cognition and Instruction 31: 388–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solomon, Tracy L., Marina Vasilyeva, Janellen Huttenlocher, and Susan C. Levine. 2015. Minding the gap: Children’s difficulty conceptualizing spatial intervals as linear measurement units. Developmental Psychology 51: 1564–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stahl, Anne E., and Lisa Feigenson. 2015. Observing the Unexpected Enhances Infants’ Learning and Exploration. Science 348: 91–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Star, Jon R., Courtney Pollack, Kelley Durkin, Bethany Rittle-Johnson, Kathleen Lynch, Kristie Newton, and Claire Gogolen. 2014. Learning from comparison in algebra. Contemporary Educational Psychology 40: 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- The University of Chicago School Mathematics Project. 2007. Everyday Mathematics, 3rd ed. Chicago: Wright Group/McGraw Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, Tony D., and Ronald V. Preston. 2004. Measurement in the middle grades: Insights from NAEP and TIMSS. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School 9: 514–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- TIMSS. 2019. U.S. Highlights Web Report (NCES 2021-021). U.S. Department of Education. Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. Available online: https://nces.ed.gov/timss/results19/index.asp (accessed on 1 January 2024).
- Wason, Peter C. 1960. On the Failure to Eliminate Hypotheses in a Conceptual Task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 12: 129–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, Paul B., and Douglas W. Carnine. 1981. Relationship between range of examples and of instructions and attention in concept attainment. The Journal of Educational Research 74: 144–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Training Condition | Hatch Mark | Read-Off | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Disconfirming Evidence Alone | 2.88 | 2.80 | 4.71 | 1.34 |
Disconfirming Evidence + Structural Alignment | 1.00 | 1.00 | 3.00 | 2.92 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kwon, M.-K.; Congdon, E.; Ping, R.; Levine, S.C. Overturning Children’s Misconceptions about Ruler Measurement: The Power of Disconfirming Evidence. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 62. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070062
Kwon M-K, Congdon E, Ping R, Levine SC. Overturning Children’s Misconceptions about Ruler Measurement: The Power of Disconfirming Evidence. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(7):62. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070062
Chicago/Turabian StyleKwon, Mee-Kyoung, Eliza Congdon, Raedy Ping, and Susan C. Levine. 2024. "Overturning Children’s Misconceptions about Ruler Measurement: The Power of Disconfirming Evidence" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 7: 62. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070062
APA StyleKwon, M. -K., Congdon, E., Ping, R., & Levine, S. C. (2024). Overturning Children’s Misconceptions about Ruler Measurement: The Power of Disconfirming Evidence. Journal of Intelligence, 12(7), 62. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070062