Overconfidence in the Cognitive Reflection Test: Comparing Confidence Resolution for Reasoning vs. General Knowledge
Abstract
:- (a)
- Comparing confidence for correct responses in conflict vs. no-conflict problems provides a test of the metacognitive advantage of having awareness of alternative solutions. According to the metacognitive advantage model (Mata 2019a, 2019b, 2020; Mata and Almeida 2014; Mata et al. 2013b), correct responders should have a confidence boost for conflict problems, as they realize that they were able to overcome a tricky problem for which there is a compelling but misleading response. For no-conflict problems, on the other hand, it is less likely that there is such an awareness of alternative responses, and therefore confidence for correct responses should be high, but not so high as for conflict problems.
- (b)
- Comparing confidence for incorrect responses in conflict vs. no-conflict problems provides a test of whether reasoners can detect their reasoning errors to the same extent when there is an intuitive (though incorrect) response that comes to mind vs. when intuition is absent and responders rely solely on deliberation.
- (c)
- Finally, comparing confidence for incorrect responses in conflict problems vs. confidence for correct responses to no-conflict problems provides an extreme test of failure to detect errors. Because tricky conflict problems are designed to trigger an intuitive (though incorrect) response, the confidence that comes with that response should be greater than the confidence that comes with errors in the no-conflict problems. Still, if reasoners realize somehow that they made a mistake, they should rate their confidence lower than that for correct responses to the control problems. If they do not, this is revealing of their awareness (or lack thereof) of their errors.
1. Study 1
1.1. Method
1.1.1. Participants
1.1.2. Procedure
1.2. Results
1.2.1. Performance
1.2.2. Global Confidence
1.2.3. Local Confidence
2. Study 2
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Procedure
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Performance
2.2.2. Global Confidence
2.2.3. Local Confidence
3. Study 3
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Performance
3.2.2. Confidence
4. Study 4
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Procedure
4.2. Results
4.2.1. Performance
4.2.2. Confidence
4.2.3. PDP
5. General Discussion
5.1. Unskilled and Unaware: On Conflict Detection and Error Monitoring
5.2. Skilled and Aware: The Metacognitive Advantage of Deliberative Responders
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Questions | Response Options | ||
---|---|---|---|
What is the name for an instant camera? | Canon camera | Polaroid camera | Minolta camera |
Where do flounders usually live? | Among the reeds | Amongst coral reefs | On the sea bottom |
What is a rollmop made of? | herring | pork | salmon |
What country does the Nobel Prize winner in Literature Gabriel García Márquez come from? | Spain | Venezuela | Colombia |
What artistic movement does anacreontics belong to? | Rococo | Romanticism | Realism |
What is a hot chilli sauce? | Tabasco | Curacao | Macao |
How many letters are there in the Russian alphabet? | 40 | 33 | 26 |
“Tosca” is an opera by …? | G. Puccini | G. Verdi | A. Vivaldi |
What is the name of the Greek Goddess of wisdom? | Pallas Athena | Nike | Penelope |
What is the most abundant metal on Earth? | iron | Aluminum | Copper |
What is a word to describe an unknowing person? | Ignatius | ignorant | ideologue |
Who was the first person to fly around the Eiffel Tower in an airship? | Santos-Dumont | Count Zeppelin | Saint-Exupéry |
What is the name of Eskimo snow shelter? | wigwam | igloo | tipi |
What enterprise belongs to Bill Gates? | Intel | Microsoft | Dell Computers |
What is the Islamic month of fasting called? | Sharia | Ramadan | Imam |
What language does the term “Fata Morgana” come from? | Arabic | Swahili | Italian |
How long does it take for a hen to hatch an egg? | 21 days | 14 days | 28 days |
What is ascorbic acid? | apple vinegar | vitamin A | vitamin C |
Conflict Version | No-Conflict Version |
---|---|
A TV and a DVD together cost 88 euros. The TV costs 80 euros more than the DVD. How much does the DVD cost? | A cake and a piece of fruit cost $8.47 in total. The cake costs $6.71. How much does the piece of fruit cost? |
If it takes 10 hens 10 days to lay 10 eggs, how long would it take 100 hens to lay 100 eggs? | If it takes 14 min for a turtle to lay 5 eggs, how long would it take for the turtle to lay 100 eggs? |
A computer virus is spreading through the system of a computer. Every minute, the number of infected files doubles. If it takes 100 min for the virus to infect all of the system, how long would it take for the virus to infect half of the system? | In a town, there is a flood. If it takes 48 days for the water to cover the entire town, how long would it take for it to cover a town that is 5 times larger? |
Conflict Version (Intuitive and Correct Responses, and Source) | No-Conflict Version |
---|---|
A TV and a DVD together cost 88 euros. The TV costs 80 euros more than the DVD. How much does the DVD cost? (intuitive response = 8; correct response = 4; Mata and Almeida 2014, adapted from Frederick 2005) | A TV and a DVD together cost 88 euros. The TV costs 80 euros. How much does the DVD cost? |
If it takes 10 hens 10 days to lay 10 eggs, how long would it take 100 hens to lay 100 eggs? (intuitive response = 100; correct response = 10; Mata and Almeida 2014, adapted from Frederick 2005) | If 10 hens lay 10 eggs in one day, how many eggs would 100 chickens lay in one day? |
A computer virus is spreading through the system of a computer. Every minute, the number of infected files doubles. If it takes 100 min for the virus to infect all of the system, how long would it take for the virus to infect half of the system? (intuitive response = 50; correct response = 99; Mata and Almeida 2014, adapted from Frederick 2005) | A computer virus is spreading through the system of a computer. Every minute, one file gets infected. If it takes 100 min for the virus to infect all of the system, how long would it take for the virus to infect half of the system? |
A farmer had 15 sheep and all but 8 died. How many are left? (intuitive response = 7; correct response = 8; Thomson and Oppenheimer 2016) | A farmer had 15 sheep and 8 died. How many are left? |
How many cubic feet of dirt are there in a hole that is 3’deep x 3’ wide x 3’ long? (intuitive response = 27; correct response = none; Thomson and Oppenheimer 2016) | How many cubic feet of water are there in a fish tank that is 3’deep x 3’ wide x 3’ long? |
Steve was standing in a long line. To amuse himself he counted the people waiting, and saw that he stood 15th from the beginning and 15th from the end of the line. How many people stood in the line? (intuitive responses = 31 or 30; correct response = 29; Ackerman 2014) | Steve was standing in a long line. To amuse himself he counted the people waiting, and saw that there were 15 people in front of him and 15 behind him. How many people stood in the line? |
Ellen and Kim are running around a track. They run equally fast but Ellen started later. When Ellen has run 5 laps, Kim has run 15 laps. When Ellen had run 30 laps, how many has Kim run? (intuitive response = 90; correct response = 40; Van Dooren et al. 2005) | Ellen and Kim are running around a track. Kim runs 3 times as fast as Ellen. When Ellen has run 5 laps, Kim has run 15 laps. When Ellen had run 30 laps, how many has Kim run? |
One month of the year has 28 days. How many of the remaining 11 months have 30 days? (intuitive response = 5; correct response = 11; Weber 2016); | One month of the year has 28 days. How many of the remaining 11 months have 31 days? |
You have a book of matches and enter a cold, dark room. You know that in the room there is an oil lamp, a candle, and a heater. What do you light first? (intuitive response = oil lamp, candle, or heater; correct response = matches; Boland 2013) | You enter a cold, dark room. You know that in the room there is an oil lamp, a candle, and a heater. What do you light first? |
There are 25 soldiers in a row standing 3 m from each other. How long is the row? (intuitive response = 75 m; correct response = 72 m; Oldrati et al. 2016). | There are 25 vans in a row. Each of them is 3 m long. How long is the row? |
Which city is further north: Rome or New York? (intuitive response = New York; correct response = Rome; Juslin et al. 2000) | Which city is further north: Rio de Janeiro or New York? |
What is the capital of California: San Francisco, Los Angeles, or Sacramento? (intuitive responses = San Francisco, Los Angeles; correct response = Sacramento; Koriat 1995) | What is the largest city in California: San Francisco, Los Angeles, or Sacramento? |
Would it be ethical for a man to marry the sister of his widow? (intuitive response = yes or no, justified; correct response = impossible, he is dead; Sirota et al. 2021) | Would it be ethical for a man to marry the sister of his wife? |
The wind blows west. An electric train runs east. In which cardinal direction does the smoke from the locomotive blow? (intuitive response = west; correct response = there is no smoke in an electric train; Sirota et al. 2021) | The wind blows west. A train runs east. In which cardinal direction does the smoke from the locomotive blow? |
What is the capital of Australia: Sydney, Canberra, or Melbourne? (intuitive response = Sidney; correct response = Canberra; Weber 2016) | What is the largest city in Australia: Sydney, Canberra, or Melbourne? |
Who was the Mathematician who was the author of the theorem that states that the square of the length of the hypotenuse of a rectangle is equal to the sum of squares of the lengths of other sides? (intuitive response = Pythagoras; correct response = no one; it was a triangle, not a rectangle; Mata et al. 2013a) | Who was the Mathematician who was the author of the theorem that states that the square of the length of the hypotenuse of a triangle is equal to the sum of squares of the lengths of other sides? |
When was the first air raid: 1849 or 1937? (intuitive response = 1937; correct response = 1849; Fischhoff et al. 1977; Koriat 2008) | When was the first air raid: 1819 or 1849? |
How many animals of each kind did Moses take in the ark? (intuitive response = 2; correct response = zero; Erickson and Mattson 1981) | How many animals of each kind did Noah take in the ark? |
What are the names of the Portuguese archipelagos located to the east of continental Portugal? (intuitive response = Madeira and Azores; correct response = the archipelagos are located to the west, not east; Mata et al. 2013a) | What are the names of the Portuguese archipelagos located to the west of continental Portugal? |
Potatoes are native to Ireland or Peru? (intuitive response = Ireland; correct response = Peru; Fischhoff et al. 1977) | Potatoes are native to Peru or Japan? |
References
- Ackerman, Rakefet. 2014. The diminishing criterion model for metacognitive regulation of time investment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 1349–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aczel, Balazs, Aba Szollosi, and Bence Bago. 2016. Lax monitoring versus logical intuition: The determinants of confidence in conjunction fallacy. Thinking & Reasoning 22: 99–117. [Google Scholar]
- Atir, Stav, Emily Rosenzweig, and David Dunning. 2015. When knowledge knows no bounds: Self-perceived expertise predicts claims of impossible knowledge. Psychological Science 26: 1295–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bago, Bence, and Wim De Neys. 2017. Fast logic?: Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition 158: 90–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialek, Michal, and Gordon Pennycook. 2018. The cognitive reflection test is robust to multiple exposures. Behavior Research Methods 50: 1953–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boland, Sam. 2013. Reflection expanded: Expanding the Cognitive Reflection Task. Available online: https://pt.scribd.com/document/138024235/Reflection-Expanded-Expanding-the-Cognitive-Reflection-Task (accessed on 21 November 2022).
- Bonner, Carissa, and Ben R. Newell. 2010. In conflict with ourselves? An investigation of heuristic and analytic processes in decision making. Memory & Cognition 38: 186–96. [Google Scholar]
- Branas-Garza, Pablo, Praveen Kujal, and Balint Lenkei. 2019. Cognitive reflection test: Whom, how, when. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics 82: 101455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burič, Roman, and Jakub Šrol. 2020. Individual differences in logical intuitions on reasoning problems presented under two-response paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 32: 460–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Neys, Wim. 2012. Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7: 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Neys, Wim, Sandrine Rossi, and Olivier Houdé. 2013. Bats, balls, and substitution sensitivity: Cognitive misers are no happy fools. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 269–73. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, Wim, Sofie Cromheeke, and Magda Osman. 2011. Biased but in doubt: Conflict and decision confidence. PLoS ONE 6: e15954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunning, David. 2011. The Dunning–Kruger effect: On being ignorant of one’s own ignorance. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press, vol. 44, pp. 247–96. [Google Scholar]
- Erickson, Thomas D., and Mark E. Mattson. 1981. From words to meaning: A semantic illusion. Journal of Memory and Language 20: 540–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, Mário B., André Mata, Christopher Donkin, Steven J. Sherman, and Max Ihmels. 2016. Analytic and heuristic processes in the detection and resolution of conflict. Memory & Cognition 44: 1050–63. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira, Mário. B., Jerônimo C. Soro, Joana Reis, André Mata, and Valerie A. Thompson. 2022. When Type 2 Processing Misfires: The Indiscriminate Use of Statistical Thinking about Reasoning Problems. Journal of Intelligence 10: 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, Mário B., Leonel Garcia-Marques, Steven J. Sherman, and Jeffrey W. Sherman. 2006. Automatic and controlled components of judgment and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 797–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischhoff, Baruch, Paul Slovic, and Sarah Lichtenstein. 1977. Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3: 552–64. [Google Scholar]
- Frederick, Shane. 2005. Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives 19: 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frey, Darren, Eric D. Johnson, and Wim De Neys. 2017. Individual differences in conflict detection during reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71: 1188–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghasemi, Omid, Simon Handley, Stephanie Howarth, Ian R. Newman, and Valerie A. Thompson. 2022. Logical intuition is not really about logic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 151: 2009–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ghasemi, Omid, Simon. J. Handley, and Stephanie Howarth. 2023. Illusory intuitive inferences: Matching heuristics explain logical intuitions. Cognition 235: 105417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, Larry L. 1991. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30: 513–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, Larry L., Jeffrey P. Toth, and Andrew P. Yonelinas. 1993. Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122: 139–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Eric D., Elisabet Tubau, and Wim De Neys. 2016. The Doubting System 1: Evidence for automatic substitution sensitivity. Acta Psychologica 164: 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Juslin, Peter, Anders Winman, and Henrik Olsson. 2000. Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: A critical examination of the hard–easy effect. Psychological Review 107: 384–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klauer, Karl Christoph, and Henrik Singmann. 2013. Does logic feel good? Testing for intuitive detection of logicality in syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39: 1265–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koriat, Asher. 1995. Dissociating knowing and the feeling of knowing: Further evidence for the accessibility model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124: 311–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, Asher. 2008. Subjective confidence in one’s answers: The consensuality principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34: 945–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kruger, Justin. 1999. Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average effect” and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 221–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kruger, Justin, and David Dunning. 1999. Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 1121–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liberman, Varda. 2004. Local and global judgments of confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30: 729–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André. 2019a. Further tests of the metacognitive advantage model: Counterfactuals, confidence and affect. Psychological Topics (Special Issue on Meta-Reasoning) 28: 155–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André. 2019b. Social metacognition in moral judgment: Decisional conflict promotes perspective taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 117: 1061–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mata, André. 2020. Metacognition and social perception: Bringing meta-reasoning and social cognition together. Thinking and Reasoning 26: 140–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André, and Tiago Almeida. 2014. Using metacognitive cues to infer others’ thinking. Judgment and Decision Making 9: 349–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André, Anna-Lena Schubert, and Mário B. Ferreira. 2014. The role of language comprehension in reasoning: How “good enough” representations induce biases. Cognition 133: 457–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André, Klaus Fiedler, Mário B. Ferreira, and Tiago Almeida. 2013a. Reasoning about others’ reasoning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49: 486–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mata, André, Mário B. Ferreira, and Joana Reis. 2013b. A process-dissociation analysis of semantic illusions. Acta Psychologica 144: 433–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mata, André, Mário B. Ferreira, and Steven J. Sherman. 2013c. The metacognitive advantage of deliberative thinkers: A dual-process perspective on overconfidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105: 353–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer-Grant, Constatin G., Nicole Cruz, Henrik Singmann, Samuel Winiger, Spriha Goswami, Brett K. Hayes, and Karl Christoph Klauer. 2022. Are logical intuitions only make-believe? Reexamining the logic-liking effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michailova, Julija, and Joana K. Katter. 2014. Quantifying overconfidence in experimental finance. International Journal of Behavioural Accounting and Finance 4: 351–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, Don, and Paul J. Healy. 2008. The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review 115: 502–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oldrati, Viola, Jessica Patricelli, Barbara Colombo, and Alessandro Antonietti. 2016. The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in inhibition mechanism: A study on cognitive reflection test and similar tasks through neuromodulation. Neuropsychologia 91: 499–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pennycook, Gordon, Robert M. Ross, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2017. Dunning–Kruger effects in reasoning: Theoretical implications of the failure to recognize incompetence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1774–84. [Google Scholar]
- Pulford, Briony D., and Andrew M. Colman. 1997. Overconfidence: Feedback and item difficulty effects. Personality and Individual Differences 23: 125–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Šćeta, Lamija, Josip Sliško, and Nikola Erceg. 2022. Predicting the most common incorrect response: Metacognitive advantage of deliberative over intuitive responders on Cognitive Reflection Test. Studia Psychologica 64: 256–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, Gregory, and Teresa Debacker Roedel. 1994. Test difficulty and judgment bias. Memory & Cognition 22: 63–69. [Google Scholar]
- Sirota, Miroslav, Chris Drewberry, Marie Juanchich, Lenka Valus, and Amanda C. Marshall. 2021. Measuring Cognitive Reflection without Maths: Developing and Validation of the Verbal Cognitive Reflection Test. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 34: 322–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Šrol, Jakub, and Wim De Neys. 2021. Predicting individual differences in conflict detection and bias susceptibility during reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning 27: 38–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stupple, Edward J., Linden J. Ball, Jonathan S. B. Evans, and Emily Kamal-Smith. 2011. When logic and belief collide: Individual differences in reasoning times support a selective processing model. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 23: 931–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stupple, Edward J., Melanie Pitchford, Linden J. Ball, Thomas E. Hunt, and Richard Steel. 2017. Slower is not always better: Response-time evidence clarifies the limited role of miserly information processing in the Cognitive Reflection Test. PLoS ONE 12: e0186404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomson, Kella S., and Daniel M. Oppenheimer. 2016. Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision Making 11: 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trouche, Emmanuel, Emmanuel Sander, and Hugo Mercier. 2014. Arguments, more than confidence, explain the good performance of reasoning groups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 1958–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Trouche, Emmanuel, Jing Shao, and Hugo Mercier. 2019. Objective evaluation of demonstrative arguments. Argumentation 33: 23–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Dooren, Wim, Dirk De Bock, An Hessels, Dirk Janssens, and Lieven Verschaffel. 2005. Not everything is proportional: Effects of age and problem type on propensities for overgeneralization. Cognition and Instruction 23: 57–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, Mel. 2016. The Role of Metacognition and Anxiety in College Students’ Performance on a General-Knowledge Test (Honors Thesis). Available online: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/7ae5/ee960a080a62fe15cd9e17fe01265ec1e522.pdf?_ga=2.29673148.2064039678.1582733636-1459196501.1522233394 (accessed on 6 December 2022).
CRT | GK | |
---|---|---|
Correct | 8.49 (1.32) | 7.11 (1.45) |
Incorrect | 6.82 (2.23) | 4.35 (2.23) |
CRT | GK | |
---|---|---|
Correct | 7.99 (1.74) | 6.72 (1.05) |
Incorrect | 6.56 (2.04) | 4.09 (1.58) |
Conflict | No-Conflict | |
---|---|---|
Correct | 8.05 (1.49) | 7.33 (1.59) |
Incorrect | 6.27 (2.17) | 5.37 (2.81) |
Conflict | No-Conflict | |
---|---|---|
Correct | 6.86 (1.67) | 7.05 (0.99) |
Incorrect | 6.27 (1.36) | 4.17 (2.26) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mata, A. Overconfidence in the Cognitive Reflection Test: Comparing Confidence Resolution for Reasoning vs. General Knowledge. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050081
Mata A. Overconfidence in the Cognitive Reflection Test: Comparing Confidence Resolution for Reasoning vs. General Knowledge. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(5):81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050081
Chicago/Turabian StyleMata, André. 2023. "Overconfidence in the Cognitive Reflection Test: Comparing Confidence Resolution for Reasoning vs. General Knowledge" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 5: 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050081
APA StyleMata, A. (2023). Overconfidence in the Cognitive Reflection Test: Comparing Confidence Resolution for Reasoning vs. General Knowledge. Journal of Intelligence, 11(5), 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11050081