The Search for the Elusive Basic Processes Underlying Human Intelligence: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Major Contentions of this Article
- The search for basic processes of intelligence has had, at best, mixed success because researchers do not know how to find truly basic processes. They would not even know if they found the basic processes because there is no empirical test that will reveal a set of processes as basic. Nor is it even clear what a “basic process” is. As a result, much of the literature is searching for a kind of “grail” that, if it existed, would not be recognized if it were found.
- The attempt of researchers of intelligence to argue for the superiority of their information-processing analysis of intelligence based on levels of correlation coefficients is misguided and fruitless for five reasons.
- As we all learn in elementary statistics, correlation does not imply causation.
- Correlations of process measures with measures of intelligence tend to be modest or at best moderate, in any case (see essays Sternberg 1984, 2020c).
- Even when the correlations are moderate, the direction of causality is unclear: for example, the correlations may be due to hidden third variables.
- Although the information-processing approach was originally designed to redefine intelligence research and move beyond psychometrically-based correlational studies (e.g., see, Hunt et al. 1973; Sternberg 1977), the correlations of information-processing measures with psychometric tests assume, somewhat ironically, that conventional psychometric tests are an appropriate ultimate criterion for whether the information-processing analysis is valid. As a result, the information-processing analyses become subservient to the psychometric analyses they originally were designed to improve upon and perhaps even ultimately to replace.
- Meta-analyses of the strength of relation between information-processing measures and psychometrically measured intelligence (see, e.g., Grudnik and Kranzler 2001; Jensen 1998; Kranzler and Jensen 1989; Redick and Lindsey 2013) do not address the question of basic processes because, for the most part, they combine results of correlational analyses, and thus are themselves correlational analyses. That is, combining correlational analyses still leaves one with a correlational analysis.
- There are now so many measures that have been found to correlate significantly with measured intelligence, more or less in line with Spearman’s (1904, 1927) findings, that any claims of causality are largely useless because correlations already have shown themselves not to discriminate well among alternative claims regarding what is basic (see Conway and Kovacs 2020; Ellingsen et al. 2020; Nettelbeck et al. 2020). If statistically significant correlations signify a winner of the “basic process” derby, then there are too many winners of this derby.
3. Historical Perspectives on the Processes Underlying Intelligence
3.1. Psychometric Origins
- Direction. This process involves knowing what has to be accomplished and how it can be accomplished. Today it might be called something like “problem formulation.”
- Adaptation. This process requires selecting, implementing, and monitoring one’s strategies for problem solving so as to maximize adaptation to the environment.
- Control. This process involves the ability to critique and correct one’s thoughts and actions. It is a reflective process that ensures one can regulate how one solves a problem.
- Apprehension of experience. This process is what we today might call encoding, as in reading the terms of the analogy and understanding what they mean;
- Eduction of relations. This is an inferential process, as in recognizing the relation between dark and light;
- Eduction of correlates. This process involves applying the relation one has inferred, as in applying the relationship of antonyms to recognize the opposite of tall is short.
- Cognition—the ability to understand or comprehend;
- Memory recording—the ability to encode information and enter it into memory;
- Memory retention—the ability to remember information;
- Divergent production—the ability to generate multiple solutions to a problem lacking any one so-called “correct” answer;
- Convergent production—the ability to come upon a correct answer to a problem;
- Evaluation—the ability to judge whether an answer is correct or, at least, reasonable.
3.2. Early Information-Processing Psychology
3.2.1. The Cognitive-Correlates Approach
3.2.2. The Cognitive-Components Approach
3.2.3. The Working-Memory Approach
3.2.4. Other Approaches
4. What Intelligence Is Varies in Part from Place to Place and Time to Time
4.1. The Inconstancy of the Processes of Intelligence over Time
4.2. The Inconstancy of the Processes of Intelligence over Space
5. Breadth of Information Processing Underlying Intelligence
- Broader theories of kinds of intelligence are not really theories of intelligence at all. On this view, the theories use the word “intelligence” but are really theories of something else, perhaps of “skills”, “abilities”, or “aptitudes”, etc. In this case, the question of whether the processes of intelligence need to be broadened simply does not apply. One recent review of intelligence (Deary 2020) does not appear to view these various theories of intelligence as serious contenders for a place in the intelligence literature. Hunt (2011) took a similar view.
- Broader theories perhaps are theories of kinds of intelligence, but these kinds of intelligence are, at best, peripheral in the study of intelligence. In his textbook on intelligence, Mackintosh (2011) viewed the broader theories of intelligence as interesting but as somewhat peripheral.
- Broader theories need to subsume and possibly replace existing theories of intelligence, which simply are too narrow. This view would be that of Gardner (2011) and of Sternberg (2021b). Gardner and Sternberg would argue that traditional theories have been too narrow and have “missed the boat” in terms of understanding the abilities that intelligence should encompass. Gardner’s theory, however, has lacked traditional empirical support.
6. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ang, Soon, and Linn Van Dyne, eds. 2015. Handbook of Cultural Intelligence: Theory, Measurement, and Applications. Abingdon and Oxfordshire: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Ang, Soon, Kok Yee Ng, and Thomas Rockstuhl. 2020. Cultural intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 780–801. [Google Scholar]
- Barbey, Aron K., Sherif Karama, and Richard J. Haier, eds. 2021. Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence and Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Berry, John W. 1974. Radical cultural relativism and the concept of intelligence. In Culture and Cognition: Readings in Cross-Cultural Psychology. Edited by John W. Berry and Pierre R. Dasen. London: Methuen, pp. 225–29. [Google Scholar]
- Binet, Alfred, and Théodore Simon. 1916. The Development of Intelligence in Children. Translated by Elizabeth S. Kite. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
- Boring, Edwin G. 1923. Intelligence as the tests measure it. New Republic 36: 35–37. [Google Scholar]
- Boyatzis, Richard E. 2020. Leadership intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 802–19. [Google Scholar]
- Brody, Nathan. 2003. Construct validation of the Sternberg Triarchic abilities test: Comment and reanalysis. Intelligence 31: 319–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bronfenbrenner, Urie, and Stephen J. Ceci. 1994. Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review 101: 568–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Business Tech. 2014. The Biggest Killer Diseases in History. October 26. Available online: https://businesstech.co.za/news/trending/71652/the-biggest-killer-diseases-in-history/ (accessed on 3 April 2022).
- Carpenter, Patricia A., Marcel A. Just, and Peter Shell. 1990. What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review 97: 404–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carroll, John B. 1976. Psychometric tests as cognitive tasks: A new "Structure of Intellect". In The Nature of Intelligence. Edited by Lauren B. Resnick. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 27–56. [Google Scholar]
- Carroll, John B. 1993. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Wurvey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ceci, Stephen J., and Antonio Roazzi. 1994. The effects of context on cognition: Postcards from Brazil. In Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and Richard K. Wagner. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 74–101. [Google Scholar]
- Ceci, Stephen J., and Urie Bronfenbrenner. 1985. “Don’t forget to take the cupcakes out of the oven”: Prospective memory, strategic time-monitoring, and context. Child Development 56: 152–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chi, Michelene T. H. 1978. Knowledge structure and memory development. In Children’s Thinking: What Develops? Edited by Robert S. Siegler. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 73–96. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, Michelene T. 1992. Conceptual change within and across ontological categories: Examples from learning and discovery in science. In Cognitive Models of Science: Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Edited by Ronald N. Giere. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 129–86. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, Dov, and Shinobu Kitayama, eds. 2020. Handbook of Cultural Psychology, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cole, Michael, John Gay, Joseph A. Glick, and Donald W. Sharp. 1971. The Cultural Context of Learning and Thinking. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Colvin, Dearborn, Haggerty Freeman, Peterson Henmon, Pressey Pintner, Terman Ruml, Thurstone Thorndike, and Woodrow Whipple. 1921. “Intelligence and its measurement”: A symposium. Journal of Educational Psychology 12: 123–147, 195–216, 271–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conway, Andrew R. A., and Kristof Kovacs. 2013. Individual differences in intelligence and working memory: A review of latent variable models. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Brian H. Ross. Cambridge: Academic Press, pp. 233–70. [Google Scholar]
- Conway, Andrew R. A., and Kristof Kovacs. 2018. The nature of the general factor of intelligence. In The Nature of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conway, Andrew R. A., and Kristof Kovacs. 2020. Working memory and intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 504–27. [Google Scholar]
- Cronbach, Lee J. 1957. The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist 12: 671–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, Lee J., and Richard E. Snow. 1977. Aptitudes and Instructional Methods. New York: Irvington. [Google Scholar]
- Daneman, Meredyth, and Patricia A. Carpenter. 1983. Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 9: 561–84. [Google Scholar]
- Deary, Ian J. 2020. Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction, 2nd ed.New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Deary, Ian J. 2000. Looking Down on Human Intelligence: From Psychometrics to the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Deary, Ian J., and Con Stough. 1996. Intelligence and inspection time: Achievements, prospects, and problems. American Psychologist 51: 599–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, Ian J., and Robert J. Sternberg. 2021. Ian Deary and Robert Sternberg answer five self-inflicted questions about human intelligence. Intelligence 85: 101539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Detterman, Douglas K. 1980. Does “g” exist? Intelligence 6: 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncker, Karl. 1972. On Problem-Solving. Godkin Lectures at Harvard University. Westport: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
- Ellingsen, Victor J., Randall W. Engle, and Robert J. Sternberg. 2020. Cognitive approaches to intelligence. In Human Intelligence: An Introduction. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 104–38. [Google Scholar]
- Engle, Randall W. 2018. Working memory and executive attention: A revisit. Perspectives on Psychological Science 13: 190–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engle, Randall W., and Michael J. Kane. 2004. Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Brian H. Ross. Abingdon and Oxfordshire: Academic Press, vol. 44, pp. 145–99. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn, James R. 1987. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations. Psychological Bulletin 101: 171–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, James R. 2012. Are We Getting Smarter? New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn, James R. 2016. Does Your Family Make You Smarter? Nature, Nurture, and Human Autonomy. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Freud, Sigmund. 1989. Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis. New York: Liveright. [Google Scholar]
- Fry, Astrid F., and Sandra Hale. 1996. Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science 7: 237–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fukuyama, Francis. 2006. The End of History and the Last Man. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Galton, Francis. 1883/1907/1973. Inquiries Into Human Faculty and Its Development. New York: AMS Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner, Howard. 2011. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. rev. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Garner, Wendell R., Harold W. Hake, and Charles W. Eriksen. 1956. Operationism and the concept of perception. Psychological Review 63: 149–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geher, Glenn, Scott Barry Kaufman, Julie A. Planke, and Jacqueline M. Di Santo. 2020. Mating intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 846–70. [Google Scholar]
- Gignac, Gilles, and Philip A. Vernon. 2003. Digit symbol rotation. Intelligence 31: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, Sam, Dana Princiotta, and Jack A. Naglieri, eds. 2019. Handbook of Intelligence: Evolutionary Theory, Historical Perspective, and Current Concepts. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Greenfield, Patricia M. 2020. Historical evolution of intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 916–39. [Google Scholar]
- Greiff, Samuel, Sascha Wüstenberg, Gyöngyvér Molnár, Andreas Fischer, Joachim Funke, and Benő Csapó. 2013. Complex problem solving in educational contexts—Something beyond g: Concept, assessment, measurement invariance, and construct validity. Journal of Educational Psychology 105: 364–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grigorenko, Elena L., Elisa Meier, Jerry Lipka, Gerald Mohatt, Evelyn Yanez, and Robert J. Sternberg. 2004. Academic and practical intelligence: A case study of the Yup’ik in Alaska. Learning and Individual Differences 14: 183–207. [Google Scholar]
- Grudnik, Jennifer L., and John H. Kranzler. 2001. Meta-analysis of the relationship between inspection intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence 29: 523–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guilford, Joy Paul. 1967. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Guilford, Joy Paul. 1982. Cognitive psychology’s ambiguities: Some suggested remedies. Psychological Review 89: 48–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guilford, Joy Paul, and Ralph Hoepfner. 1971. The Analysis of Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Haier, Richard J. 2016. The Neuroscience of Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Haier, Richard J. 2020. Biological approaches to intelligence. In Human Intelligence: An Introduction. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 139–73. [Google Scholar]
- Haier, Richard J., and Rex E. Jung. 2007. Beautiful minds (i.e., brains) and the neural basis of intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 174–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedlund, Jennifer. 2020. Practical intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 736–55. [Google Scholar]
- Horn, John L. 1967. On subjectivity in factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement 27: 811–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, John L., and John R. Knapp. 1973. On the subjective character of the empirical base of Guilford’s structure-of-intellect model. Psychological Bulletin 80: 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, Earl. 1980. Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology 71: 449–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hunt, Earl. 2011. Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, Earl, Clifford Lunneborg, and Joe Lewis. 1975. What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology 7: 194–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, Earl, Nancy Frost, and Clifford Lunneborg. 1973. Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Edited by Gordon H. Bower. Oxford: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jeffries, Robin. P., Peter G. Polson, Lydia Razran, and Michael E. Atwood. 1977. A process model for missionaries: Cannibals and other river-crossing problems. Cognitive Psychology 9: 412–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, Arthur R. 1982a. Reaction time and psychometric g. In A Model for Intelligence. Edited by Hans Jurgen Eysenck. Heidelberg: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, Arthur R. 1982b. The chronometry of intelligence. In Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 1, pp. 255–310. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, Arthur R. 1998. The g Factor. Westport, CT: Praeger-Greenwood. [Google Scholar]
- Jung, Rex E., and Richard J. Haier. 2007. The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 135–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Kihlstrom, John F., and Nancy Cantor. 2020. Social intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 756–79. [Google Scholar]
- Köhler, Wolfgang. 1970. Gestalt Psychology: The Definitive Statement of the Gestalt Theory, 2nd rev. ed. New York: Liveright. [Google Scholar]
- Kornhaber, Mindy L. 2020. The theory of multiple intelligences. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 659–78. [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2019a. A unified cognitive/differential approach to human intelligence: Implications for IQ testing. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 8: 255–72. [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2019b. What is IQ? Life beyond “general intelligence”. Current Directions in Psychological Science 28: 189–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kranzler, John H., and Arthur R. Jensen. 1989. Inspection time and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence 13: 329–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, Thomas. 2012. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 50th anniversary ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kyllonen, Patrick C., and Raymond E. Christal. 1990. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity? Intelligence 14: 389–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mackintosh, Nicholas J. 2011. IQ and Human Intelligence, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Malone, Thomas W., and Anita Williams Woolley. 2020. Collective intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 780–801. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, John D., Peter Salovey, and David R. Caruso. 2004. Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry 15: 197–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGrew, Kevin S. 2005. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities: Past, present, and future. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, Issues, 2nd ed. Edited by Dawn P. Flanagan and Patti L. Harrison. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 136–81. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, George A., Eugene Galanter, and Karl H. Pribram. 1960. Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: Henry Holt. [Google Scholar]
- Mulholland, Timothy M., James W. Pellegrino, and Robert Glaser. 1980. Components of geometric analogy solution. Cognitive Psychology 12: 252–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neisser, Ulric. 1967. Cognitive Psychology. Upper Saddle River: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [Google Scholar]
- Nettelbeck, Ted, and M. Lally. 1976. Inspection time and measured intelligence. British Journal of Psychology 67: 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nettelbeck, Ted, Oliver Zwalf, and Con Stough. 2020. Basic processes of intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 471–503. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, Allen, and Herbert Alexander Simon. 1972. Human Problem Solving. Upper Saddle River: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Nuñes, Terezhina. 1994. Street intelligence. In Encyclopedia of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Macmillan, vol. 2, pp. 1045–49. [Google Scholar]
- Pellegrino, James W., and Robert Glaser. 1979. Cognitive correlates and components in the analysis of individual differences. In Human Intelligence: Perspectives on Its Theory and Measurement. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and Douglas K. Detterman. Norwood: Ablex, pp. 61–88. [Google Scholar]
- Pellegrino, James W., and Robert Glaser. 1980. Components of inductive reasoning. In Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction: Cognitive Process Analyses of Aptitude. Edited by Richard E. Snow, Pat-Anthony Federico and William E. Montague. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, vol. 1, pp. 177–217. [Google Scholar]
- Pellegrino, James-W., and Robert Kail. 1982. Process analyses of spatial aptitude. In Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, vol. 1, pp. 311–65. [Google Scholar]
- Posner, Michael I., and Aron K. Barbey. 2020. General intelligence in the age of neuroimaging. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 18: 100126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Posner, Michael I., and Ronald F. Mitchell. 1967. Chronometric analysis of classification. Psychological Review 74: 392–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preiss, David D., and Robert J. Sternberg. 2005. Technologies for working intelligences. In Intelligence and Technology. The Impact of Tools on the Nature and Development of Human Abilities. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and David D. Preiss. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 183–208. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John C. 1938. Progressive Matrices: A Perceptual Test of Intelligence. Individual Form. Stockport: Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John. 1986. Manual for Raven Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Stockport: Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Redick, Thomas S., and Dakota R. B. Lindsey. 2013. Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 1102–13. [Google Scholar]
- Reicher, Gerald M. 1969. Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology 81: 275–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivers, Susan E., Isaac J. Handley-Miner, John D. Mayer, and David R. Caruso. 2020. Emotional intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 709–35. [Google Scholar]
- Royer, Fred L. 1971. Information processing of visual figures in the digit symbol substitution task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 87: 335–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, Benedikt, Nicolas Becker, Florian Krieger, Frank M. Spinath, and Jörn R. Sparfeldt. 2020. Teaching the underlying rules of figural matrices in a short video increases test scores. Intelligence 82: 101473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serpell, Robert. 1974. Aspects of intelligence in a developing country. African Social Research 17: 576–96. [Google Scholar]
- Shepard, Roger N., and Jacqueline Metzler. 1971. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171: 701–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Skinner, Burrhus Frederic. 1965. Science and Human Behavior. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, Richard E., and David F. Lohman. 1984. Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 347–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snow, Richard E., and David F. Lohman. 1989. Implications of cognitive psychology for educational measurement. In Educational Measurement, 3rd ed. Edited by Robert L. Linn. New York: Macmillan, pp. 263–331. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, Richard E., Patrick C. Kyllonen, and Brachia Marshalek. 1984. The topography of ability and learning correlations. In Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, vol. 2, pp. 47–103. [Google Scholar]
- Spearman, Charles. 1904. “General intelligence”, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology 15: 201–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spearman, Charles. 1923. The Nature of “Intelligence” and the Principles of Cognition. New York: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Spearman, Charles. 1927. The Abilities of Man. New York: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Stenhouse, David. 1973. The Evolution of Intelligence: A General Theory and Some of Its Implications. New York: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1977. Intelligence, Information Processing, and Analogical Reasoning: The Componential Analysis of Human Abilities. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1981. Intelligence and nonentrenchment. Journal of Educational Psychology 73: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1983. Components of human intelligence. Cognition 15: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1984. A theory of knowledge acquisition in the development of verbal concepts. Developmental Review 4: 113–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1985a. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., ed. 1985b. Human Abilities: An Information-Processing Approach. San Francisco: Freeman. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1987. The psychology of verbal comprehension. In Advances in Instructional Psychology. Edited by Robert Glaser. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 3, pp. 97–151. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1990. Metaphors of Mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2004. Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist 59: 325–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2010. College Admissions for the 21st Century. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2020a. A history of research on intelligence: Part 1: Pre-twentieth-century origins in philosophy. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 18–30. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2020b. A history of research on intelligence: Part 2: Psychological theory, research, and practice in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 31–46. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., ed. 2020c. Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., ed. 2020d. Human Intelligence: An Introduction. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2020e. The augmented theory of successful intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 679–708. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2021a. Adaptive intelligence: Intelligence is not a personal trait but rather a person x task x situation interaction. Journal of Intelligence 9: 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2021b. Adaptive Intelligence: Surviving and Thriving in a World of Uncertainty. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2021c. Meta-intelligence: Understanding, control, and coordination of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. In Intelligenz—Theoretische Grundlagen und Praktische Anwendungen. Heidelberger Jahrbücher Online Band 6. Edited by Rainer Matthias Holm-Hadulla, Joachim Funke and Michael Wink. Heidelberg: University of Heidelberg, pp. 473–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Bathsheva Rifkin. 1979. The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27: 195–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Karin Sternberg. 2017. Measuring scientific reasoning for graduate admissions in psychology and related disciplines. Journal of Intelligence 5: 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Rainbow Project Collaborators. 2006. The Rainbow Project: Enhancing the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical and creative skills. Intelligence 34: 321–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Catherine Nokes, P. Wenzel Geissler, Ruth Prince, Frederick Okatcha, Donald A. Bundy, and Elena L. Grigorenko. 2001. The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya. Intelligence 29: 401–18. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Chak Haang Wong, and Anastasia P. Kreisel. 2021a. Understanding and assessing cultural intelligence: Maximum-performance and typical-performance approaches. Journal of Intelligence 9: 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Chak Haang Wong, and Karin Sternberg. 2019. The relation of tests of scientific reasoning to each other and to tests of fluid intelligence. Journal of Intelligence 7: 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Christine Powell, Patricia McGrane, and Sally Grantham-McGregor. 1997. Effects of a parasitic infection on cognitive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 3: 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Jerry L. Ketron, and Janet S. Powell. 1982. Componential approaches to the training of intelligent performance. In How and How Much Can Intelligence Be Increased? Edited by Douglas K. Detterman and Robert J. Sternberg. Norwood: Ablex, pp. 155–72. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Karin Sternberg, and Rebel J. E. Todhunter. 2017. Measuring reasoning about teaching for graduate admissions in psychology and related disciplines. Journal of Intelligence 5: 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Rebel J. E. Todhunter, Aaron Litvak, and Karin Sternberg. 2020. The relation of scientific creativity and evaluation of scientific impact to scientific reasoning and general intelligence. Journal of Intelligence 8: 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Vlad Glaveanu, Sareh Karami, James C. Kaufman, Shane N. Phillipson, and David D. Preiss. 2021b. Meta-intelligence: Understanding, control, and interactivity between creative, analytical, practical, and wisdom-based approaches in problem solving. Journal of Intelligence 9: 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Saul. 1966. High-speed scanning in human memory. Science 153: 652–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Saul. 1969. Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist 57: 421–57. [Google Scholar]
- Sujan, Harish, and Mita Sujan. 2020. Consumer and marketer intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 871–90. [Google Scholar]
- Thomson, Godfrey H. 1916. A hierarchy without a general factor. British Journal of Psychology 8: 271–81. [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike, Edward L. 1911. Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies. New York: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike, Robert L., and Saul Stein. 1937. An evaluation of the attempts to measure social intelligence. Psychological Bulletin 34: 275–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thurstone, Louis Leon, and Thelma Gwinn Thurstone. 1941. Factorial Studies of Intelligence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Thurstone, Louis Leon. 1924. The Nature of Intelligence. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Thurstone, Louis Leon. 1938. Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vickers, Douglas, and Philip L. Smith. 1986. The Rationale for the Inspection Time Index. Personality and Individual Differences 7: 609–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Visser, Beth A., Michael C. Ashton, and Philip A. Vernon. 2006. Beyond g: Putting multiple intelligence theory of the test. Intelligence 34: 487–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, David. 1940. Non-intellective factor in general intelligence. Psychological Bulletin 37: 444–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, David. 1944. The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler, Daniel D. 1970. Processes in word recognition. Cognitive Psychology 1: 59–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- White, Margaret B., and Alfred E. Hall. 1980. An overview of intelligence testing. Educational Horizons 58: 210–16. [Google Scholar]
- Wicherts, Jelte M., Conor V. Dolan, David J. Hessen, Paul Oosterveld, G. Caroline M. van Baal, Dorret I. Boomsma, and Mark M. Span. 2004. Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect. Intelligence 32: 509–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wissler, Clark. 1901. The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplement 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodley, Michael Anthony, Jante Nijenhuis, Olev Must, and Aasa Must. 2014. Controlling for increased guessing enhances the independence of the Flynn effect from g: The return of the Brand effect. Intelligence 43: 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Problem | Example |
---|---|
We do not know how to find truly basic processes. | Investigators have used psychometric, biological, information-processing, and other analyses to uncover basic processes of intelligence, but the results have not converged convincingly either within or between methods. |
If researchers discovered the basic processes of intelligence, they would not have the empirical operations to identify them truly as basic. | Many processes have been identified, but there has been no compelling demonstration of a method to label them as “basic”. |
Many analyses depend on correlational methods, but correlations do not necessarily imply causation. | The fact that reaction times, for example, correlate with psychometric g does not in any way conclusively show that the processes are basic. |
Researchers often do not know that a given process labeled as corresponding to a reaction time or error rate is actually the, or even a, correspondent process. The label may be wishful thinking. | Jensen (1998) claimed to measure the speed of neuronal conduction through his choice of reaction-time measure, but never showed that his experimental operation actually corresponded to a measure of speed of neuronal conduction. |
Correlations of information-processing measures with scores on psychometric tests tend to be modest, or moderate at best. | Even if the correlations were psychologically meaningful, at their obtained magnitudes, it would not mean that they were indicative of basic processes. |
The information-processing measures were originally designed to provide a causal understanding of intelligence, but their “validity” then often has been determined on the basis of correlations with the indices they were supposed to explain. | Sternberg (1983) set out to “explain” scores on fluid-ability tests, but he then interpreted as meaningful those component scores that showed significant correlations with those same psychometric tests he sought to explain. Moreover, the highest correlation was for the preparation-response component (the regression constant). |
The correlations also are less than meaningful because there is no “true” correlation. The correlation will depend on the population, the task, and the situation in which the task is administered, as well as their interactions (Sternberg 2021c). | Meta-analyses have revealed a wide range of correlations between information-processing tasks and psychometric tests, depending on the population and circumstances of administration. |
Meta-analyses solved none of these problems, because they too, for the most part, have been correlational. An analysis of many correlations is still correlational. | Meta-analyses of inspection time have helped to organize correlations, but they have never found any “true” single correlation, because there is none. At best, one can find a wide range. |
There are too many “winners”. Large numbers of information-processing tasks correlate significantly and meaningfully with psychometric tests. | Most cognitive tasks show some correlation with psychometrically measured intelligence, as follows from Spearman’s (1927) theory. Some correlations, however, are extremely modest or negative (Sternberg et al. 2001). |
Broader theories of intelligence, although in various states of validation, suggest that even if correlations are found with general intelligence, those correlations may apply only to a limited range of what meaningfully could be called “intelligence”. | Tests of emotional intelligence show correlations with a wide range of information-processing tasks and real-world behaviors (Rivers et al. 2020), and we have no basis for excluding this kind of intelligence (among others) from any analysis of “basic” processes. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sternberg, R.J. The Search for the Elusive Basic Processes Underlying Human Intelligence: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020028
Sternberg RJ. The Search for the Elusive Basic Processes Underlying Human Intelligence: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(2):28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020028
Chicago/Turabian StyleSternberg, Robert J. 2022. "The Search for the Elusive Basic Processes Underlying Human Intelligence: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 2: 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020028
APA StyleSternberg, R. J. (2022). The Search for the Elusive Basic Processes Underlying Human Intelligence: Historical and Contemporary Perspectives. Journal of Intelligence, 10(2), 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020028