Next Article in Journal
Evaluation of Systemic Renin and Angiotensin II Levels in Normal Tension Glaucoma
Next Article in Special Issue
Gut-Induced Inflammation during Development May Compromise the Blood-Brain Barrier and Predispose to Autism Spectrum Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Revision Surgery for Postoperative Spondylodiscitis at Cage Level after Posterior Instrumented Fusion in the Lumbar Spine—Anterior Approach Is Not Absolutely Indicated
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Possibility of Intracranial Hypertension in Patients with Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Computed Tomography
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Autism Spectrum Disorder and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Comorbidities Delineate Clinical Phenotypes in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Novel Insights from the Assessment of Psychopathological and Neuropsychological Profiles

1
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
2
IRCCS Stella Maris, Scientific Institute of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Calambrone, 56128 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9(12), 3839; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9123839
Submission received: 1 November 2020 / Revised: 22 November 2020 / Accepted: 24 November 2020 / Published: 26 November 2020

Abstract

:
Although childhood-onset psychiatric disorders are often considered as distinct and separate from each other, they frequently co-occur, with partial overlapping symptomatology. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) commonly co-occur with each other and with other mental disorders, particularly disruptive behavior disorders, oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder (ODD/CD). Whether these associated comorbidities represent a spectrum of distinct clinical phenotypes is matter of research. The aim of our study was to describe the clinical phenotypes of youths with ADHD with and without ASD and/or ODD/CD, based on neuropsychological and psychopathological variables. One-hundred fifty-one participants with ADHD were prospectively recruited and assigned to four clinical groups, and assessed by means of parent-reported questionnaires, the child behavior checklist and the behavior rating inventory of executive functions. The ADHD alone group presented a greater impairment in metacognitive executive functions, ADHD+ASD patients presented higher internalizing problems and deficits in Shifting tasks, and ADHD+ODD/CD subjects presented emotional-behavioral dysregulation. Moreover, ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD individuals exhibited greater internalizing and externalizing problems, and specific neuropsychological impairments in the domains of emotional regulation. Our study supports the need to implement the evaluation of the psychopathological and neuropsychological functioning profiles, and to characterize specific endophenotypes for a finely customized establishment of treatment strategies.

1. Introduction

Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most frequent reasons for consultation in the context of mental health services for minors, causing significant impairment in various life contexts from childhood to adolescence and adulthood [1,2,3]. Despite previous attempts to characterize its multifaceted nature, the considerable heterogeneity of its clinical presentations still remains a major subject of debate among clinicians [1]. One of the aspects of this heterogeneity is the comorbidity between ADHD and other psychiatric disorders in at least 60% of patients, mostly other neurodevelopmental conditions—particularly autism spectrum disorders (ASD), intellectual disabilities, Tourette syndrome, and motor coordination disorders [1,4,5,6,7,8]; and disruptive behavior disorders—such as oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD); but also internalizing disorders—especially anxiety and mood disorders. The coexistence of clinical comorbidities in the context of ADHD represents a serious matter especially in terms of early detection. Consequently, therapeutic interventions are even more challenging to define, with the risk of being less specific and less effective.
The impact of comorbidities in ADHD patients can be substantial. For instance, the severity of ADHD symptomatology is further worsened, in terms of emotional and behavioral problems, when ASD phenotype overlaps along with its adaptive impairment [9,10,11,12]. Particularly, these patients exhibit more severe externalizing behaviors, and greater impairments in verbal working memory [10]. A dual diagnosis of ADHD and ASD is now allowed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Fifth Edition (DSM-5) criteria [13], with rates of co-occurrence of up to 42% [14,15]. On the other hand, ADHD children with comorbid ODD and/or CD are characterized by higher rates of learning difficulties and school problems, including neglect, expulsion, and dropouts from school [16,17], and lower performances in visual–motor integration and visuo-spatial tasks [18]. Additionally, they are more prone, in adulthood, to develop drug abuse, and to be engaged in criminal behaviors and antisocial conduct [19,20].
Recent studies have raised the question of whether ADHD with associated comorbidities is merely characterized by an increased symptoms variety with enhanced impairment severity, or it represents a spectrum of distinct phenotypes [21]. Interestingly, the identification of clinical phenotypes is a developing area of research aimed at best characterizing the multifaceted aspect of neurodevelopmental disorders. Current research is focused on the fine investigation of some dimensional constructs, such as executive function (EF) deficits, which are well known to characterize the neuropsychological profiles of affected individuals [21]. Indeed, studies comparing EF in ADHD with different comorbidities have identified specific executive profiles across different clinical phenotypes. It is still unclear, however, whether these neuropsychological difficulties are either trans-diagnostic or specific to that particular clinical entity.
EF deficits in children with ADHD and/or ASD feature some shared characteristics, as shown by a recent meta-analysis by Craig and colleagues [22], including 26 studies that used different assessment measures of EF. Although the results of such studies are not entirely consistent, the authors identified overlapping impairments in EF profiles—for instance, attention deficits significantly higher both in ADHD alone and ADHD+ASD patients. While performances in working memory and fluency tasks did not differ between the clinical groups, inhibition and cognitive flexibility appeared more impaired in ASD and ADHD+ASD children. Therefore, ADHD+ASD group may show a distinct and more severe dysexecutive profile, characterized by the presence of characteristic deficits of both disorders. Additionally, a more recent study by Berenguer and colleagues [23] assessed EF profiles in children with ADHD, ASD, and ADHD+ASD, comparing them with a control group, by means of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) scale. Results revealed alterations in most BRIEF variables in all three clinical groups, with the exception of inhibit and material organization subscales, which appeared significantly affected only in the two groups with symptoms of ADHD. Moreover, ADHD alone and ADHD+ASD patients showed higher deficits in the working memory, plan/organize and monitor subscales than ASD group.
On the other hand, EF deficits have also been identified in children with ODD/CD, and they partially overlap with those typically found in ADHD patients [24,25]. Some results suggest that the executive dysfunction of children with ADHD and ODD/CD might be milder than that of children with ADHD alone [26]. Contradictory findings showed, however, that EF profiles of children with ADHD+ODD/CD were impaired, especially in terms of inhibition, more slightly [27] or more severely [28], than those of children with ADHD alone. Another study assessing EF by means of different environmentally valuable tools [29] proved that adolescents with ADHD+ODD/CD are less inclined to identify effective strategies and control behavioral responses, when compared to individuals with ODD/CD only. A study by Qian and colleagues [30] compared participants with ADHD and ADHD+ODD/CD to a healthy control group using the behavior rating inventory of executive function—second version (BRIEF-2) [31], showing that the comorbid group got worse scores in inhibit, shift, and emotional control subscales. Interestingly, Hobson et al. [32] confirmed overlapping profiles in metacognitive EF profile, assessed with standardized performance test, for individuals with ADHD and ODD/CD, who share similar difficulties in tolerating delayed rewards, but showed greater impairment in emotional regulation EF in ODD/CD group. Both the studies suggest that children with ADHD and ODD/CD may present a more pronounced impairment in emotional, rather than in cognitive control. From that perspective, a fairly simplistic but clinically useful distinction among two major dimensions of EF, namely, between “cold” and “hot” EF, has been proposed [33]. While the former are more strictly related to metacognitive skills such as working memory, programming, and organization attitudes, the latter refer to processes underlying the affective modulation of behavioral responses, or in other words, the emotional regulation domain.
Only a few studies have investigated EF profiles among young people with ADHD and comorbid ASD and/or ODD/CD. A recent study by Leno et al. [34] aimed at comparing different EF tasks’ performances in three clinical groups by controlling for conduct problems and ADHD symptoms. The authors concluded that only the ADHD+ASD group exhibited reduced inhibition in the go/no-go task, while all three clinical groups demonstrated greater reaction time variability than the control group; similarly, both ADHD+ODD/CD and ADHD+ASD groups showed an increase in impulse responses, whereas no differences could be detected in cognitive flexibility and switch. Finally, a study by Tye and colleagues [35], showed that stronger callous-unemotional traits in children with ASD improve performance in the go/no-go task, highlighting greater skills in inhibition processes.
Based on these findings, we hypothesize that ADHD alone and ADHD with associated comorbidities represent distinct phenotypes, with suitable clusters of psychopathological and neuropsychological features. These psychopathological and neuropsychological indices may be useful not only for differentiating clinical subtypes of patients, but also for identifying specific intervention pathways. To test this hypothesis, the main aim of our preliminary study is to describe the clinical phenotypes of minors with ADHD in its “pure” presentation and with psychiatric comorbidities (ASD and/or ODD/CD), with respect to psychopathological and neuropsychological variables, to highlight similarities and differences among the groups. This study represents one of few literature reports that evaluates clinical phenotypes of children with ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD comorbidity. Furthermore, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study assessing EF in such a cohort through parental reports, by means of BRIEF-2 questionnaire designed to explore EF of youths in daily contexts, related to everyday life behaviors. Furthermore, the BRIEF-2 inventory finely captures the multidimensional structure of EF, and particularly the distinction between cold (metacognition) and hot (emotional regulation) dimensions.
Therefore, we assessed psychopathological profiles using a dimensional approach for identifying phenotypic domains, representing meaningful variations across multiple domains of behaviors, with two broader dimensions, internalizing and externalizing behaviors [36,37,38].

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Participants and Diagnostic Procedures

Our study prospectively included 151 drug-naïve participants (137 boys, age range 6–18 years old, mean age 9.51 ± 2.64 years) recruited from March 2019 to December 2019 in the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry of our third level hospital with a nation-wide catchment. The investigation was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki.
The diagnoses, based on DSM-5 diagnostic criteria, were made using historical information, and a structured clinical interview, the Italian version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) [39], administered by child psychiatry trainees under the supervision of the senior child psychiatrist. Diagnoses were finally confirmed by consensus of a multidisciplinary board.
The Italian version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) [40] was also routinely administered to all patients to assess the intellectual functioning. Moreover, the Italian versions of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Second Edition (ADOS-2) [41] and of the Autism Diagnostic Interview—revised (ADI-R) [42] were administered only in those patients for whom an initial diagnosis of ASD according to DSM-5 criteria was discussed. Thus, they were used to assist clinicians in the diagnostic procedure and to either confirm or to question the ASD diagnoses by final consensus of our multidisciplinary team, as routinely performed in the Department according to international guidelines. All assessments were conducted by highly experienced psychologists specialized in child neuropsychological evaluations.

2.2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Patients were included whether they received a diagnosis of ADHD with or without comorbid ASD, ODD/CD, or both. Other associated psychiatric conditions, such as anxiety and mood disorders and learning disabilities, were accepted but not primarily considered for analytical purposes. Exclusion criteria were as follows: a total WISC-IV IQ score lower than 70 points; younger than 6 years old or older than 18 years old; use of psychoactive medications; neurologic impairments or neurodegenerative conditions. All recruited patients met inclusion criteria and agreed to participate in the study after informed consent was obtained by parents.

2.3. Clinical Groups

We identified four clinical groups in our sample: ADHD alone group, including 64 participants (12.5% girls, mean age 10.02 ± 2.49 years); comorbid ADHD+ASD group (hereinafter referred to as ADHD+ASD group), including 19 participants (5.26% girls, mean age 9.58 ± 2.69 years) who did not fulfill ODD/CD diagnostic criteria; comorbid ADHD+ODD/CD group (hereinafter referred to as ADHD+ODD/CD group), including 43 participants (9.3% girls, mean age 9.37 ± 2.95 years) who did not fulfill ASD diagnostic criteria; comorbid ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (hereinafter referred to as ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group), including 25 participants (4% girls, mean age 8.40 ± 2.24 years) who fulfilled diagnostic criteria for both ODD/CD and ASD. These groups could present other associated psychiatric conditions, i.e., anxiety and mood disorders or learning disabilities.

2.4. Measures

All patients were assessed with the Italian version of the child behavior checklist for ages 6 to 18 years (CBCL-6/18) [43], a 118-item scale, completed by parents, with 8 different syndromes scales, a total problem score, and two broad-band scores designated as internalizing problems and externalizing problems. The reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s alpha) were 0.82, 0.81, and 0.82, respectively [43].
The parents of all patients were asked to fill in the Italian version of the BRIEF-2 inventory [31]. The instrument is the updated version of the BRIEF questionnaire and provides a structured assessment of EF behaviors in everyday life environments, allowing the identification of helpful clinical manifestations in different contexts, i.e., home and school. Designed for ages 5 to 18, BRIEF-2 is available in three versions (parent-report, teacher-report, and self-report). In this study, the parent-reported version was used, which includes 63 items assessing the frequency of occurrence of common behaviors in everyday life settings. BRIEF-2 is a multidimensional measurement that investigates nine factors related to specific EFs: inhibit, self-monitor, shift, emotional control, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task monitor, organization of materials. Three composite scales are also identifiable: behavioral regulation index (BRI) (including inhibit and self-monitor), emotional regulation index (ERI) (shift and emotional control), and cognitive regulation index (CRI) (initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task monitor, organization of materials). A global executive composite (GEC) score was also computed as the sum of the three composite indexes.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed by means of MatLab® (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and RStudio® (RStudio Inc., Boston, MA, USA) software. For each clinical variable with a continuous distribution, outliers were defined as observations lying outside the range between (first quartile − 1.5 * interquartile range) and (third quartile + 1.5 * interquartile range) and removed. For each BRIEF-2 subscale-related variable, observations were removed if the corresponding values for either the infrequency or the inconsistency scale were higher than the 99th percentile of normalized data.
The χ2 test was used to detect significant differences (p-value < 0.05) for clinical and demographic categorial variables, such as gender and clinical comorbidities. When more than 20% of observations had expected frequencies less than 5, Fisher’s exact test was performed. Analyses of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to assess significant differences (p-value < 0.05) between clinical and demographic variables with continuous distribution. Homogeneity of variances across groups was checked using Levene’s test before applying ANOVA; all variables showed equal variances in the different groups (Levene’s test: p-value > 0.05), so that variance homogeneity assumption was reliably satisfied. A Tukey post-hoc test was utilized whenever the ANOVA led to a statistically significant result in order to retrieve significant comparisons between variables.
Finally, we performed a principal component analysis (PCA) with Promax oblique rotation to empirically derive variables that were subsequently compared between clinical groups. All BRIEF-2 and CBCL-6/18 subscale variables were used to conduct the PCA—specifically, the inhibit, self-monitor, emotional control, shift, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task-monitor, and organization of materials scales of former; and the anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, social problems, somatic complaints and thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking behavior, and aggressive behavior symptoms scales of the latter. A scree-plot was used to determine the appropriate number of components. Thus, a number (greater than 1) of components was extracted, and component scores for each participant were computed using regression approach. An ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test was then conducted to assess significant differences between groups in the continuous data of the three components identified through the PCA.

3. Results

3.1. Clinical and Demographic Characteristics

As shown in Table 1, the four clinical groups did not differ in terms of age or gender. No significant difference emerged in WISC-IV scores, nor in full-scale intelligence quotient, nor in its four composite scores; nonetheless, all four groups scored appreciably lower in working memory and processing speed composites than the verbal comprehension and perceptual reasoning composites. Clinical comorbidities are also reported; as expected, mood disorders were more expressed in the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group than the ADHD group only, while anxiety disorders were more prevalent in the ADHD+ASD group than the ADHD only group.

3.2. Child Behavior Checklist

Only one outlier was removed from the analysis in the somatic complaints subscale of the CBCL questionnaire, belonging to the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group. No outliers were identified and removed based on interquartile range for all the other CBCL variables. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups and ANOVA statistics in the CBCL are illustrated in Figure 1A–C and Table 2. Significant differences were detected in the externalizing problems scale (Figure 1A) between ADHD+ASD (59.72 ± 8.10) and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD (67.77 ± 9.62) groups, but no significant differences emerged for the internalizing and the total problems scales, though the mean scores of all four groups exceeded the clinical cut-offs for both.
As for the syndromes scale (Figure 1B), a significant difference was found in the attention problems scale between ADHD alone (70.73 ± 8.44) and ADHD+ODD/CD groups (66.25 ± 8.55); in the rule-breaking behaviors scale the ADHD+ODD/CD group (62.97 ± 7.57) scored significantly higher than the ADHD+ASD group (56.78 ± 5.68).
As for the DSM-oriented scales (Figure 1C), the ADHD+ODD/CD group scored significantly higher (65.67 ± 8.48) than the ADHD+ASD group (59.63 ± 7.68) in the oppositional-defiant problems scale, and significantly higher (64.39 ± 8.29) than the ADHD+ASD group (56.52 ± 5.89) in the conduct problems scale, as did the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (65.09 ± 7.52).
Finally, significant differences emerged in the sluggish cognitive tempo scale between ADHD alone (62.72 ± 9.13) and ADHD+ODD/CD groups (57.88 ± 7.26), and in the obsessive-compulsive problems scale between ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD (64.13 ± 10.63), and both ADHD alone (56.69 ± 6.91) and ADHD+ODD/CD groups (56.33 ± 7.02).

3.3. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function

No outliers were identified and removed based on BRIEF-2 variables’ interquartile ranges. Profiles of the scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the BRIEF-2 scales and ANOVA statistics are shown in Figure 2A,B and Table 3. As for the BRIEF-2 general indexes (Figure 2A), the only significant difference was found for the CRI, with higher scores obtained by ADHD alone group (70.41 ± 10.16) compared to ADHD+ODD/CD group (63.97 ± 11.41). It may be noticed that, although differences did not reach statistical significance, ODD/CD and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD groups scored the highest in both BRI and ERI, exceeding cut-off values. Similarly, although differences among groups were not significant, all four groups presented elevated scores, exceeding the cut-off values, in the GEC scale.
As for the BRIEF-2 subscales (Figure 2B), inhibition, self-monitor, and task monitor subscales did not differentiate the four groups. The ADHD alone group scored significantly higher than the other three groups in working memory, plan/organize, and organization of materials subscales. The initiate subscale score was significantly higher in the ADHD alone group, compared to the ADHD+ODD/CD group, but not compared to the other two groups. The ADHD+ODD/CD group scored significantly higher than the ADHD alone in the emotional control scale, while the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group only approached statistical significance.

3.4. Principal Component Analysis

The PCA we performed led to a three-component solution explaining 59% of variance. As shown in Table 4, the first rotated component (RC1) included the cognitive regulation-related subscales (initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task-monitor, and organization of Materials) of the BRIEF-2 and the attention problems of the CBCL. The second rotated component (RC2) included the shift subscale of the BRIEF-2 and the internalizing problems-related subscales of the CBCL (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, social problems, somatic complaints, and thought problems). The third rotated component (RC3) included the behavioral regulation-related subscales (inhibit and self-monitor) and the emotional control subscale of the BRIEF-2, and the externalizing problems-related subscales of the CBCL (rule-breaking behavior and aggressive behavior). High scores represent greater impairment impairments in all variables included in the different components.
As illustrated in Figure 3, the ADHD alone group scored the highest in the RC1 component and significantly differed from the ADHD+ODD/CD and the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD groups. As for the RC2 component, a significant difference emerged between the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD and the ADHD+ODD/CD groups, the former scoring higher than the latter. On the contrary, the ADHD+ODD/CD group scored the highest in the RC3 component, significantly differing from the ADHD alone and the ADHD+ASD groups, while the difference approached statistical significance between the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD and the ADHD groups.

4. Discussion

The main aim of the present study was to identify possible phenotypes of patients with ADHD, ASD, and ODD/CD, based on EF and psychopathological domains, in order to better characterize their diagnostic frameworks. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study assessing EF through parental reports, by means of the BRIEF-2 questionnaire, in a clinical sample of youth with comorbid conditions.
The BRIEF-2 questionnaire provides a structured assessment of EF behaviors in everyday life environments, by means of a multi-comprehensive assessment of different aspects of child behavior. A large number of world-wide clinical trials and case-control studies are indeed available to derive specific normative data based on age and gender. For these reasons, we chose the BRIEF-2 questionnaire as a valuable instrument for the assessment of EF in our sample.
The four groups in our sample were similar in terms of age, gender, and cognitive abilities. Our analyses demonstrated in the ADHD alone group a specific pattern of impairment in attentive and metacognitive domains, as revealed by CBCL questionnaire and BRIEF-2 inventory (cognitive regulation, working memory, plan/organize, organization of materials, initiate), with greater impairment in the domains of the so-called “cold” EF. These features appear more evident in the ADHD alone group when compared to those associated with comorbid conditions, which seems to support the executive dysfunction theory of ADHD [25,44,45]. Other features (i.e., inhibition, self-monitoring, shift, and task monitoring) do not seem to be affected by comorbidities, as they are indifferently present both in ADHD alone and when it is associated with ODD/CD and/or ASD.
As expected, when ADHD is comorbid with ODD/CD, it is associated not only with attention problems (but not with the sluggish cognitive tempo subscale), but also with rule breaking behavior [16]. Externalizing problems score is not significantly higher in ADHD+ODD/CD compared to ADHD alone and ADHD+ASD groups, and only the association ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD presents a significantly higher externalizing problems score. In contrast, the ADHD+ASD condition is prevalently associated with higher obsessive-compulsive traits and sluggish cognitive tempo, lower scores in rule breaking behaviors and conduct problems, and when associated with ODD/CD, lower shift abilities, compared to ODD/CD without ASD.
The PCA supports these findings. Indeed, the ADHD alone group is associated with the attention problems of CBCL, and with the cognitive regulation-related subscales (initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task-monitor, and organization of materials) of BRIEF-2. The triple comorbidity ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD is associated with both RC2 and RC3. However, the RC2 (internalizing problems-related subscales of CBCL, anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, social problems, somatic complaints, and thought problems, and the shift subscale of BRIEF-2) is prevalently associated with both ADHD+ASD and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD, with a prevalent contribution of the ASD component. On the other hand, the RC3 (externalizing problems-related subscales of the CBCL; rule-breaking behavior and aggressive behavior, inhibit, and self-monitor; and the emotional control subscale of the BRIEF-2) is prevalently associated with the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD and ADHD+ODD/CD, with a prevalent contribution of the ODD/CD component.
While the profile of the ADHD+ODD/CD is consistent with previous findings regarding the increased severity of both externalizing symptoms and emotional dysregulation [46,47,48], the profile of ADHD participants with comorbid ASD is more difficult to interpret, and consistent with the notion that the ADHD+ASD phenotype does not simply reflect the sum of the symptoms of the two disorders [9,49]. Our results suggest that the presence of ASD traits modulates the severity of the externalizing symptoms, further increasing aggressive and shattering behaviors. Indeed, externalizing symptoms are lowest when ASD is associated with ADHD, even compared to ADHD alone, and are highest when ASD is associated with both ADHD and ODD/CD, thereby worsening the externalizing impact of this association. However, in line with previous studies [50], the principal component analysis we performed suggests that the ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD association further increases internalizing problems (anxiety, mood symptoms, and social problems). Consistently, Cooper at al. (2014) correlated the presence of autistic traits in children with ADHD with a greater severity of behavioral symptoms and anxiety symptoms.
In summary, our study highlighted that, compared to ADHD alone, comorbid ODD/CD and ASD worsen externalizing and internalizing symptoms’ severity, and the neuropsychological profile, but with different patterns. ADHD alone appears to be characterized by a more specific impairment of metacognitive (“cold”) functioning and attentive capacities. On the other hand, comorbid ODD/CD seems more significantly related to “hot” executive dysfunctions and emotional self-regulation. Less consistent findings have been found regarding the implications of ASD comorbidity, namely, a more selective impairment in shifting, possibly related to their low cognitive flexibility.
Our data are in line with novel approaches to the classical categorical nosographic entities, using dimensional psychopathological and neuropsychological tools, in a transdiagnostic fashion, possibly with supporting evidence from biological markers, to understand the common patterns of neural dysfunction that link apparently different disorders, including ADHD and ASD [51]. Within this framework, defining a new approach to understanding mental illness, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) project [52] presented mixed dimensional abnormalities based on brain circuits and connectivity, which are conceptualized as underlying dysfunctions that can contribute to clinically diverging mental disorders, such as ADHD and ASD, as recently shown [53,54]. Our study does not provide biological markers, and dimensional and psychopathological tools were applied to behavioral syndromes according to the “old” DSM-5 categories. This “false assumption” [55] has been limited so far to the extension of this novel approach to child and adolescent psychiatry [56]. However, the intent of RDoC is not to become a new diagnostic classification system, but it may be useful for refining current diagnostic classifications through the lens of fundamental components, i.e., executive functions, that cut across diagnoses [56]. This approach may be also functional for understand the heterogeneity within a single DSM disorder, based on neuropsychological, psychopathological, or temperamental dimensions.
Our study shows, however, a number of limitations. Importantly, a marked discrepancy in the sizes of the four groups, particularly between the ADHD alone group and those associated with comorbid ASD. Furthermore, these groups were not recruited through an age-matching protocol design, though no significant difference in age emerged when comparing them by means of statistical tests. Additionally, we did not include a control group of healthy children. Therefore, future studies with larger age- and sex-matched samples and healthy controls could be carried out to support our results.
Moreover, an ecological parent report-based measure of EF was used in the present study. It should be noted that low correlations have been reported between performance measures and behavioral rating scales of EF in ADHD and ASD research; however, task-based measures capture only limited facets of the EF system in a limited temporal span, while neglecting the integrated multidimensional decisional process related to and based on a priority-based strategic analysis performed by the individual, which is what daily life situations often require. From this perspective, ecological measures are important to foresee the severity of dysfunction in daily life situations experienced by children at home and school, which are important settings wherein parents and teachers assessing the essential expressions of executive functioning may provide a valuable amount of information which could help clinicians in their measurements.
Another limitation of our study is that it did not include the “limited prosocial emotion” specifier. Further research could be performed, including using more participants showing high levels of callous-unemotional traits, in order to possibly identify further phenotypes of ADHD with comorbid disruptive behavior disorders. Similarly, we could not ascertain the presence of subthreshold symptoms within the different clinical domains explored by the K-SADS interview and the ADOS-2 observation; subthreshold symptoms could be usefully considered in a multidimensional perspective in future studies. Unfortunately, we could not provide teacher-rated measures of ADHD symptoms to compare parent reports, though educational observations were performed for all patients. Finally, we did not correct our tests for multiple comparisons, as all comparisons we performed would not survive such correction with a statistical threshold below 0.001. Nonetheless, given the exploratory nature of our study and the relatively limited sample size, we believed it more convenient to keep all statistically significant comparisons while waiting for future larger studies aimed at corroborating—or invalidating—our results.

5. Conclusions

Our study provides a further contribution to a better understanding of the clinical manifestations of ADHD and comorbidities with ASD and/or ODD/CD, and suggests that specific comorbidities might help in the selection of the treatment strategies. Our findings support the need to associate, within the clinical assessment and diagnostic framework, the evaluation of psychiatric comorbidities and neuropsychological functioning, with more thoroughly characterized specific clinical phenotypes, i.e., by means of standardized neuropsychological tests in clinical settings.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.S., C.C., S.B., P.C., P.F., E.I., A.N., C.P., F.R., A.T., E.V., V.V., A.M., and G.M.; methodology, G.S., C.C., S.B., P.C., P.F., E.I., A.N., C.P., F.R., A.T., E.V., V.V., A.M., and G.M.; writing—original draft preparation, G.S., C.C., P.C., A.M., and G.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We wish to acknowledge all the patients, family members, and research staff from all the units that participated in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

Masi has received institutional research grants from Lundbeck and Humana, was in an advisory board for Angelini, and has been speaker for Angelini, FB Health, Janssen, Lundbeck, and Otsuka. All the other authors do not have conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Biederman, J.; Faraone, S.V.; Spencer, T.; Wilens, T.; Norman, D.; Lapey, K.A.; Mick, E.; Lehman, B.K.; Doyle, A. Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity, cognition, and psychosocial functioning in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 1993, 150, 1792–1798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Klein, R.G.; Mannuzza, S.; Ramos Olazagasti, M.A.; Roizen, E.; Hutchison, J.A.; Lashua, E.C.; Castellanos, F.X. Clinical and functional outcome of childhood attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder 33 years later. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2012, 69, 1295–1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Mrug, S.; Brooke, B.S.; Hoza, B.; Gerdes, A.C.; Hinshaw, S.P.; Hechtman, L.; Arnold, L.E. Peer rejection and friendships in children with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: Contributions to long-term outcomes. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2012, 40, 1013–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Barkley, R.; Fischer, M.; Edelbrock, C.S.; Smallish, L. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1990, 29, 546–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Masi, G.; Mucci, M.; Pfanner, C.; Berloffa, S.; Magazù, A.; Perugi, G. Developmental pathways for different subtypes of early-onset bipolarity in youths. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2012, 73, 1335–1341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Green, J.L.; Sciberras, E.; Anderson, V.; Efron, D.; Rinehart, N. Association between autism symptoms and functioning in children with ADHD. Arch. Dis. Child. 2016, 101, 922–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Jensen, C.M.; Steinhausen, H.C. Comorbid mental disorders in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a large nationwide study. ADHD Atten. Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 2015, 7, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gnanavel, S.; Sharma, P.; Kaushal, P.; Hussain, S. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbidity: A review of literature. World J. Clin. Cases 2019, 7, 2420–2426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Scandurra, V.; Emberti Gialloreti, L.; Barbanera, F.; Scordo, M.R.; Pierini, A.; Canitano, R. Neurodevelopmental Disorders and Adaptive Functions: A Study of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and/or Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Cooper, M.; Martin, J.; Langley, K.; Hamshere, M.; Thapar, A. Autistic traits in children with ADHD index clinical and cognitive problems. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2014, 23, 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Maskey, M.; Warnell, F.; Parr, J.R.; Le Couteur, A.; McConachie, H. Emotional and behavioural problems in children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 851–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Yamawaki, K.; Ishitsuka, K.; Suyama, S.; Suzumura, S.; Yamashita, H.; Kanba, S. Clinical characteristics of boys with comorbid autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr. Int. 2020, 62, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; CBS Publishers: New Delhi, India, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  14. Stevens, M.C.; Gaynor, A.; Bessette, K.L.; Pearlson, G.D. A preliminary study of the effects of working memory training on brain function. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 10, 387–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Sprenger, L.; Bühler, E.; Poustka, L.; Bach, C.; Heinzel-Gutenbrunner, M.; Kamp-Becker, I.; Bachmann, C. Impact of ADHD symptoms on autism spectrum disorder symptom severity. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 3545–3552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Atherton, O.E.; Lawson, K.M.; Ferrer, E.; Robins, R.W. The role of effortful control in the development of ADHD, ODD, and CD symptoms. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Erskine, H.E.; Norman, R.E.; Ferrari, A.J.; Chan, G.C.K.; Copeland, W.E.; Whiteford, H.A.; Scott, J.G. Long-Term Outcomes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Conduct Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 841–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Moffitt, T.E.; Silva, P.A. Self-Reported Delinquency, neuropsychological deficit, and history of attention deficit disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1988, 16, 553–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Barkley, R.; Fischer, M.; Smallish, L.; Fletcher, K. Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: Antisocial activities and drug use. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2004, 45, 195–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sibley, M.H.; Waxmonsky, J.G.; Robb, J.A.; Pelham, W.E. Implications of Changes for the Field: ADHD. J. Learn. Disabil. 2013, 46, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Ter-Stepanian, M.; Grizenko, N.; Cornish, K.; Talwar, V.; Mbekou, V.; Schmitz, N.; Joober, R. Attention and Executive Function in Children Diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Comorbid Disorders. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 26, 21–30. [Google Scholar]
  22. Craig, F.; Margari, F.; Legrottaglie, A.R.; Palumbi, R.; de Giambattista, C.; Margari, L. A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 1191–1202. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Berenguer, C.; Roselló, B.; Colomer, C.; Baixauli, I.; Miranda, A. Children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Relationships between symptoms and executive function, theory of mind, and behavioral problems. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 83, 260–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Antonini, T.N.; Becker, S.P.; Tamm, L.; Epstein, J.N. Hot and Cool Executive Functions in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Comorbid Oppositional Defiant Disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2015, 21, 584–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Glenn, A.L.; Remmel, R.J.; Ong, M.Y.; Lim, N.S.J.; Ang, R.P.; Threadgill, A.H.; Ryerson, N.; Raine, A.; Fung, D.; Ooi, Y.P. Neurocognitive characteristics of youth with noncomorbid and comorbid forms of conduct disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 2017, 77, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Schachar, R.; Mota, V.L.; Logan, G.D.; Tannock, R.; Klim, P. Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2000, 28, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shuai, L.; Wang, Y. Executive function characteristic in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder comorbid learning disabilities. Beijing Da Xue Xue Bao 2007, 39, 526–530. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Van der Meere, J.; Marzocchi, G.M.; De Meo, T. Response inhibition and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without oppositional defiant disorder screened from a community sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005, 28, 459–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Clark, C.; Prior, M.; Kinsella, G.J. Do executive function deficits differentiate between adolescents with ADHD and Oppositional Defiant/Conduct Disorder? A neuropsychological study using the Six Elements Test and Hayling Sentence Completion Test. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2000, 28, 403–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Qian, Y.; Shuai, L.; Cao, Q.; Chan, R.C.K.; Wang, Y. Do executive function deficits differentiate between children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and ADHD comorbid with oppositional defiant disorder? A cross-cultural study using performance-based tests and the behavior rating inventory. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2010, 24, 793–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Guy, S.C.; Kenworthy, L.; Baron, I.S. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000, 6, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hobson, C.W.; Scott, S.; Rubia, K. Investigation of cool and hot executive function in ODD/CD independently of ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2011, 52, 1035–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Zelazo, P.D.; Carlson, S.M. Hot and Cool Executive Function in Childhood and Adolescence: Development and Plasticity. Child Dev. Perspect. 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Carter Leno, V.; Chandler, S.; White, P.; Pickles, A.; Baird, G.; Hobson, C.; Smith, A.B.; Charman, T.; Rubia, K.; Simonoff, E. Testing the specificity of executive functioning impairments in adolescents with ADHD, ODD/CD and ASD. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2018, 27, 899–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Tye, C.; Bedford, R.; Asherson, P.; Ashwood, K.L.; Azadi, B.; Bolton, P.; McLoughlin, G. Callous-unemotional traits moderate executive function in children with ASD and ADHD: A pilot event-related potential study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 26, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hudziak, J.J.; Achenbach, T.M.; Althoff, R.R.; Pine, D.S. A dimensional approach to developmental psychopathology. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res 2007, 16, 16–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Achenbach, T.M.; Ivanova, M.Y.; Rescorla, L.A.; Turner, L.V.; Althoff, R.R. Internalizing/Externalizing Problems: Review and Recommendations for Clinical and Research Applications. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 647–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Van Dam, N.T.; O’Connor, D.; Marcelle, E.T.; Ho, E.J.; Cameron Craddock, R.; Tobe, R.H.; Gabbay, V.; Hudziak, J.J.; Xavier Castellanos, F.; Leventhal, B.L.; et al. Data-Driven Phenotypic Categorization for Neurobiological Analyses: Beyond DSM-5 Labels. Biol. Psychiatry 2017, 81, 484–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Kaufman, J.; Birmaher, B.; Brent, D.; Rao, U.; Flynn, C.; Moreci, P.; Williamson, D.; Ryan, N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 980–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wechsler, D. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2004; ISBN 9780470135389. [Google Scholar]
  41. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; Di Lavore, P.; Risi, S.; Gotham, K.; Bishop, S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Second Edition (ADOS-2). Available online: https://www.wpspublish.com/ados-2-autism-diagnostic-observation-schedule-second-edition (accessed on 22 February 2020).
  42. Rutter, M.; LeCouteur, A.; Lord, C. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). Available online: https://www.wpspublish.com/adi-r-autism-diagnostic-interview-revised (accessed on 22 February 2020).
  43. Achenbach, T.; Rescorla, L. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles: An Integrated System of Multi-Informant Assessment; University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Retzlaff, P.D.; Espy, K.A. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 249–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Willcutt, E.G.; Doyle, A.E.; Nigg, J.T.; Faraone, S.V.; Pennington, B.F. Validity of the Executive Function Theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Meta-Analytic Review. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1336–1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Stringaris, A.; Goodman, R. Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: Irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2009, 48, 404–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Thapar, A.; Harrington, R.; McGuffin, P. Examining the comorbidity of ADHD-related behaviours and conduct problems using a twin study design. Br. J. Psychiatry 2001, 179, 224–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Drechsler, R.; Zulauf Logoz, M.; Walitza, S.; Steinhausen, H.C. The Relations Between Temperament, Character, and Executive Functions in Children With ADHD and Clinical Controls. J. Atten. Disord. 2018, 22, 764–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Craig, F.; Lamanna, A.L.; Margari, F.; Matera, E.; Simone, M.; Margari, L. Overlap Between Autism Spectrum Disorders and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Searching for Distinctive/Common Clinical Features. Autism Res. 2015, 8, 328–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Mansour, R.; Dovi, A.T.; Lane, D.M.; Loveland, K.A.; Pearson, D.A. ADHD severity as it relates to comorbid psychiatric symptomatology in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Res. Dev. Disabil. 2017, 60, 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Insel, T.R. The nimh research domain criteria (RDOC) project: Precision medicine for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 2014, 171, 395–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Cuthbert, B.N. The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry 2014, 13, 28–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Di Martino, A.; Zuo, X.N.; Kelly, C.; Grzadzinski, R.; Mennes, M.; Schvarcz, A.; Rodman, J.; Lord, C.; Castellanos, F.X.; Milham, M.P. Shared and distinct intrinsic functional network centrality in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 74, 623–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Kernbach, J.M.; Satterthwaite, T.D.; Bassett, D.S.; Smallwood, J.; Margulies, D.; Krall, S.; Shaw, P.; Varoquaux, G.; Thirion, B.; Konrad, K.; et al. Shared endo-phenotypes of default mode dsfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 2018, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Beauchaine, T.P.; Hinshaw, S.P. RDoC and Psychopathology among Youth: Misplaced Assumptions and an Agenda for Future Research. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2020, 49, 322–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Garvey, M.; Avenevoli, S.; Anderson, K. The National Institute of Mental Health Research Domain Criteria and Clinical Research in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the child behavior checklist questionnaire are illustrated: (A) problems scales, (B) syndromes scales, and (C) DSM-oriented scales are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Graphs represent means with standard deviation bars. * p-values < 0.05, ** p-values < 0.01.
Figure 1. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the child behavior checklist questionnaire are illustrated: (A) problems scales, (B) syndromes scales, and (C) DSM-oriented scales are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Graphs represent means with standard deviation bars. * p-values < 0.05, ** p-values < 0.01.
Jcm 09 03839 g001
Figure 2. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the brief rating inventory of executive functions questionnaire are illustrated: (A) global indexes and (B) specific subscales are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Graphs represent means with standard deviation bars. * p-values < 0.05.
Figure 2. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the brief rating inventory of executive functions questionnaire are illustrated: (A) global indexes and (B) specific subscales are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Graphs represent means with standard deviation bars. * p-values < 0.05.
Jcm 09 03839 g002
Figure 3. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the three components of the principal component analysis, as described in detail in the main text, are illustrated here: (left) RC1, (middle) RC2, and (right) RC3 are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Boxplots represent medians and first and third quartiles with minimum/maximum bars. * p-values < 0.05, ** p-values < 0.01. Abbreviations: RC1 = first rotated component; RC2 = second rotated component; RC3 = third rotated component.
Figure 3. Scores obtained by the four clinical groups in the three components of the principal component analysis, as described in detail in the main text, are illustrated here: (left) RC1, (middle) RC2, and (right) RC3 are compared between ADHD alone group (blue bars), ADHD+ASD group (red bars), ADHD+ODD/CD group (yellow bars), and ADHD+ASD+ODD/CD group (purple bars). Boxplots represent medians and first and third quartiles with minimum/maximum bars. * p-values < 0.05, ** p-values < 0.01. Abbreviations: RC1 = first rotated component; RC2 = second rotated component; RC3 = third rotated component.
Jcm 09 03839 g003
Table 1. Demographic and clinical data.
Table 1. Demographic and clinical data.
ADHDADHD+ASDADHD+ODD/CDADHD+ASD+ODD/CDp-Values
Subjects a64 (42.38)19 (12.58)43 (28.48)25 (16.56)
Age b10.02 ± 2.499.58 ± 2.699.37 ± 2.958.40 ± 2.240.0760
Adolescentsa19 (29.69)6 (31.58)10 (23.26)4 (16)0.5238
Gender a8 (12.5)1 (5.26)4 (9.3)1 (4)0.6998
WISC-IV
VCIb104.89 ± 17.41104.76 ± 17104.5 ± 14.61107.32 ± 19.240.9440
PRIb99.11 ± 12.76104.76 ± 17.07103.82 ± 18.17107.77 ± 16.190.2860
WMIb87.07 ± 13.3888.07 ± 7.3288.95 ± 11.5390.33 ± 17.450.8710
PSIb83.56 ± 15.2883.73 ± 13.9789.47 ± 18.4683.1 ± 14.890.5470
FSIQ or GAI b93 ± 14.9892.69 ± 1796.86 ± 16.0598.94 ± 18.060.5440
Comorbidities
Mood Disa8 (12.50) *7 (36.84)14 (32.56)13 (52.00) *0.0011 *
Anxiety Disa7 (10.94) *8 (42.11) *7 (16.28)8 (32.00)0.0087 *
Data are presented either as (a) number (percentage) for dichotomous variables or (b) mean ± standard deviation for continuous variables. * p-values < 0.05. Abbreviations: VCI = verbal comprehension index; PRI = perceptual reasoning index; WMI = working memory index; PSI = processing speed index; FSIQ = full-scale intelligence quotient; GAI = general ability index; Dis = disorders.
Table 2. CBCL-6/18 scales.
Table 2. CBCL-6/18 scales.
CBCL – 6/18ADHDADHD+ASDADHD+ODD/CDADHD+ASD+ODD/CDF Valuep-Values
Internalizing P62.47 ± 8.5961.37 ± 11.6259.95 ± 10.0962.61 ± 12.750.6120.608
Externalizing P63.13 ± 9.2859.72 ± 8.1065.51 ± 8.6967.77 ± 9.633.2220.025 *
Total Problems65.89 ± 7.2465.22 ± 6.7365.49 ± 8.0968.45 ± 8.180.8960.445
Anxious/Dep62.09 ± 8.0961.63 ± 9.6260.88 ± 7.8463.41 ± 9.720.4550.714
Withdrawn/Dep63.19 ± 9.7366.11 ± 10.1961.56 ± 7.3765.35 ± 12.071.3610.257
Somatic C57.90 ± 6.8156.37 ± 8.1755.81 ± 5.9958.48 ± 8.161.1210.343
Social Problems62.78 ± 7.1464.84 ± 8.5362.59 ± 7.6667.09 ± 7.882.2390.086
Thought P60.63 ± 7.9163.16 ± 9.5960.84 ± 7.4764.87 ± 9.791.8140.147
Attention P70.73 ± 8.4567.53 ± 8.5966.26 ± 8.5567.10 ± 7.392.7860.043 *
Rule-Breaking B61.28 ± 7.5156.79 ± 5.6862.98 ± 7.5762.36 ± 7.913.2520.024 *
Aggressive B64.78 ± 10.1661.16 ± 8.8565.93 ± 10.4667.61 ± 12.351.4480.231
DP Index197.94 ± 20.62190.32 ± 19.90192.93 ± 23.40198.64 ± 26.560.9300.428
Affective P66.05 ± 7.0263.63 ± 7.5462.70 ± 8.1866.30 ± 10.621.8850.135
Anxious P63.25 ± 7.2063.58 ± 7.9962.12 ± 7.5464.04 ± 9.090.3800.767
Somatic P55.60 ± 6.4753.95 ± 6.8354.72 ± 5.7954.78 ± 7.150.3890.761
ADHD P69.09 ± 7.6265.06 ± 8.2467.02 ± 7.4866.22 ± 6.651.8700.137
ODD P62.63 ± 7.4759.63 ± 7.6865.67 ± 8.4865.43 ± 8.173.3160.022 *
Conduct P61.63 ± 7.8656.53 ± 5.8964.40 ± 8.2965.09 ± 7.585.6930.001 **
SCT61.94 ± 7.8662.72 ± 9.1357.88 ± 7.2759.30 ± 7.882.9220.036 *
OCD P56.69 ± 6.9159.71 ± 8.8256.33 ± 7.0264.14 ± 10.635.9520.001 **
PTSD P65.40 ± 7.8565.83 ± 6.4464.33 ± 7.5667.32 ± 8.440.7410.529
Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation. * p-values < 0.05; ** p-values < 0.01. Abbreviations: B = behaviors; C = complaints; Dep = depressed; DP = dysregulation profile; OCD = obsessive compulsive disorder; P = problems; PTSD = post-traumatic stress disorder; SCT = sluggish cognitive tempo.
Table 3. BRIEF-2 scales.
Table 3. BRIEF-2 scales.
BRIEF-2ADHDADHD+ASDADHD+ODD/CDADHD+ASD+ODD/CDF Valuep-Values
ERI63.92 ± 12.9564.00 ± 11.6065.16 ± 11.0469.22 ± 13.101.0860.358
BRI66.40 ± 13.5163.76 ± 11.5470.82 ± 11.5368.00 ± 11.931.5620.201
CRI70.42 ± 10.1664.88 ± 10.3063.97 ± 11.4165.57 ± 10.423.5510.016 *
GEC70.40 ± 11.0067.59 ± 10.4768.29 ± 11.7369.22 ± 12.170.4260.735
Inhibition66.23 ± 12.3766.35 ± 10.5871.82 ± 12.0368.61 ± 12.341.8090.149
Self-Monitor60.22 ± 12.5558.76 ± 12.4764.39 ± 9.6864.87 ± 9.062.0360.112
Shift63.48 ± 12.5866.00 ± 11.3759.58 ± 11.6968.13 ± 14.852.4080.059 §
Emotional C59.98 ± 13.6963.76 ± 11.2268.03 ± 11.2566.83 ± 12.753.6980.014 *
Initiate65.15 ± 10.3464.71 ± 9.8059.50 ± 9.6662.43 ± 12.732.4230.059 §
Working M70.45 ± 9.5763.82 ± 11.6764.95 ± 10.7065.48 ± 8.373.7050.013 *
Plan/Organize68.02 ± 10.4063.35 ± 11.1762.5 ± 11.6563.09 ± 11.482.5490.050 *
Org of Materials65.21 ± 11.3158.82 ± 10.2558.61 ± 12.7057.83 ± 12.613.8020.012 *
Task Monitor65.24 ± 9.0162.59 ± 10.1862.52 ± 11.6859.35 ± 10.102.0320.112
Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation. § p-values < 0.06; * p-values < 0.05. Abbreviations: C = control; M = memory; Org = organization.
Table 4. Principal component analysis.
Table 4. Principal component analysis.
RC1RC2RC3
BRIEF-2—Plan/Organize0.84200.23890.1921
BRIEF-2—Task Monitor0.80190.05760.2487
BRIEF-2—Working Memory0.78480.00500.1382
BRIEF-2—Organization of Materials0.6903−0.01650.1760
BRIEF-2—Initiate0.69000.33060.0691
CBCL—Attention Problems0.54210.37780.2531
CBCL—Anxious/Depressed0.01620.82540.1641
CBCL—Somatic Complaints0.21130.7475−0.0119
CBCL—Social Problems0.01690.72590.3128
CBCL—Withdrawn/Depressed0.17710.71070.1029
CBCL—Thought Problems0.06250.69220.2486
BRIEF-2—Shift0.41500.51620.1894
BRIEF-2—Inhibit0.26570.05200.8497
CBCL—Rule-Breaking Behaviors0.20060.11690.7782
CBCL—Aggressive Behaviors0.14910.37670.7615
BRIEF-2—Emotional Control0.16130.31990.6968
BRIEF-2—Self-Monitor0.17310.11650.6913
Unadjusted Eigenvalue6.61142.22531.7460
Adjusted Eigenvalue5.91951.68791.3212
Proportion Variance0.21510.21210.1954
Cumulative Variance0.21510.42710.6225
Data are presented as z-scores. Abbreviations: RC1 = first rotated component; RC2 = second rotated component; RC3 = third rotated component. Component loadings > 0.5 are shown in bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sesso, G.; Cristofani, C.; Berloffa, S.; Cristofani, P.; Fantozzi, P.; Inguaggiato, E.; Narzisi, A.; Pfanner, C.; Ricci, F.; Tacchi, A.; et al. Autism Spectrum Disorder and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Comorbidities Delineate Clinical Phenotypes in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Novel Insights from the Assessment of Psychopathological and Neuropsychological Profiles. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 3839. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9123839

AMA Style

Sesso G, Cristofani C, Berloffa S, Cristofani P, Fantozzi P, Inguaggiato E, Narzisi A, Pfanner C, Ricci F, Tacchi A, et al. Autism Spectrum Disorder and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Comorbidities Delineate Clinical Phenotypes in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Novel Insights from the Assessment of Psychopathological and Neuropsychological Profiles. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020; 9(12):3839. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9123839

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sesso, Gianluca, Chiara Cristofani, Stefano Berloffa, Paola Cristofani, Pamela Fantozzi, Emanuela Inguaggiato, Antonio Narzisi, Chiara Pfanner, Federica Ricci, Annalisa Tacchi, and et al. 2020. "Autism Spectrum Disorder and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Comorbidities Delineate Clinical Phenotypes in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Novel Insights from the Assessment of Psychopathological and Neuropsychological Profiles" Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 12: 3839. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9123839

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop