Next Article in Journal
A Motivational Model of BCI-Controlled Heuristic Search
Next Article in Special Issue
Ten Key Insights into the Use of Spinal Cord fMRI
Previous Article in Journal
Gait Training with Bilateral Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation in Stroke Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial
Previous Article in Special Issue
Neural Responses to Consciously and Unconsciously Perceived Emotional Faces: A Spinal fMRI Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Lateralized Brainstem and Cervical Spinal Cord Responses to Aversive Sounds: A Spinal fMRI Study

by
Stephen D. Smith
1,
Tiffany A. Kolesar
2 and
Jennifer Kornelsen
1,2,3,*
1
Department of Psychology, University of Winnipeg, Winnipeg, MB R3B 2E9, Canada
2
Department of Physiology and Pathophysiology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB R3E 0J9, Canada
3
Department of Radiology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB R3A 1R9, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2018, 8(9), 165; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8090165
Submission received: 29 June 2018 / Revised: 25 August 2018 / Accepted: 29 August 2018 / Published: 31 August 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Functional Neuroimaging of the Spinal Cord)

Abstract

:
Previous research has delineated the networks of brain structures involved in the perception of emotional auditory stimuli. These include the amygdala, insula, and auditory cortices, as well as frontal-lobe, basal ganglia, and cerebellar structures involved in the planning and execution of motoric behaviors. The aim of the current research was to examine whether emotional sounds also influence activity in the brainstem and cervical spinal cord. Seventeen undergraduate participants completed a spinal functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study consisting of two fMRI runs. One run consisted of three one-minute blocks of aversive sounds taken from the International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) stimulus set; these blocks were interleaved by 40-s rest periods. The other block consisted of emotionally neutral stimuli also drawn from the IADS. The results indicated a stark pattern of lateralization. Aversive sounds elicited greater activity than neutral sounds in the right midbrain and brainstem, and in right dorsal and ventral regions of the cervical spinal cord. Neutral stimuli, on the other hand, elicited less neural activity than aversive sounds overall; these responses were left lateralized and were found in the medial midbrain and the dorsal sensory regions of the cervical spinal cord. Together, these results demonstrate that aversive auditory stimuli elicit increased sensorimotor responses in brainstem and cervical spinal cord structures.

1. Introduction

The cervical spinal cord lies at an important nexus between the brain, which programs emotional motoric responses, and the peripheral nervous system that executes these responses. This region of the central nervous system consists of eight segments (C1–C8) that innervate the scalp, neck, shoulders, and upper limbs [1]. Ventral regions of the cord send projections that trigger responses in muscles and internal organs, whereas dorsal regions of the cord receive sensory feedback from these structures [1]. Although often thought of as a purely sensorimotor structure, previous research has demonstrated that viewing emotional stimuli leads to enhanced activity in both ventral and dorsal regions of the cervical spinal cord [2]. These responses are likely goal-directed, as emotional images depicting or implying responses by the upper limbs elicit more activity than emotional images related to lower-limb responses (which would require activity in the lumbar spinal cord [3]). In the current research, we examine whether emotion-dependent spinal cord activity can be elicited by salient stimuli that are auditory, rather than visual, in nature.
Earlier neuroimaging studies have delineated the brain areas involved in the perception of emotional sounds (see [4,5] for reviews). Several studies have highlighted the role of the amygdala in the processing of emotional sounds (e.g., [6,7,8,9]), a finding consistent with this structure’s role in rapid responses to emotionally salient stimuli [10,11]. The primary and secondary auditory cortices, as well as the superior temporal cortex, also reliably demonstrate enhanced responses to emotional stimuli (e.g., [12,13]). Recent data indicate that these temporal lobe structures interact during the perception of affective sounds. Using dynamic causal modeling of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data, Kumar and colleagues [12] found that emotion-dependent activity in the auditory cortex triggers amygdalar responses to aversive sounds. This relationship appears to be reciprocal, with the amygdala also feeding back to the auditory cortex [14]. Importantly, the amygdala can also influence responses in the spinal cord. This modulation can occur via direct amygdalo-spinal projections [15] or through brainstem nuclei such as the parabrachial nucleus or the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus [16]. Taken together, the enhanced activity in the amygdala and auditory cortices suggests that more neural resources are allocated to the processing of emotional sounds, a pattern similar to that found for the visual perception of emotional images [17,18]; based on neuroanatomical studies, it is possible that this preferential processing could modulate spinal cord activity.
Emotional sounds also activate neural regions associated with interoception, the ability to detect changes in internal somatosensory states [19,20]. Indeed, the insula and anterior cingulate cortex—which, together, comprise the brain’s salience network [21]—frequently respond to emotional sounds [9,22]. This pattern is noteworthy because numerous behavioural studies have demonstrated that aversive sounds elicit changes in electrodermal responses, heart rate, and pupil dilation [23,24,25]. Viscerosensory feedback associated with these psychophysiological changes inputs to dorsal nuclei in the spinal cord which, in turn, projects to the brain via the dorsal spinothalamic tract [26]. These pathways input to the nucleus of the solitary tract—a brainstem site of sensory, autonomic, and behavioural integration—along with the parabrachial nucleus and periaqueductal grey [27]. These brainstem regions extend to numerous cortical sites including the insula, amygdala, rostral and anterior midcingulate cortex, and the medial orbitofrontal cortex [28]. These neuroanatomical studies are supported by neuroimaging research demonstrated that autonomic responses in the body are associated with increased activity in the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and insula [29,30]. Based on these complementary lines of research, it is reasonable to conclude that the insula and anterior cingulate activity observed during the perception of emotional sounds is related to the body’s somatic responses to those stimuli [31].
Importantly, emotional auditory stimuli have also been shown to modulate activity in motoric regions of the brain. In a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies, Früholz and colleagues [32] noted that the inferior frontal gyrus, basal ganglia, and cerebellum consistently show larger responses to aversive than neutral stimuli. This activity is likely related to response preparation (e.g., a withdrawal or freezing response), particularly when the perceived stimuli are simple nonhuman sounds rather than more complex vocal prosody [9,32]. This hypothesis is consistent with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies showing that emotional stimuli increase the sensitivity of the corticospinal tract [33,34]. In contrast, suppressing sensorimotor activity via inhibitory TMS pulse sequences interferes with the processing of emotional vocalizations [35,36,37]. Together, these studies, using a variety of neuroimaging techniques, demonstrate that the processing of aversive emotional sounds involves motoric regions of the brain.
Given the sensorimotor nature of the brain’s responses to emotion-evoking sounds, we hypothesized that emotionally aversive auditory stimuli would also elicit greater levels of activity in the cervical spinal cord than would emotionally neutral sounds. This pattern of activity would indicate that the ventral spinal cord is initiating changes in upper-body musculature in response to the aversive sounds and that the dorsal spinal cord is receiving sensory information about these emotional responses.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Nineteen undergraduate students were included in this study; two were excluded due to technical difficulties leaving 17 participants in the final analysis (13 female, 15 right-handed, age range 18–25). Ethics approval was obtained from the University of Manitoba’s Bannatyne Human Research Ethics Board (project identification code HS18235 (B2014:077); approval received 8 July 2014) and the University of Winnipeg’s Human Research Ethics Board (multi-site approval received 26 August 2014). All participants gave written informed consent and were screened for magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) safety prior to scanning. Participants receive a remuneration of $25.

2.2. Stimulus Materials

Stimuli consisted of 10 neutral and 10 negative emotion-evoking sounds, obtained from the International Affective Digital Sounds database (IADS, [38]). The Negative sounds consisted of the following IADS items: 115 (bees buzzing), 275 (male horror-movie-style scream), 276 (female horror-movie-style scream), 420 (car horns in a traffic jam), 422 (car accident), 424 (cars colliding), 600 (car crash with screaming), 709 (alarm clock ringing), 711 (sirens), and 719 (dentist drill). The Neutral sounds consisted of the following IADS items: 104 (dog panting), 245 (hiccup), 358 (eraser), 373 (scraping and rinsing dishes), 374 (rinsing dishes), 376 (lawnmower), 382 (shoveling snow), 698 (fireplace), 720 (brushing teeth), and 722 (non-threatening footsteps). The selection of the aversive stimuli was based on the norms accompanying the IADS database [38]. Neutral items were rated as being low in arousal and negative valence; specific neutral items were selected based on their semantic similarity to the emotional items (e.g., a human scream was matched with a human hiccup). t-tests comparing the average frequency and peak frequency of the emotional and neutral stimuli found no significant difference between the two groups (both t-values < 1). Stimuli were presented at 90 dB through the scanner room speakers. Although participants wore ear plugs for hearing protection in the noisy MRI environment, participants reported easily hearing the stimuli. Additionally, during a post-scan interview, participants were asked to list stimuli that they found particularly salient; all participants mentioned several sounds, suggesting that the stimuli were accurately perceived.

2.3. Experimental Design

Stimuli were presented in a boxcar design. In total, two spinal fMRI runs were collected: one run of neutral sounds and one run of negative emotion-evoking sounds. Each run consisted of three 60 s blocks of stimuli, interleaved by 40 s of rest. Ten sounds, each lasting for six seconds, were used per stimulus block. The order of the stimuli was randomized in each block for each participant.

2.4. fMRI Scanning Parameters and Preprocessing

MRI scanning was conducted using a 3-Tesla Siemens MAGNETOM Trio system (Erlangen, Germany) at the Winnipeg Regional Health Authority MRI satellite clinic. T2-weighted anatomical/functional cervical spinal cord images were acquired in sagittal orientation, extending from the corpus callosum to approximately T1 (single-shot fast spin echo (HASTE) sequence, TE/TR: 76/6750 ms per volume, FOV 280 × 210 mm, 1.46 mm × 1.46 mm resolution, 9 slices, 0 gap between slices, slice thickness: 2 mm, 50 volumes) (see [39] for a discussion of the challenges and advances in spinal cord functional imaging).
Spinal cord fMRI data were preprocessed with the custom-written Matlab scripts ‘spinalfMRI8’ (version 8, P.W. Stroman, Kingston, ON, Canada) [40,41] used in previous studies [2,3,42,43,44]. Slice timing correction and coalignment to correct for bulk motion using a non-linear 3D adjustment was applied. An automated normalization was performed that matched regions of the spinalfMRI8 spatially normalized template to the fMRI images by matching the corpus callosum, midbrain and pons in order to identify the cord in consecutive segments moving from rostral to caudal. Spatial smoothing with a 3D Gaussian filter was applied in the rostro-caudal direction at 2 mm.

2.5. fMRI Data Analysis

Spinal cord fMRI data were analyzed with the custom-written Matlab script ‘spinalfMRI8’ [40,41] used in previous studies [2,3,42,43,44]. Individual level data were analyzed using the General Linear Model (GLM) for each run to compare the stimuli blocks to the rest periods (p = 0.001). A second-level random effects analysis was performed to identify differences between the two conditions. Mean voxel-wise β -values from the individual-level GLMs were averaged and divided by the standard error of the mean to calculate t-statistics between conditions using a paired contrast (p = 0.01, contrast coefficients of −1, 1 for the Neutral and Negative conditions). A spatial extent method—where contiguous active voxels are considered more likely to be true positives and randomly distributed single voxels are considered more likely to be false positives—was applied to balance Type-I and Type-II errors. Thus, large clusters are likely to represent true activity while small, non-contiguous clusters are more likely to be false positive; for this reason, clusters consisting of five or fewer voxels have been excluded from results tables. Voxels exceeding threshold were displayed on the spatially normalized template, regions of activity were identified via comparison to the labelled normalized template, and significant voxels were manually counted. The center of gravity coordinates were extracted and represent the distance, in mm, from the pontomedullary junction (X = left to right; Y = ventral to dorsal; Z = rostral to caudal).

3. Results

The results from the contrasts of the negative and neutral fMRI runs can be found in Table 1 and Table 2 and in Figure 1. At the level of the midbrain, significant emotion-dependent activity was observed in regions of the right periaqueductal gray (PAG), right caudal inferior colliculus, and a region including the right rostral inferior colliculus and caudal superior colliculus. At the levels of the brainstem, enhanced responses were observed in the medial region of the pons, and the right medulla (in the region of the nucleus reticularis gigantocellularis). In contrast, neutral sounds led to greater activity in the left medial midbrain. When summed across the midbrain and brainstem regions, aversive sounds elicited activity in 156 voxels and neutral sounds elicited activity in 39 voxels.
The spinal cord results showed right-lateralized responses to negative emotional stimuli. The aversive sounds elicited more activity than neutral sounds in the ventral (motoric) regions of spinal cord segments C2 as well as in the dorsal (sensory) regions of C5 and C6. In contrast, neutral sounds led to greater activity in medial/left dorsal C8. When summed across the cervical spinal cord segments, aversive sounds elicited activity in 57 voxels in the right hemicord and neutral sounds elicited activity in 26 voxels in the medial/left hemicord.

4. Discussion

In the current study, aversive sounds led to a pronounced lateralization of activity in the midbrain, brainstem, and cervical spinal cord. These emotionally negative stimuli elicited right-lateralized activity in the medial and dorsal midbrain and brainstem and in both dorsal (sensory) and ventral (motoric) regions of several cervical spinal cord segments. Neutral stimuli, on the other hand, led to medial/left-lateralized activity in medial and dorsal (sensory) regions of the midbrain and spinal cord. Together, these data suggest that emotional sounds trigger activity in areas of the central nervous system that prepare an individual to perform an overt behavioural response.
It is important to note that the active voxels in our study reflect the results of a contrast analysis (i.e., Aversive vs. Neutral fMRI runs). Although the results of this analysis did not find any activity in the left brainstem or cervical spinal cord in response to aversive sounds, this does not mean that these structures were not active at all. Rather, it demonstrates that the amount of activity did not differ between the two conditions. This point is important; brainstem and ventral spinal cord activity isolated to one side of the body would not produce an evolutionarily useful motoric response to aversive stimuli.
The cervical spinal cord data are consistent with earlier studies reporting that negative images elicit greater activity than neutral images in both dorsal and ventral regions of the cervical spinal cord [2,3,44]. The fact that aversive auditory stimuli also led to increased spinal cord activity suggests that the emo-motoric responses found in earlier research are part of a general threat-response system rather than being modality-specific. Future research using additional sensory modalities is necessary to determine whether any aversive sensory experience is sufficient to stimulate spinal cord neurons or whether this system reacts only to stimuli that are threatening in nature. Additional studies could also clarify whether these sensorimotor emotional responses are triggered by emotional valence (i.e., positive vs. negative) or arousal, or to an interaction of these two stimulus characteristics.
The observed activity in the midbrain and brainstem is also likely a sensorimotor response. The enhanced activity in the inferior colliculus is almost certainly related to this structure’s role in orienting to auditory stimuli [45,46]. Emotional stimuli are more salient than neutral stimuli and should, therefore, elicit more activity in this region than neutral stimuli. Projections from the inferior colliculi could then influence activity in other subcortical brain regions including the pulvinar nucleus and the amygdala [32], thus allowing the individual to allocate more attention toward these aversive stimuli.
The activity in the PAG is consistent with this structure’s role in integrating affective, autonomic, and motoric information [47]. Animal studies have noted that the lateral/dorsolateral column of the PAG is related to fight-or-flight responses [48]. These responses are mediated by descending projections to more caudal brainstem structures including the paragigantocellularis lateralis nucleus of the rostral ventrolateral medulla [49], a structure that appeared to show emotion-dependent activity in the current study. Given that previous neuroimaging studies have detected PAG activity in response to threatening stimuli [50] and frustration [51]—both of which involve changes in muscular responses (e.g., [52])—it seems reasonable to conclude that the PAG responses in the current study are also emo-motoric in nature.
The aversive auditory stimuli also elicited activity in the dorsomedial region of the pons. Although the current scanning parameters lacked the precision to isolate specific pontine nuclei, the dorsomedial subregion of the pons does include the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus, a cholinergic structure that modulates both arousal and motoric responsivity in combination with noradrenergic outputs from the locus coeruleus [53,54]. These ascending projections have been shown to modulate cortical responses in the supplementary motor area and motor cortices [53]. They also influence arousal by disinhibiting thalamic nuclei that project to the cortex [55]. It is possible that such activity was elicited by the aversive stimuli used in the current study.
An obvious question that emerges from the current data relates to the specific nature of the motoric response. Put simply, what type of motoric response is being planned? Although the exact nature of this behavioral response is difficult to assess within the physical confines of an MRI scanner, previous behavioral studies suggest two likely possibilities. The first is that the participants wished to avoid or withdraw from the aversive stimuli. If this view is accurate, then the observed activity in the brainstem and cervical spinal cord would reflect a form of “action preparedness” that would allow the individual to make a rapid and efficient response to a threatening stimulus [56,57]. Alternatively, the changes in brainstem and spinal cord activity could be associated with a “freezing” response. Freezing involves tensing muscles throughout the body in order to reduce or eliminate any movement that could be detected by a predator [58]. Although the participants in the current study were not directly threatened, freezing behaviors have been identified in experimental paradigms using human participants [59]. Given that the MR scanning environment limited the number of overt behaviors that could be performed by participants, freezing may have been a potential motoric response to the aversive stimuli. Consistent with this view, freezing has been linked with increased activity in the PAG [60,61], a region that responded to aversive stimuli in the current study. It is also possible that both forms of motoric behavior occurred simultaneously, with freezing occurring while a behavioral response was being planned [62]. Future research utilizing psychophysiological and electromyographical (EMG) measurement techniques will be necessary to test these possibilities.
An intriguing element of the current research is that the neural responses to aversive stimuli were lateralized to the right side of the midbrain, brainstem, and spinal cord. At first glance, it is tempting to assume that this activity is part of an overall pattern of laterality. Indeed, numerous studies have highlighted the importance of right-hemisphere structures in the perception of emotionally negative and/or avoidance-related stimuli (see [63] for a review of competing theories of the lateralization of emotion). However, such a conclusion would be premature. The numerous ascending and descending pathways of the central nervous system decussate at different levels of the brainstem. It is unclear, therefore, whether the lateralized activity in the brainstem and spinal cord is an extension of hemispheric differences in the other brain areas such as the amygdala or prefrontal cortices (e.g., [8,29,64]). Future investigations using functional MRI of the brain and spinal cord—with parameters optimized to disambiguate the activity of different brainstem nuclei—would allow us to better characterize the nature of these right-lateralized responses.
An additional possibility is that the right-lateralization of motoric responses were due to the fact that 88% of our participants were right-handed. Although there is little evidence that handedness greatly affects the lateralization of emotional perception in the brain [65], it is possible that the motoric programs resulting from this perception might be biased toward an individual’s dominant hand. This possibility could be tested in future studies by scanning equal numbers of left- and right-handed participants.
Although the current study provides novel information about how the brainstem and cervical spinal cord process aversive sounds, there are limitations worthy of discussion. First, only aversive and neutral items were used. Although it would be interesting to investigate whether different types of emotions—particularly happiness—would trigger motoric responses, pilot testing of the IADS stimuli suggested that the aversive stimuli were perceived as being more salient and threatening. Therefore, for our initial study of spinal cord responses to emotions, we focused on stimuli that were likely to elicit a motoric response such as withdrawal or freezing. Second, the stimuli used in the current study consisted primarily of environmental sounds. A previous meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies noted that different types of emotional auditory stimuli recruit slightly different neural networks [32]. That said, this same analysis identified a core network of brain areas recruited across emotional stimulus categories; this network includes the amygdala and sensorimotor structures relevant to the current study. Therefore, we can be relatively confident that similar patterns of spinal cord activity would be detected if different categories of emotional sounds were used.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the results of the current study extend our understanding of the neural networks involved in the perception of aversive sounds. Future studies examining the sensitivity of these midbrain, brainstem, and spinal cord regions to manipulations of emotional valence and arousal will help to further refine our understanding of how the central nervous system responds to emotional auditory stimuli.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.D.S. and J.K.; Methodology, S.D.S. and J.K.; Software, J.K.; Data Curation, S.D.S., T.A.K., and J.K.; Formal analysis, T.A.K.; Writing-Original Draft Preparation, S.D.S., T.A.K., and J.K.; Writing-Review & Editing, S.D.S., T.A.K., and J.K.; Supervision: S.D.S. and J.K.; Project Administration: T.A.K. and S.D.S.; Funding Acquisition, S.D.S.

Funding

This research was funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada under Grant RGPIN-2014-03928.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the technical staff at the Winnipeg Regional Health Authority Satellite MRI Centre for their assistance with data collection. This research was funded by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. This paper benefited from the comments of two anonymous reviewers.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Lee, M.W.L.; McPhee, R.W.; Stringer, M.D. An evidence-based approach to human dermatomes. Clin. Anat. 2008, 21, 363–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Smith, S.D.; Kornelsen, J. Emotion-dependent responses in spinal cord neurons: A spinal fMRI study. Neuroimage 2011, 58, 269–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. McIver, T.A.; Kornelsen, J.; Smith, S.D. Limb-specific emotional modulation of cervical spinal cord neurons. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 13, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Frühholz, S.; Trost, W.; Grandjean, D. The role of the medial temporal limbic system in processing emotions in voice and music. Prog. Neurobiol. 2014, 123, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Gerdes, A.B.M.; Wieser, M.J.; Alpers, G.W. Emotional pictures and sounds: A review of multimodal interactions of emotion cues in multiple domains. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Anders, S.; Eippert, F.; Weiskopf, N.; Veit, R. The human amygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and sounds irrespective of arousal: An fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2008, 3, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Fecteau, S.; Belin, P.; Joanette, Y.; Armony, J.L. Amygdala responses to nonlinguistic emotional vocalizations. Neuroimage 2007, 36, 480–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Frühholz, S.; Grandjean, D. Processing of emotional vocalizations in bilateral inferior frontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2847–2855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Zald, D.H.; Pardo, J.V. The neural correlates of aversive auditory stimulation. Neuroimage 2002, 16, 746–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. LeDoux, J.E. Emotion Circuits in the Brain. Focus (Madison) 2009, 7, 274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zald, D.H. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res. Rev. 2003, 41, 88–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kumar, S.; Von Kriegstein, K.; Friston, K.; Griffiths, T.D. Features versus feelings: Dissociable representations of the acoustic features and valence of aversive sounds. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 14184–14192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Pannese, A.; Grandjean, D.; Frühholz, S. Amygdala and auditory cortex exhibit distinct sensitivity to relevant acoustic features of auditory emotions. Cortex 2016, 85, 116–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Fruholz, S.; Ceravolo, L.; Grandjean, D. Specific brain networks during explicit and implicit decoding of emotional prosody. Cereb. Cortex 2012, 22, 1107–1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Mizuno, N.; Takahashi, O.; Satoda, T.; Matsushima, R. Amygdalo-spinal projections in the macaque monkey. Neurosci. Lett. 1985, 53, 327–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Price, J.L.; Amaral, D.G. An autoradiographic study of the projections of the central nucleus of the monkey amygdala. J. Neurosci. 1981, 1, 1242–1259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Morris, J.S.; Friston, K.J.; Büchel, C.; Frith, C.D.; Young, A.W.; Calder, A.J.; Dolan, R.J. A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions. Brain 1998, 121, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Surguladze, S.A.; Brammer, M.J.; Young, A.W.; Andrew, C.; Travis, M.J.; Williams, S.C.R.; Phillips, M.L. A preferential increase in the extrastriate response to signals of danger. Neuroimage 2003, 19, 1317–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Barrett, L.F.; Simmons, W.K. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 419–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Craig, A.D. How do you feel now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Seeley, W.W.; Menon, V.; Schatzberg, A.F.; Keller, J.; Glover, G.H.; Kenna, H.; Reiss, A.L.; Greicius, M.D. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 2349–2356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Büchel, C.; Dolan, R.J.; Armony, J.L.; Friston, K.J. Amygdala-hippocampal involvement in human aversive trace conditioning revealed through event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 1999, 19, 10869–10876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Bradley, M.M.; Lang, P.J. Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 204–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Da Silva, S.P.; Backs, R.W. Cardiac response during auditory selective attention to tones and affective sounds. Psychophysiology 2015, 52, 1099–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Partala, T.; Surakka, V. Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2003, 59, 185–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Critchely, H.D. Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J. Comp. Neurol. 2005, 493, 154–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Critchley, H.D.; Harrison, N.A. Visceral influence on brain and behavior. Neuron 2013, 77, 624–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Blessing, W.W. The Lower Brainstem and Bodily Homeostasis; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  29. Critchley, H.D.; Mathias, C.J.; Josephs, O.; O’Doherty, J.; Zanini, S.; Dewar, B.K.; Cipolotti, L.; Shallice, T.; Dolan, R.J. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: Converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 2003, 126, 2139–2152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Kotz, S.A.; Kalberlah, C.; Bahlmann, J.; Friederici, A.D.; Haynes, J.D. Predicting vocal emotion expressions from the human brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 1971–1981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Frühholz, S.; Trost, W.; Kotz, S.A. The sound of emotions—Towards a unifying neural network perspective of affective sound processing. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 68, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Giovannelli, F.; Banfi, C.; Borgheresi, A.; Fiori, E.; Innocenti, I.; Rossi, S.; Zaccara, G.; Viggiano, M.P.; Cincotta, M. The effect of music on corticospinal excitability is related to the perceived emotion: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cortex 2013, 49, 702–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Komeilipoor, N.; Pizzolato, F.; Daffertshofer, A.; Cesari, P. Excitability of motor cortices as a function of emotional sounds. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Agnew, S.K.; Banissy, Z.K.; McGettigan, M.J.; Walsh, C.; Scott, V. Investigating the neural basis of theta burst stimulation to premotor cortex on emotional vocalization perception: A combined TMS-fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 150. [Google Scholar]
  36. Banissy, M.J.; Sauter, D.A.; Ward, J.; Warren, J.E.; Walsh, V.; Scott, S.K. Suppressing sensorimotor activity modulates the discrimination of auditory emotions but not speaker identity. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 13552–13557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Hoekert, M.; Vingerhoets, G.; Aleman, A. Results of a pilot study on the involvement of bilateral inferior frontal gyri in emotional prosody perception: An rTMS study. BMC Neurosci. 2010, 11, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Bradley, M.M.; Lang, P.J. The International Affective Digitized Sounds; Affective Ratings of Sounds and Instruction Manual; Technical Report B-3; University of Florida: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2007; pp. 29–46. [Google Scholar]
  39. Stroman, P.W.; Wheeler-Kingshott, C.; Backon, M.; Schwab, J.M.; Bosma, R.; Brooks, J.; Cadotte, D.; Carlstedt, T.; Ciccarelli, O.; Cohen-Adad, J.; et al. The current state-of-the-art of spinal cord imaging. NeuroImage 2014, 4, 1070–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Bosma, R.L.; Stroman, P.W. Assessment of data acquisition parameters, and analysis techniques for noise reduction in spinal cord fMRI data. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2014, 32, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Leitch, J.K.; Figley, C.R.; Stroman, P.W. Applying functional MRI to the spinal cord and brainstem. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2010, 28, 1225–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kornelsen, J.; Smith, S.D.; McIver, T.A. A neural correlate of visceral emotional responses: Evidence from fMRI of the thoracic spinal cord. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 584–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Smith, S.D.; Kornelsen, J.; McIver, T.A. Generating facial expressions of disgust activates neurons in the thoracic spinal cord: Aspinal fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 328–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Wilson, A.D.; Kolesar, T.A.; Kornelsen, J.; Smith, S.D. Neural responses to consciously and unconsciously perceived emotional faces: A spinal fMRI study. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Konishi, M. Auditory localization. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience; Squire, L., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010; pp. 721–728. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rinne, T.; Balk, M.H.; Koistinen, S.; Autti, T.; Alho, K.; Sams, M. Auditory selective attention modulates activation of human inferior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 2008, 100, 3323–3327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Holstege, G. The mesopontine rostromedial tegmental nucleus and the emotional motor system: Role in basic survival behavior. J. Comp. Neurol. 2009, 513, 559–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Lovick, T.A. The periaqueductal gray-rostral medulla connection in the defence reaction: Efferent pathways and descending control mechanisms. Behav. Brain Res. 1993, 58, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Satpute, A.B.; Wager, T.D.; Cohen-Adad, J.; Bianciardi, M.; Choi, J.K.; Buhle, J.T.; Wald, L.L.; Barrett, L.F. Identification of discrete functional subregions of the human periaqueductal gray. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 17101–17106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Lindner, K.; Neubert, J.; Pfannmöller, J.; Lotze, M.; Hamm, A.O.; Wendt, J. Fear-potentiated startle processing in humans: Parallel fMRI and orbicularis EMG assessment during cue conditioning and extinction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2015, 98, 535–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Yu, R.; Mobbs, D.; Seymour, B.; Rowe, J.B.; Calder, A.J. The neural signature of escalating frustration in humans. Cortex 2014, 54, 165–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Bradley, M.M.; Moulder, B.; Lang, P.J. When good things go bad: The reflex physiology of defense. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 468–473. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  53. Aravamuthan, B.R.; Muthusamy, K.A.; Stein, J.F.; Aziz, T.Z.; Johansen-Berg, H. Topography of cortical and subcortical connections of the human pedunculopontine and subthalamic nuclei. Neuroimage 2007, 37, 694–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Venkatraman, A.; Edlow, B.L.; Immordino-Yang, M.H. The Brainstem in Emotion: A Review. Front. Neuroanat. 2017, 11, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Mesulam, M.M. Cholinergic Pathways and the Ascending Reticular Activating System of the Human Brain. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1995, 757, 169–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Coombes, S.A.; Tandonnet, C.; Fujiyama, H.; Janelle, C.M.; Cauraugh, J.H.; Summers, J.J. Emotion and motor preparation: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of corticospinal motor tract excitability. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2009, 9, 380–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Van Loon, A.M.; Van den Wildenberg, W.P.; Van stegeren, A.H.; Hajcak, G.; Ridderinkhof, K.R. Emotional stimuli modulate readiness for action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 10, 174–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Fanselow, M.S. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1994, 1, 429–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Lang, P.J.; Davis, M. Emotion, motivation, and the brain: Reflex foundations in animal and human research. Prog. Brain Res. 2006, 156, 3–29. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  60. Hermans, E.J.; Henckens, M.J.; Roelofs, K.; Fernández, G. Fear bradycardia and activation of the human periaqueductal grey. NeuroImage 2013, 66, 278–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Wendt, J.; Löw, A.; Weymar, M.; Lotze, M.; Hamm, A.O. Active avoidance and attentive freezing in the face of approaching threat. NeuroImage 2017, 158, 196–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Roelofs, K. Freeze for action: Neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2017, 372, 1718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Demaree, H.A.; Everhart, D.E.; Youngstrom, E.A.; Harrison, D.W. Brain lateralization of emotional processing: Historical roots and a future incorporating dominance. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2005, 4, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Baas, D.; Aleman, A.; Kahn, R.S. Lateralization of amygdala activation: A systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Brain Res. Rev. 2004, 45, 96–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Elias, L.; Bryden, M.P.; Bulman-Fleming, M.B. Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization. Neuropsychologia 1998, 36, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Group-level midbrain, brainstem and cervical spinal cord activity for the negative > neutral sounds contrast (p = 0.01; n = 17). Cervical spinal cord segments are labeled on the left of the figure for both sagittal and axial views. Voxels in orange represent significant activity for the negative as compared to the neutral condition and voxels in blue represent significant activity for the neutral as compared to the negative condition. Sagittal (above) and axial (below) views allow for Rs = rostral, C = caudal, D = dorsal, V = ventral, R = right, and L = left localization. Numbers 1–11 correspond to the clusters listed in Table 1 and Table 2.
Figure 1. Group-level midbrain, brainstem and cervical spinal cord activity for the negative > neutral sounds contrast (p = 0.01; n = 17). Cervical spinal cord segments are labeled on the left of the figure for both sagittal and axial views. Voxels in orange represent significant activity for the negative as compared to the neutral condition and voxels in blue represent significant activity for the neutral as compared to the negative condition. Sagittal (above) and axial (below) views allow for Rs = rostral, C = caudal, D = dorsal, V = ventral, R = right, and L = left localization. Numbers 1–11 correspond to the clusters listed in Table 1 and Table 2.
Brainsci 08 00165 g001
Table 1. Location of brainstem activity in the contrast of the negative sounds and neutral sounds.
Table 1. Location of brainstem activity in the contrast of the negative sounds and neutral sounds.
LocationSideDorsal/VentralCoordinates (mm)Voxels
XYZ
Negative > Neutral
1. Midbrain (caudal IC)Right −2212724
2. Midbrain (rostral IC/caudal SC)Right −718308
3. Midbrain (Periaqueductal Gray)RightMedial−392081
4. PonsMedialDorsal−161023
5. MedullaRightMedial−45−820
Neutral > Negative
6. MidbrainMedial/LeftMedial172639
IC = inferior colliculus; SC = superior colliculus. Results are presented at p < 0.01, with a cluster threshold of ≥5. Coordinates are position, in mm, relative to the pontomedullary junction for the center of mass. The cluster number corresponds to Figure 1.
Table 2. Location of spinal cord activity in the contrast of the negative sounds and neutral sounds.
Table 2. Location of spinal cord activity in the contrast of the negative sounds and neutral sounds.
LocationSideDorsal/VentralCoordinates (mm)Voxels
XYZ
Negative > Neutral
7. C2RightVentral−44−3413
8. C5RightDorsomedial/medial−35−6713
9. C6RightDorsal−47−7826
10. C6RightDorsal−58−855
Neutral > Negative
11. C8Medial/LeftDorsal18−10726
Results are presented at p < 0.01, with a cluster threshold of ≥5. Coordinates are position, in mm, relative to the pontomedullary junction for the center of mass. The cluster number corresponds to Figure 1.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Smith, S.D.; Kolesar, T.A.; Kornelsen, J. Lateralized Brainstem and Cervical Spinal Cord Responses to Aversive Sounds: A Spinal fMRI Study. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 165. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8090165

AMA Style

Smith SD, Kolesar TA, Kornelsen J. Lateralized Brainstem and Cervical Spinal Cord Responses to Aversive Sounds: A Spinal fMRI Study. Brain Sciences. 2018; 8(9):165. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8090165

Chicago/Turabian Style

Smith, Stephen D., Tiffany A. Kolesar, and Jennifer Kornelsen. 2018. "Lateralized Brainstem and Cervical Spinal Cord Responses to Aversive Sounds: A Spinal fMRI Study" Brain Sciences 8, no. 9: 165. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8090165

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop