Next Article in Journal
Cognitive Control After ACL Reconstruction: A Cross-Sectional Study on Impaired Proactive Inhibition Compared to Healthy Controls
Previous Article in Journal
The Role of Attachment in Refugees with Impaired Mental Health: A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Comorbidity Between Hikikomori and Autistic Traits May Be Identified as a Phenotypical Presentation Characterized by Greater Severity

1
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 80138 Naples, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2025, 15(5), 496; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050496
Submission received: 15 March 2025 / Revised: 22 April 2025 / Accepted: 30 April 2025 / Published: 10 May 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Neuropsychiatry)

Abstract

:
Objectives: Hikikomori is a condition characterized by extreme social withdrawal, functional impairment, and mental distress, which has gained increasing recognition worldwide. While it can be associated with comorbid psychiatric disorders, hikikomori shares similarities with autism spectrum, prompting investigations into their relationship. Given that hikikomori commonly manifests in early adulthood, this study aimed to explore the relationship between autistic features and hikikomori tendencies among university students. Methods: A total of 2037 university students were recruited via an online survey and assessed with the Adult Autism Subthreshold (AdAS) Spectrum and the Hikikomori Questionnaire (HQ-25). Participants were categorized into four groups: healthy controls (HCs), subjects with hikikomori tendencies (HKs), subjects with significant autistic traits (ATs), and subjects with both significant ATs and hikikomori tendencies (AT-HKs). Results: Results showed significant effects of both hikikomori presence and significant ATs on AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 scores, while a significant effect of their interaction was detected on AdAS Spectrum scores. The AT-HK group consistently scored higher on both AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 compared to other groups, with the AT and HK groups outperforming HCs in specific domains. HQ-25 Socialization and Isolation domains predicted higher AdAS Spectrum scores in hikikomori subjects, while various AdAS Spectrum domains served as predictors of HQ-25 scores in AT subjects. Conclusions: This study highlights a significant relationship between ATs and hikikomori tendencies in university students, suggesting that their comorbidity may represent a more severe phenotype, where each condition may exacerbate the other.

1. Introduction

Hikikomori syndrome is an emerging psychiatric condition characterized by extreme social withdrawal lasting at least six months, accompanied by significant functional impairment or mental distress. The phenomenon was described for the first time in Japan, where it progressively gained interest since the 1990s [1,2,3]. The term “hikikomori” derives from two Japanese characters: “hiku” meaning “to pull back” and “komoru” meaning “to seclude oneself,” describing an individual “who has withdrawn into seclusion’ [hikikomotta], a concept that is deeply intertwined with Japan’s cultural emphasis on community [4]. People with hikikomori typically withdraw from social life, in particular from the workplace or school, for prolonged periods of time, spending extended periods confined to their homes [1,2]. While hikikomori was firstly described as a culture-bound syndrome, its spreading in different countries all over the world has led to a reconceptualization of it as a society-bound syndrome linked to the increasing phenomenon of marginalization in post-industrial society in times of economic crisis [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. In particular, it is possible that individuals with a vulnerability towards psychopathology, including patients affected by other psychiatric disorders, may be more likely to present this specific psychopathological manifestation in specific environmental conditions [10,11].
However, despite its growing recognition, hikikomori is still not yet included in the official diagnostic classification systems, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorder 5th Edition—Text Revision (DSM-5-TR) and the International Classification of Disease (ICD-11), although described in the section of cultural manifestation of distress of the DSM-5-TR [12,13]. Although no formal diagnostic criteria exist, experts agree on certain core features of hikikomori such as severe social isolation, which includes the physical withdrawal in one’s own place of residence, the absence of active participation in academic and labor settings, as well as limited involvement in social relationships, for a duration of at least six months, and the presence of significant functional impairment or distress [9]. Moreover, its prevalence seems to be steadily increasing. Indeed, a survey conducted in Japan in the early 2000s revealed that at least 1.2% of individuals aged 15 to 49 met the criteria for hikikomori [14], while in 2016, the number of affected individuals in this age group had risen to 540,000, with males being disproportionately more impacted—approximately three times more than females [15]. Interestingly, the gender trend appears to reverse in countries outside of Japan, with females being more affected [16].
Moreover, the presence of hikikomori seems to be often associated with other comorbid psychiatric disorders, though research on this is still in its early stages. Common co-occurring disorders include mood disorders, anxiety disorders, psychotic disorders, and personality disorders; however, the relationship between these conditions and prolonged social withdrawal remains under investigation [17]. Furthermore, the potential connection between hikikomori and neurodevelopmental disorders, particularly autism spectrum disorder (ASD), is currently being explored. Indeed, ASD shares strong similarities with hikikomori, with many studies suggesting that individuals with hikikomori may exhibit traits commonly associated with autism spectrum [17,18,19,20].
ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by deficits in social communication and interactions, as well as restrictive, repetitive behaviors [13]. The presentation of ASD is highly variable, ranging from individuals with severe intellectual disabilities and limited verbal communication to those with average intelligence, with possibly some field of hyperfunctioning. This broad heterogeneity has led, in recent decades, to a reconceptualization of ASD as a spectrum of traits distributed along a continuum from the general population to the clinical population. In this framework, recent studies have stressed the need to examine not only the full-blown clinical presentations but also the milder, subclinical manifestations of the autism spectrum [21]. Nowadays, it is widely recognized that ASD and even subthreshold autistic traits (ATs), are often associated with other psychiatric comorbidities like anxiety disorders, mood disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorders, and psychosis [22,23,24,25,26,27]. In high-functioning individuals, these comorbidities may mask the underlying neurodevelopmental disorder, as they often compensate for their social difficulties [23,28,29]. In these subjects, stressful life events can trigger the development of psychiatric disorders. In particular, a very delicate phase appears to be that of the transition between high school and university life, which often coincides with the beginning of life as an out-of-town student. In this framework, challenges in social autonomy and living independently may exacerbate a greater difficulty in masking the core difficulties of the disorder, becoming a potential trigger for the development of other psychiatric conditions [30,31,32].
Given these premises and considering that hikikomori commonly begins in adolescence/early adulthood, we sought to investigate the relationship between autistic features and hikikomori, particularly among university students—a group that may be especially vulnerable due to the unique pressures they face. In this context, the aim of our study was to explore the relationship between hikikomori and ATs, focusing on identifying possible specific autistic and hikimomori features presented by subjects with only one or both these conditions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sample and Procedures

For the aim of this study, we enrolled students of University of Pisa bachelor’s or master’s degrees or of single-cycle degree programs. To encourage participation, all students attending the University of Pisa were invited via email to take part in the study. Participants who agreed to participate provided their consent, completed self-report psychometric assessments, and submitted a form with sociodemographic information. All procedures were conducted anonymously through an online platform.
Students who participated in the study had the option to consult with a psychiatrist to discuss their questionnaire results in more detail. Additionally, no financial compensation or other incentives were offered for participation.
The following self-report questionnaires were used to evaluate each participant: the Adult Autism Subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS Spectrum) for the investigation of ATs and the Hikikomori Questionnaire—25 (HQ—25) for the assessment of hikikomori tendencies.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Adult Autism Spectrum Questionnaire (AdAS Spectrum)

The AdAS Spectrum is a self-report tool designed to identify both full- and subthreshold ATs and traits in individuals without intellectual disabilities [33]. Childhood/adolescence, verbal and non verbal communication, empathy, inflexibility and adherence to routine, restricted interests and rumination, and hyper-hypo reactivity to sensory input are the seven areas that compose the questionnaire’s 160 items. It demonstrates excellent internal consistency, test–retest reliability, and strong convergent validity with other ASD measures [34]. There are two validated threshold scores: a cut-off of 70 for full-blown symptoms of ASD and a cut-off of 43 for clinically significant autistic features [35]. The questionnaire supports a dimensional approach to autism, encompassing threshold-level manifestations, mild/atypical symptoms, gender-specific features, and related personality traits.

2.2.2. Hikikomori Questionnaire—25 (HQ—25)

The HQ-25 is a tool designed to investigate the intensity of hikikomori symptoms experienced over the past six months [36]. It consists of three domains: socialization, emotional support, and isolation. Responses are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 to 4. A validation study established a threshold score of 42 to effectively differentiate individuals at risk for hikikomori from those who are not, demonstrating good sensitivity and specificity. A validated Italian version of the questionnaire is also available [37], demonstrating its cross-cultural applicability. These results affirm the HQ-25 as an effective instrument for screening and studying hikikomori across various cultures and age groups, including adolescents.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

The population sample was divided into four groups based on the presence or absence of clinically relevant ATs, of a risk for hikikomori tendencies, or both. The differentiation between subjects with clinically relevant ATs was made based on the threshold score of 43 at the AdAS Spectrum, while the differentiation between subjects at risk of hikikomori and not was made on the basis of the threshold score of 42 in the HQ-25 questionnaire.
Afterwards, chi-square testing and ANOVA were used to compare the sociodemographic variables between the four groups.
Two-way MANOVAs were then performed to assess the effect of the presence/absence of hikikomori and of the presence/absence of significant ATs on the AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 domains. For this purpose, the AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 domain scores were used as dependent variables and the presence/absence of hikikomori and of significant ATs, according to HQ-25 and AdAS Spectrum cut-offs, were used as independent variables. Moreover, two factorial ANOVAs were performed to assess the effect of the presence/absence of hikikomori and of the presence/absence of significant AT on the AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 total scores, with the AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 total scores used as dependent variables and the presence/absence of hikikomori and of significant ATs, according to HQ-25 and AdAS Spectrum cut-offs, as independent variables.
A one-way ANOVA analysis followed by post hoc Bonferroni corrections were also conducted in order to compare the scores obtained on the AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 questionnaires among the four different groups and in order to confirm the results from the MANOVA and factorial ANOVAs.
Then, we performed linear regression to investigate which HQ-25 domains were predictive of the presence of higher AdAS Spectrum scores in the hikikomori group. For this purpose, we used the presence of clinically relevant ATs as dependent variable and the HQ-25 domains as independent variables.
Lastly, a second linear regression analysis was conducted to investigate which AdAS Spectrum domains were predictive of hikikomori tendencies in the AT group. For this purpose, we used HQ-25 as the dependent variable and AdAS Spectrum domains as independent variables.
All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS version 26.0.

3. Results

The total sample consisted of 2037 students enrolled in three-year, master’s, or single-cycle degree programs. Participants were divided into four groups based on the presence or absence of clinically relevant ATs, of a risk for hikikomori tendencies, or both:
  • A total of 550 subjects without clinically relevant ATs and at no risk of hikikomori (HCs);
  • A total of 118 subjects with significant hikikomori tendencies but without significant ATs (HKs);
  • A total of 818 subjects with significant ATs but without hikikomori (ATs);
  • A total of 821 subjects who had significant ATs and also showed hikikomori tendencies (AT-HKs).
The four groups significantly differed in gender and age (see Table 1), with the AT group being significantly younger. Females were more represented in the HC group, while males predominated in the AT-HK group.
MANOVA results using AdAS Spectrum domains as dependent variables showed a significant effect of both hikikomori presence (Wilks’ lambda = 0.932, F = 24.112, p < 0.001) and significant ATs (Wilks’ lambda = 0.563, F = 256.142, p < 0.001), as well as their interaction (Wilks’ lambda = 0.989, F = 3.791, p < 0.001) on all AdAS Spectrum domains —except nonverbal communication (Table 2a, Figure 1a). Regarding HQ-25 domains, significant effects were found for hikikomori (Wilks’ lambda = 0.453, F = 924.828, p < 0.001) and ATs presence (Wilks’ lambda = 0.944, F = 45.398, p < 0.001), but no significant interaction was observed (Wilks’ lambda = 0.999, F = 0.834, p = 0.475) (Table 2a, Figure 1b). Two-factorial ANOVAs confirmed significant effects of both hikikomori and ATs on AdAS total score (AT: F = 1713.334, p < 0.001; hikikomori: F = 86.511, p < 0.001) and HQ-25 total score (AT: F = 128.484, p < 0.001; hikikomori: F = 2774.002, p < 0.001). A significant effect of hikikomori and ATs interaction was observed for AdAS total scores (F = 20.212, p < 0.001) but not for HQ-25 total scores (F = 0.128, p = 0.721) (Table 2b,c; Figure 1c,d).
ANOVA results (Table 3, Figure 2 and Figure 3) show that the AT-HK group scored significantly higher than all other groups on AdAS total and in multiple domains: childhood/adolescence, verbal communication, empathy, inflexibility and adherence to routine, rumination and restricted interests, and hyper-hyporeactivity to sensory input. The AT group scored higher than both HK and HC groups, which did not differ significantly. For the nonverbal communication domain, scores followed a descending order: AT-HK > AT > HK > HC. For the HQ-25, AT-HK scored highest on total score and the socialization and isolation domains, followed by HK, then AT, with HCs lowest. In the emotional support domain, AT-HK and HK groups scored higher than AT, which outperformed HCs.
A Linear regression in the HK group (Table 4) showed that the HQ-25 socialization and isolation domains significantly predicted higher AdAS scores. Another regression in the AT group identified AdAS domains—childhood/adolescence, verbal and non verbal communication, empathy, and restricted interests and rumination—as significant predictors of higher HQ-25 scores. Interestingly, inflexibility and adherence to routine emerged as a negative predictor (Table 5).

4. Discussion

This study examined the link between ATs and hikikomori tendencies among university students, focusing on how these conditions manifest individually and together. Findings revealed that both hikikomori risk and clinically relevant ATs significantly affected all domains of the AdAS Spectrum, both independently and interactively. Specifically, while higher ATs naturally influence AdAS Spectrum scores, the presence of hikikomori also has a significant impact on these scores, highlighting the complex and interconnected relationship between the two conditions. The interaction between ATs and hikikomori on AdAS Spectrum scores suggests that among individuals with elevated ATs, those who also experience hikikomori tend to show a notably greater increase in autism severity compared to AT subjects without hikikomori. In contrast, within the nonautistic population, the presence of hikikomori is associated with only a modest increase in autism-related symptoms. This indicates that the effect of hikikomori on autism severity is much more pronounced in individuals with high ATs than in those without.
Our results also showed a significant effect of both hikikomori risk and clinically relevant ATs on all domains of the HQ-25: in particular, not only subjects with hikikomori tendencies showed, as expected, higher hikikomori symptoms, but also subjects with ATs, suggesting that subjects with ATs would be at higher risk to show greater hikikomori symptoms. These results align with previous research suggesting that, due to core challenges in social communication, ATs may predispose individuals to develop hikikomori [20,38,39,40]. Indeed, many authors have proposed that challenges in social interactions and in communication proper of autism may lead to social withdrawal, increasing vulnerability towards hikikomori tendencies, up to its extreme pathological drift [41]. Emphasizing the importance of neurodevelopmental alterations, some studies have suggested that ASD and hikikomori may share a neurodevelopmental basis. In this framework, the innate propensity for seclusion exhibited by individuals with high levels of ATs—referred to by some authors as “hikikomori affinity”—and the ability to occasionally predict the full-blown onset of hikikomori syndrome lend credence to this notion [20,38,39].
Earlier studies, particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic, have emphasized the link between ATs and heightened social withdrawal [42]. A recent systematic review examining the effects of quarantine on individuals with ASD reported that, after the significant reduction in social interactions caused by confinement measures, ASD individuals experienced stronger behavioral effects from isolation, including anxiety, irritability, and emotional dysregulation [42]. Similarly, neurodevelopmentally atypical children facing loneliness show mental health risks with likely long-term effects [43]. In our data, individuals with both ATs and hikikomori scored higher on all AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 measures, suggesting that individuals with comorbid ATs and hikikomori represent a distinct phenotype marked by greater severity in both conditions. This supports the notion that ATs may increase susceptibility to severe hikikomori, while hikikomori may be more likely in those with elevated ATs. Globally, these findings provide additional support for the hypotheses previously proposed in the existing literature, suggesting that while the presence of ATs may generally increase the vulnerability towards higher hikikomori tendencies, eventually playing the role of a predisposing factor, among subjects in the autism spectrum, hikikomori tendencies would more likely be specifically associated with more pronounced ATs, as evidenced also by the interaction between the two conditions on autism spectrum severity. Interestingly, the group with only hikikomori was much smaller than the comorbid group (12.6% vs. 87.4%), while ATs were evenly distributed between those with and without hikikomori (50.01% vs 49.99%). Additionally, while AT-only individuals had elevated HQ-25 scores, hikikomori-only individuals did not differ significantly from controls on most AdAS items, except for nonverbal communication. These findings suggest that most hikikomori cases involve co-occurring ATs, while those with ATs, although more constantly presenting a higher tendency towards non-extreme social isolation, may also present with different psychopathological manifestations with respect to hikikomori. In this framework, neuroatypicality may act as a broader vulnerability factor, shaping diverse psychopathological outcomes through interactions with environmental and biological influences. The specific timing, nature, and severity of neurodevelopmental alterations—combined with interactions between biological and environmental factors—may shape the trajectory of illness, contributing to the emergence of diverse clinical presentations and comorbidities, including, in this context, hikikomori [31,41]. On the other hand, it could be hypothesized an eventual role of other psychopathological dimensions in the limited number of subjects with hikikomori without significant ATs, such as mood disorders, psychosis, or trauma- or stress-related disorders, which would be in line with the acknowledged presence of hikikomori secondary to other mental conditions [3,31,41].
Even in the absence of significant ATs, individuals with hikikomori exhibited marked deficits in nonverbal communication. This finding is particularly significant given that nonverbal communication accounts for approximately 65% of all human communication and plays a key role in conveying emotional content [44]. This aligns with studies showing that hikikomori individuals experience reduced social competence and communication, which may contribute to their gradual withdrawal [38]. Early signs, like difficulty making friends, being bullied, or preferring solitude, have also been linked to hikikomori onset. Further supporting this hypothesis, research has shown that changes in social interaction and communication difficulties, such as difficulty in joining a group of friends or making new ones, a preference for solitary activities, and being bullied, are not only early warning signs of a tendency towards social isolation but also significant predictors of the subsequent development of hikikomori [45,46,47].
Our results also suggest that stronger difficulties in the HQ-25 domains of socialization and isolation predict clinically relevant ATs in hikikomori subjects. Difficulties in social interaction and communication are central to ASD diagnosis [13] and may stem from deficits in social motivation and neurobiological functioning—especially in brain regions tied to social reward processing, such as the orbitofrontal–striatal–amygdala network [48,49,50,51]. Dysregulation of oxytocin, a key social bonding hormone, may further impair social motivation in ASD [52]. Many individuals with high ATs demonstrate a longstanding preference for isolation, likely influenced by both social impairments and the effort required to cope in social environments [41]. This can lead to avoidance and, in some cases, escalate to full-blown hikikomori [53]. Difficulties with group inclusion, a preference for solitary activities, and restricted interests may all contribute to this process [20].
Furthermore, several AdAS Spectrum domains—including childhood/adolescence, verbal and non verbal communication, empathy, and restricted interests—were predictive of more severe hikikomori symptoms among AT individuals. Indeed, not only are social challenges in childhood—such as struggles with joining peer groups, making new friends, a preference for solitary activities, and experiences of bullying—key predictors of the subsequent development of hikikomori [45,46,47] but deficits in both verbal and nonverbal communication have also been identified as contributing factors to the gradual process of social withdrawal [38]. Similarly, narrow and intensely pursued interests such as Internet and video game use have frequently been documented in hikikomori, both of which may act as a predisposing factor leading to social withdrawal [54,55].
Interestingly, the AdAS Spectrum domain of inflexibility and adherence to routine emerged instead as a negative predictor of the risk for hikikomori. This evidence, although apparently counterintuitive, may reflect how autistic rigidity, while limiting flexibility, can provide a protective structure that discourages total withdrawal by reinforcing daily routines [40].

Limits

Important limitations should be considered when evaluating these results. Only university students were included in the sample, so the results cannot be extrapolated to the wider population. Moreover, participants were selected voluntarily, and potential biases in sample selection, such as an over-representation of participants with a greater interest in the topic, should be considered. Thirdly, the fact that we relied on self-reported questionnaires raises the possibility of over- or underestimation of symptoms. Finally, the cross-sectional nature of the study means that we cannot infer causal or temporal relationships between the variables studied.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, this study provides evidence of a significant relationship between ATs and hikikomori tendencies among university students. Our findings highlight how subjects with comorbid ATs and hikikomori should be identified as a phenotype characterized by a greater severity of both conditions, supporting the hypothesis that ATs may be a vulnerability factor for developing more severe forms of hikikomori while, on the other hand, hikikomori conditions would be more likely underlain by greater ATs. Moreover, our study also suggests the presence of specific patterns of features that may help in profiling subjects with both hikikomori and ATs: in particular, while a deeper isolation and socialization difficulties may be indicative of the underlying presence of ATs in hikikomori subjects, the presence of greater ATs since childhood, communication and empathy impairments, as well as a greater tendency towards ruminative thinking may be more likely associated with the development of hikikomori in AT subjects. While these results contribute to a deeper understanding of the association between hikikomori and ATs, additional research, especially longitudinal studies, is warranted to better investigate temporal relationships and to apply these findings to different populations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.C., S.P., A.F. and L.D.; methodology, B.C., D.M. and L.D.; formal analysis, G.M.; writing—original draft preparation, B.N., C.B., C.D.F., V.T., F.P., F.G. and F.D.G.; writing—review and editing, B.C., D.M. and B.N.; supervision, L.D., A.F., S.P. and B.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All procedures were approved by Committee on Bioethics of the University of Pisa Review No. 18/2023 on 26 May 2023.

Informed Consent Statement

All recruited subjects consented to participate in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Kato, T.A.; Katsuki, R.; Kubo, H.; Shimokawa, N.; Sato-Kasai, M.; Hayakawa, K.; Kuwano, N.; Umene-Nakano, W.; Tateno, M.; Setoyama, D.; et al. Development and validation of the 22-item Tarumi’s Modern-Type Depression Trait Scale: Avoidance of Social Roles, Complaint, and Low Self-Esteem (TACS-22). Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 73, 448–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Kato, T.A.; Kanba, S.; Teo, A.R. A 39-Year-Old “Adultolescent”: Understanding Social Withdrawal in Japan. Am. J. Psychiatry 2016, 173, 112–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Kato, T.A.; Kanba, S.; Teo, A.R. Defining pathological social withdrawal: Proposed diagnostic criteria for hikikomori. World Psychiatry 2020, 19, 116–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kato, T.A.; Tateno, M.; Shinfuku, N.; Fujisawa, D.; Teo, A.R.; Sartorius, N.; Akiyama, T.; Ishida, T.; Choi, T.Y.; Balhara, Y.P.; et al. Does the ‘hikikomori’ syndrome of social withdrawal exist outside Japan? A preliminary international investigation. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2012, 47, 1061–1075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Liu, L.L.; Li, T.M.; Teo, A.R.; Kato, T.A.; Wong, P.W. Harnessing Social Media to Explore Youth Social Withdrawal in Three Major Cities in China: Cross-Sectional Web Survey. JMIR Ment. Health 2018, 5, e34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wong, J.C.M.; Wan, M.J.S.; Kroneman, L.; Kato, T.A.; Lo, T.W.; Wong, P.W.C.; Chan, G.H. Hikikomori Phenomenon in East Asia: Regional Perspectives, Challenges, and Opportunities for Social Health Agencies. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Harding, C. Hikikomori. Lancet Psychiatry 2018, 5, 28–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Orsolini, L.; Bellagamba, S.; Volpe, U.; Kato, T.A. Hikikomori and modern-type depression in Italy: A new phenotypical trans-cultural characterization? Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2022, 68, 1010–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kato, T.A.; Kanba, S.; Teo, A.R. Hikikomori: Multidimensional understanding, assessment, and future international perspectives. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 73, 427–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Uchida, Y.; Norasakkunkit, V. The NEET and Hikikomori spectrum: Assessing the risks and consequences of becoming culturally marginalized. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Carpita, B.; Bonelli, C.; Giovannoni, F.; Parri, F.; Gambini, M.; Nardi, B.; Amatori, G.; Cremone, I.M.; Pini, S.; Dell’Osso, L. Case report: Hikikomori syndrome in Italy and its link with autistic traits and internet gaming disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2024, 15, 1378572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. World Health Organization. ICD-11: International Classification of Diseases, 11th ed.; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2022.
  13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  14. Koyama, A.; Miyake, Y.; Kawakami, N.; Tsuchiya, M.; Tachimori, H.; Takeshima, T.; World Mental Health Japan Survey Group 2002–2006. Lifetime prevalence, psychiatric comorbidity and demographic correlates of “hikikomori” in a community population in Japan. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 176, 69–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Japan Cabinet Office. Wakamono No Seikatsu ni Kansuru Chousa-Houkokusho [Investigation on Life of Youth]; Japan Cabinet Office: Tokyo, Japan, 2016.
  16. Nonaka, S.; Takeda, T.; Sakai, M. Who are hikikomori? Demographic and clinical features of hikikomori (prolonged social withdrawal): A systematic review. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2022, 56, 1542–1554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kondo, N.; Sakai, M.; Kuroda, Y.; Kiyota, Y.; Kitabata, Y.; Kurosawa, M. General condition of hikikomori (prolonged social withdrawal) in Japan: Psychiatric diagnosis and outcome in mental health welfare centres. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2013, 59, 79–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Tateno, M.; Park, T.W.; Kato, T.A.; Umene-Nakano, W.; Saito, T. Hikikomori as a possible clinical term in psychiatry: A questionnaire survey. BMC Psychiatry 2012, 12, 169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kondo, N. Shishunki no hikikomori to hattatsu shougai [social withdrawal in adolescence and developmental disorders]. Jpn. J. Psychosom. Med. 2010, 50, 285. [Google Scholar]
  20. Yamada, M.; Kato, T.A.; Katsuki, R.I.; Yokoi, H.; Igarashi, M.; Komine, Y.; Kamata, Y.; Kato, N.; Iwanami, A.; Ohta, H. Pathological social withdrawal in autism spectrum disorder: A case control study of hikikomori in Japan. Front. Psychiatry 2023, 14, 1114224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dell’Osso, L.; Dalle Luche, R.; Maj, M. Adult autism spectrum as a transnosographic dimension. CNS Spectr. 2016, 21, 131–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Carpita, B.; Nardi, B.; Bonelli, C.; Massimetti, E.; Amatori, G.; Cremone, I.M.; Pini, S.; Dell’Osso, L. Presence and correlates of autistic traits among patients with social anxiety disorder. Front. Psychiatry 2024, 14, 1320558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Dell’Osso, L.; Cremone, I.M.; Carpita, B.; Fagiolini, A.; Massimetti, G.; Bossini, L.; Vita, A.; Barlati, S.; Carmassi, C.; Gesi, C. Correlates of autistic traits among patients with borderline personality disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 2018, 83, 7–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dell’Osso, L.; Amatori, G.; Bonelli, C.; Nardi, B.; Massimetti, E.; Cremone, I.M.; Carpita, B. Autistic traits underlying social anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and panic disorders. CNS Spectr. 2024, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ishizuka, K.; Ishiguro, T.; Nomura, N.; Inada, T. Autistic traits as predictors of persistent depression. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2022, 272, 211–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Richards, G.; Kenny, R.; Griffiths, S.; Allison, C.; Mosse, D.; Holt, R.; O’Connor, R.C.; Cassidy, S.; Baron-Cohen, S. Autistic traits in adults who have attempted suicide. Mol. Autism 2019, 10, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Dell’Osso, L.; Nardi, B.; Bonelli, C.; Amatori, G.; Pereyra, M.A.; Massimetti, E.; Cremone, I.M.; Pini, S.; Carpita, B. Autistic Traits as Predictors of Increased Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Severity: The Role of Inflexibility and Communication Impairment. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Marazziti, D.; Abelli, M.; Baroni, S.; Carpita, B.; Piccinni, A.; Dell’Osso, L. Recent findings on the pathophysiology of social anxiety disorder. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 2014, 11, 91–100. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dell’Osso, L.; Carpita, B.; Muti, D.; Morelli, V.; Salarpi, G.; Salerni, A.; Scotto, J.; Massimetti, G.; Gesi, C.; Ballerio, M.; et al. Mood symptoms and suicidality across the autism spectrum. Compr. Psychiatry 2019, 91, 34–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Carpita, B.; Muti, D.; Muscarella, A.; Dell’Oste, V.; Diadema, E.; Massimetti, G.; Signorelli, M.S.; Fusar Poli, L.; Gesi, C.; Aguglia, E.; et al. Sex Differences in the Relationship between PTSD Spectrum Symptoms and Autistic Traits in a Sample of University Students. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2019, 15, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dell’Osso, L.; Lorenzi, P.; Carpita, B. Autistic Traits and Illness Trajectories. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2019, 15, 94–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Flegenheimer, C.; Scherf, K.S. College as a Developmental Context for Emerging Adulthood in Autism: A Systematic Review of What We Know and Where We Go from Here. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2022, 52, 2075–2097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Dell’Osso, L.; Gesi, C.; Massimetti, E.; Cremone, I.M.; Barbuti, M.; Maccariello, G.; Moroni, I.; Barlati, S.; Castellini, G.; Luciano, M.; et al. Adult Autism Subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS Spectrum): Validation of a questionnaire investigating subthreshold autism spectrum. Compr. Psychiatry 2017, 73, 61–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Donati, M.A.; Berrocal, C.; Primi, C.; Petracchi, G.; Carpita, B.; Cosci, F.; Ruiz, A.; Carmassi, C.; Dell’Osso, L. Measuring subthreshold autistic traits in the general population: Psychometric properties of the Adult Autism Subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS Spectrum) scale. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 281, 112576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Dell’Osso, L.; Carmassi, C.; Cremone, I.M.; Muti, D.; Salerni, A.; Barberi, F.M.; Massimetti, E.; Gesi, C.; Politi, P.; Aguglia, E.; et al. Defining the Optimal Threshold Scores for Adult Autism Subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS Spectrum) in Clinical and General Population. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2020, 16, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Teo, A.R.; Chen, J.I.; Kubo, H.; Katsuki, R.; Sato-Kasai, M.; Shimokawa, N.; Hayakawa, K.; Umene-Nakano, W.; Aikens, J.E.; Kanba, S.; et al. Development and validation of the 25-item Hikikomori Questionnaire (HQ-25). Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2018, 72, 780–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Amendola, S.; Presaghi, F.; Teo, A.R.; Cerutti, R. Psychometric Properties of the Italian Version of the 25-Item Hikikomori Questionnaire. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Katsuki, R.; Tateno, M.; Kubo, H.; Kurahara, K.; Hayakawa, K.; Kuwano, N.; Kanba, S.; Kato, T.A. Autism spectrum conditions in hikikomori: A pilot case-control study. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2020, 74, 652–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Brosnan, M.; Gavin, J. The impact of higher levels of autistic traits on risk of hikikomori (pathological social withdrawal) in young adults. PLoS ONE 2023, 18, e0281833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Carpita, B.; Nardi, B.; Giovannoni, F.; Parri, F.; Cerofolini, G.; Bonelli, C.; Amatori, G.; Massimetti, G.; Cremone, I.M.; Pini, S.; et al. Exploring the relationship among hikikomori tendencies, autistic traits, computer game use and eating disorder symptoms. CNS Spectr. 2024, 29, 670–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Dell’Osso, L.; Amatori, G.; Muti, D.; Giovannoni, F.; Parri, F.; Violi, M.; Cremone, I.M.; Carpita, B. Autism Spectrum, Hikikomori Syndrome and Internet Gaming Disorder: Is There a Link? Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Alonso-Esteban, Y.; López-Ramón, M.F.; Moreno-Campos, V.; Navarro-Pardo, E.; Alcantud-Marín, F. A Systematic Review on the Impact of the Social Confinement on People with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Their Caregivers during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Kwan, C.; Gitimoghaddam, M.; Collet, J.P. Effects of Social Isolation and Loneliness in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disabilities: A Scoping Review. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hall, J.A.; Horgan, T.G.; Murphy, N.A. Nonverbal Communication. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2019, 70, 271–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Krieg, A.; Dickie, J.R. Attachment and hikikomori: A psychosocial developmental model. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2013, 59, 61–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Ding, N.; Zhang, X. Bullying Victimization and Quality of Life among Chinese Adolescents: An Integrative Analysis of Internet Addiction and Social Withdrawal. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wakuta, M.; Nishimura, T.; Osuka, Y.; Tsukui, N.; Takahashi, M.; Adachi, M.; Suwa, T.; Katayama, T. Adverse childhood experiences: Impacts on adult mental health and social withdrawal. Front. Public Health 2023, 11, 1277766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Schultz, R.T.; Gauthier, I.; Klin, A.; Fulbright, R.K.; Anderson, A.W.; Volkmar, F.; Skudlarski, P.; Lacadie, C.; Cohen, D.J.; Gore, J.C. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2000, 57, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bachevalier, J.; Loveland, K.A. The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006, 30, 97–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Scott-Van Zeeland, A.A.; Dapretto, M.; Ghahremani, D.G.; Poldrack, R.A.; Bookheimer, S.Y. Reward processing in autism. Autism Res. 2010, 3, 53–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Chevallier, C.; Kohls, G.; Troiani, V.; Brodkin, E.S.; Schultz, R.T. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Modi, M.E.; Young, L.J. The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: Animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Horm. Behav. 2012, 61, 340–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Teo, A.R. Social isolation associated with depression: A case report of hikikomori. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2013, 59, 339–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Taylor, M. Strategies of dissociation: A mimetic dimension to social problems in Japan. Anthropoetics 2006, 12, 11. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lee, Y.S.; Lee, J.Y.; Choi, T.Y.; Choi, J.T. Home visitation program for detecting, evaluating and treating socially withdrawn youth in Korea. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2013, 67, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. (a): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for AdAS domains depending on presence of hikikomori and ATs. (b): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for HQ-25 domains depending on presence of hikikomori and AT. (c): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for AdAS Spectrum total score depending on presence of hikikomori and Ats. (d): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for HQ-25 total score depending on presence of hikikomori and Ats.
Figure 1. (a): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for AdAS domains depending on presence of hikikomori and ATs. (b): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for HQ-25 domains depending on presence of hikikomori and AT. (c): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for AdAS Spectrum total score depending on presence of hikikomori and Ats. (d): Profile plot of estimated marginal means for HQ-25 total score depending on presence of hikikomori and Ats.
Brainsci 15 00496 g001aBrainsci 15 00496 g001b
Figure 2. Comparison of AdAS Spectrum domains score among groups.
Figure 2. Comparison of AdAS Spectrum domains score among groups.
Brainsci 15 00496 g002
Figure 3. Comparison of HQ-25 domains’ score among groups.
Figure 3. Comparison of HQ-25 domains’ score among groups.
Brainsci 15 00496 g003
Table 1. Age and gender comparison between groups.
Table 1. Age and gender comparison between groups.
HCs
Mean ± SD
n = 550
HK
Mean ± SD
n = 118
AT
Mean ± SD
n = 818
AT-HK
Mean ± SD
n = 821
F p
Age25.23 ± 6.3526.08 ± 9.2823.87 ± 5.2524.76 ± 5.858.344<0.001 *
n(%)n(%)n(%)n(%)Chi-squarep
GenderF316 a (57.5%) 61 b (51.7%)449 b (54.9%)410 a (49.9%)8.4280.038 *
M234 b (42.5%)57 b (48.3%)369 b (45.1%)411 b (50.1%)
* Significant for p < 0.05; * HCs, HK, AT-HK > AT. Each superscript letter indicates a subset of categories in which the row proportions are not different from each other.
Table 2. (a) Results from the two MANOVAs with the AdAS Spectrum domains and HQ-25 domains as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables. (b) Factorial ANOVA with AdAS Spectrum total score as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables. (c) Factorial ANOVA with HQ-25 total score as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables.
Table 2. (a) Results from the two MANOVAs with the AdAS Spectrum domains and HQ-25 domains as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables. (b) Factorial ANOVA with AdAS Spectrum total score as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables. (c) Factorial ANOVA with HQ-25 total score as dependent variables and presence of significant ATs and hikikomori as independent variables.
(a)
SourceDependent VariableType III Sum of SquaresdfMean SquareFp
Corrected ModelAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.13,812.18134604.060445.208<0.001 *
Verb. comm.9756.57133252.190451.629<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.23,747.78837915.929606.636<0.001 *
Empathy3137.98331045.994217.743<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine54,413.152318,137.717566.395<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.22,224.70537408.235672.611<0.001 *
Hyper-hyporeact.8942.78432980.928326.227<0.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization152,004.066350,668.0221234.124<0.001 *
Isolation70,214.727323,404.9091134.953<0.001 *
Emotional support17,129.49935709.833381.433<0.001 *
InterceptAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.66,469.476166,469.4766427.534<0.001 *
Verb. comm.35,970.629135,970.6294995.214<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.123,677.9071123,677.9079478.033<0.001 *
Empathy11,065.245111,065.2452302.440<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine203,188.2641203,188.2646345.060<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.78,145.272178,145.2727094.997<0.001 *
Hyper-hyporeact.20,717.238120,717.2382267.254<0.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization435,825.1261435,825.12610,615.419<0.001 *
Isolation211,012.7471211,012.74710,232.446<0.001 *
Emotional support73,062.762173,062.7624880.799<0.001 *
HikikomoriAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.888.5911888.59185.926<0.001 *
Verb. comm.613.8471613.84785.244<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.1037.60611037.60679.517<0.001 *
Empathy172.3221172.32235.872<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine414.8611414.86112.955<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.439.4931439.49339.903<0.001 *
Hyper-hyporeact.107.8111107.81111.7990.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization72,216.835172,216.8351758.990<0.001 *
Isolation34,208.393134,208.3931658.836<0.001 *
Emotional support9145.71919145.719610.960<0.001 *
Significant ATsAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.6605.89216605.892638.783<0.001 *
Verb. comm.4678.82514678.825649.745<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.12,358.944112,358.944947.125<0.001 *
Empathy1562.48611562.486325.261<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine34,552.256134,552.2561078.980<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.13,016.314113,016.3141181.782<0.001 *
Hyper-hyporeact.5548.49115548.491607.16<0.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization4686.35114686.351114.146<0.001 *
Isolation1356.00611356.00665.756<0.001 *
Emotional support167.4591167.45911.1870.001 *
Hikikomori * Significant ATsAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.97.697197.6979.4470.002 *
Verb. comm.124.3491124.34917.268<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.39.189139.1893.0030.083
Empathy33.545133.5456.9830.008 *
Inflex. and routine399.6721399.67212.481<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.89.889189.8898.1610.004 *
Hyper-hyporeact.115.2741115.27412.615<0.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization8.52618.5260.2080.649
Isolation9.46519.4650.4590.498
Emotional support15.115115.1151.0100.315
ErrorAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.23,816.163230310.341
Verb. comm.16,583.94823037.201
Non verb. comm.30,051.617230313.049
Empathy11,063.13423034.804
Inflex. and routine73,749.114230332.023
Restrict. interest and rum.25,365.558230311.014
Hyper-hyporeact.21,043.87023039.138
HQ-25
Socialization94,551.640230341.056
Isolation47,492.298230320.622
Emotional support34,474.587230314.969
TotalAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.190,278.0002307
Verb. comm.111,956.0002307
Non verb. comm.338,318.0002307
Empathy40,851.0002307
Inflex. and routine641,911.0002307
Restrict. interest and rum.242,057.0002307
Hyper-hyporeact.86,458.0002307
HQ-25
Socialization994,960.0002307
Isolation474,846.0002307
Emotional support173,087.0002307
Corrected totalAdAS Spectrum
Child./adolesc.37,628.3442306
Verb. comm.26,340.5182306
Non verb. comm.53,799.4042306
Empathy14,201.1172306
Inflex. and routine128,162.2662306
Restrict. interest and rum.47,590.2642306
Hyper-hyporeact.29,986.6542306
HQ-25
Socialization246,555.7072306
Isolation117,707.0252306
Emotional support51,604.0872306
(b)
SourceType III Sum of SquaresdfMean SquareFp
Corrected model808,948.5073269,649.5021014.4800.000 *
Intercept3,163,381.85213,163,381.85211,901.3340.000 *
Significant AT455,405.3541455,405.3541713.3340.000 *
Hikikomori22,994.576122,994.57686.5110.000 *
Significant AT * Hikikomori5372.39215372.39220.2120.000 *
Error612,138.8232303265.801
Total9,123,597.0002307
Corrected total1,421,087.3302306
(c)
SourceType III Sum of SquaresdFMean SquareFp
Corrected model616,619.2893205,539.7631889.5190.000 *
Intercept1,931,635.79211,931,635.79217,757.4560.000 *
Significant AT13,976.323113,976.323128.4840.000 *
Hikikomori301,752.7551301,752.7552774.0020.000 *
adascut43 * HQcut13.921113.9210.1280.721
Error250,517.7092303108.779
Total4,147,795.0002307
Corrected total867,136.9992306
(a) *: Statistically significant value (p < 0.05); df: degrees of freedom; (b) *: statistically significant value (p < 0.05); df: degrees of freedom; R2 = 0.569; adjusted R2 = 0.569. (c) *: statistically significant value (p < 0.05); df: degrees of freedom; R2 = 0.711; adjusted R2 = −0.711.
Table 3. Comparison of AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 scores among groups.
Table 3. Comparison of AdAS Spectrum and HQ-25 scores among groups.
HCs
Mean ± SD
n = 550
HK
Mean ± SD
n = 118
AT
Mean ± SD
n = 818
AT-HK
Mean ± SD
n = 821
F p
AdAS Spectrum scores
Child./adolesc.4.42 ± 2.415.54 ± 2.618.45 ± 3.2910.68 ± 3.66445.208<0.001 *
Verb. comm.3.04 ± 1.743.80 ± 2.046.27 ± 2.788.29 ± 3.14451.629<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.6.05 ± 2.667.52 ± 2.6011.97 ± 3.9814.14 ± 3.89606.636<0.001 °
Empathy1.64 ± 1.522.06 ± 1.803.55 ± 2.254.62 ± 2.54217.743<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine7.46 ± 3.727.48 ± 3.4316.82 ± 5.8119.10 ± 6.73566.395<0.001 *
Restrict. interest and rum.4.34 ± 2.614.99 ± 2.4310.24 ± 3.3211.96 ± 3.81672.611<0.001 *
Hyper-hyporeact.1.97 ± 1.841.95 ± 1.895.57 ± 3.096.76 ± 3.65326.277<0.001 *
Total score28.93 ± 9.4533.35 ± 7.0362.87 ± 16.4675.56 ± 20.241014.480<0.001 *
HQ-25
Socialization9.03 ± 5.9124.35 ± 6.1113.06 ± 6.1328.05 ± 7.011234.124<0.001 ^
Isolation6.79 ± 4.2217.05 ± 4.388.69 ± 4.1719.30 ± 5.091134.953<0.001 ^
Emotional support4.45 ± 3.2110.07 ± 3.605.40 ± 3.6910.58 ± 4.44381.433<0.001 §
Total score20.27 ± 10.2651.47 ± 8.6627.15 ± 9.5857.93 ± 11.531889.519<0.001 ^
* AT-HK > AT > HK, HCs; ° AT-HK > AT > HK > HCs; ^ AT-HK > HK > AT > HCs; §: AT-HK, HK > AT > HCs; significant for p < 0.05.
Table 4. Linear regression analysis with the presence of higher AdAS Spectrum scores as a dependent variable and HQ-25 domains as independent variables, carried in the HK group.
Table 4. Linear regression analysis with the presence of higher AdAS Spectrum scores as a dependent variable and HQ-25 domains as independent variables, carried in the HK group.
B (S.E) BETA t p
Constant27.405 (3.645) 7.519<0.001 *
Socialization1.090 (0.112)0.3239.748<0.001 *
Isolation0.547 (0.158)0.1173.4510.001 *
Emotional support0.227 (0.169)0.0421.3410.180
* Significant for p < 0.05; R2 = 0.151; Adjusted R2 = 0.148.
Table 5. Linear regression analysis with higher HQ-25 scores as a dependent variable and AdAS Spectrum domains as independent variables, carried in the AT group.
Table 5. Linear regression analysis with higher HQ-25 scores as a dependent variable and AdAS Spectrum domains as independent variables, carried in the AT group.
B (S.E.) BETA t p
constant11.968 (1.618) 7.399<0.001 *
Child./adolesc.1.044 (0.132)0.2047.913<0.001 *
Verb. comm.1.247 (0.170)0.2097.339<0.001 *
Non verb. comm.0.617 (0.123)0.1355.030<0.001 *
Empathy0.792 (0.185)0.1044.280<0.001 *
Inflex. and routine−0.249 (0.089)−0.085−2.7870.005
Restrict. interest and rum.0.481 (0.151)0.0953.1940.001
Hyper-hyporeact.−0.104 (0.151)−0.019−0.6860.493
* Significant for p < 0.05; R2 = 0.242; Adjusted R2 = 0.239.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dell’Osso, L.; Nardi, B.; Muti, D.; De Felice, C.; Tognini, V.; Parri, F.; Giovannoni, F.; Del Grande, F.; Bonelli, C.; Massimetti, G.; et al. Comorbidity Between Hikikomori and Autistic Traits May Be Identified as a Phenotypical Presentation Characterized by Greater Severity. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 496. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050496

AMA Style

Dell’Osso L, Nardi B, Muti D, De Felice C, Tognini V, Parri F, Giovannoni F, Del Grande F, Bonelli C, Massimetti G, et al. Comorbidity Between Hikikomori and Autistic Traits May Be Identified as a Phenotypical Presentation Characterized by Greater Severity. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(5):496. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050496

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dell’Osso, Liliana, Benedetta Nardi, Dario Muti, Chiara De Felice, Valeria Tognini, Francesca Parri, Federico Giovannoni, Filippo Del Grande, Chiara Bonelli, Gabriele Massimetti, and et al. 2025. "Comorbidity Between Hikikomori and Autistic Traits May Be Identified as a Phenotypical Presentation Characterized by Greater Severity" Brain Sciences 15, no. 5: 496. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050496

APA Style

Dell’Osso, L., Nardi, B., Muti, D., De Felice, C., Tognini, V., Parri, F., Giovannoni, F., Del Grande, F., Bonelli, C., Massimetti, G., Pini, S., Fiorillo, A., & Carpita, B. (2025). Comorbidity Between Hikikomori and Autistic Traits May Be Identified as a Phenotypical Presentation Characterized by Greater Severity. Brain Sciences, 15(5), 496. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050496

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop