Familiar Music Reduces Mind Wandering and Boosts Behavioral Performance During Lexical Semantic Processing †
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Design
2.2. Measures
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Lexical Semantic Judgments: Response Times and Accuracy
3.2. Mind Wandering Frequency
3.3. Musical Training, Familiarity, and Lexical-Semantic Response Times
3.4. Musical Training, Familiarity Condition, and Mind Wandering
3.5. Familiarity, Distractibility, and Enjoyment
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Croom, A. Music practice and participation for psychological well-being: A review of how music influences positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment. Music. Scentae 2015, 19, 44–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haake, A.B. Individual music listening in workplace settings: An exploratory survey of offices in the UK. Music. Sci. 2011, 15, 107–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pereira, C.S.; Teixeira, J.; Figueiredo, P.; Xavier, J.; Castro, S.L.; Brattico, E. Music and emotions in the brain: Familiarity matters. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e27241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moreno, S.; Bidelman, G.M. Understanding neural plasticity and cognitive benefit through the unique lens of musical training. Hear. Res. 2014, 308, 84–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perham, N.; Lewis, A.; Turner, J.; Hodgetts, H.M. The sound of silence: Can imagining music elicit the mood and arousal effect? Curr. Psychol. 2013, 33, 610–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schellenberg, E.G. Cognitive Performance After Music Listening: A Review of the Mozart Effect. In Music, Health and Wellbeing; MacDonald, R.A.R., Kreutz, G., Mitchel, L., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Dosseville, F.; Laborde, S.; Scelles, N. Music during lectures: Will students learn better? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 258–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feng, S.; Bidelman, G.M. Music Listening and Song Familiarity Modulate Mind Wandering and Behavioral Success During Lexical Processing. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2015), Pasadena, CA, USA, 22–25 July 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Smallwood, J.; Schooler, J.W. The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 946–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinson, D.B.; Smallwood, J.; Davidson, R.J. The persistence of thought: Evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 375–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smallwood, J.; Davies, J.B.; Heim, D.; Finnigan, F.; Sudberry, M.; O’Connor, R.; Obonsawin, M. Subjective experience and the attentional lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained attention. Conscious. Cogn. 2004, 4, 657–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smallwood, J.; Baraciaia, S.F.; Lowe, M.; Obonsawin, M.C. Task unrelated thought whilst encoding information. Conscious. Cogn. 2003, 12, 452–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smallwood, J. Mind-wandering while reading: Attentional decoupling, mindless reading and the cascade model of inattention. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2011, 5, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feng, S.; D’Mello, S.K.; Graesser, A. Mind wandering while reading easy and difficult texts. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2013, 20, 586–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, W.F.; Schellenberg, E.G.; Letnic, A.K. Fast and loud background music disrupts reading comprehension. Psychol. Music. 2011, 40, 700–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kane, M.J.; Brown, L.H.; McVay, J.C.; Silvia, P.J.; Myin-Germeys, I.; Kwapil, T.R. For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience sampling study of working memory and executive control in everyday life. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 614–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McVay, J.C.; Kane, M.J. Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 188–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unworth, N.; McMillan, B.D. Similarities and differences between mind-wandering and external distraction: A latent variable analysis of lapses of attention and their relation to cognitive abilities. Acta Psychol. 2014, 150, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, J.A.; Bidelman, G.M. Song properties and familiarity affect speech recognition in musical noise. Psychomusicology Music Mind Brain 2022, 32, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsh, J.E.; Hughes, R.W.; Jones, D.M. Interference by process, not content, determines semantic auditory distraction. Cognition 2009, 110, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perham, N.; Currie, H. Does listening to preferred music improve reading comprehension performance? Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 28, 279–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, A.D. Music, Language, and the Brain; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Slevc, L.R.; Rosenberg, J.C.; Patel, A.D. Making psycholinguistics musical: Self-paced reading time evidence for shared processing of linguistic and musical syntax. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2009, 16, 374–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koelsch, S.; Gunter, T.C.; Wittfoth, M.; Sammler, D. Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music: An ERP Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1565–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kunert, R.; Willems, R.M.; Hagoort, P. Language influences music harmony perception: Effects of shared syntactic integration resources beyond attention. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 150685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maess, B.; Koelsch, S.; Gunter, T.C.; Friederici, A.D. Musical syntax is processed in Broca’s area: An MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 540–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinbeis, N.; Koelsch, S. Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 1169–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goltz, F.; Sadakata, M. Do you listen to music while studying? A portrait of how people use music to optimize their cognitive performance. Acta Psychol. 2021, 220, 103417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, J.A.; Bidelman, G.M. Familiarity of background music modulates the cortical tracking of target speech at the “cocktail party”. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- David, F.N. Tables of the Ordinates and Probability Integral of the Distribution of the Correlation Coefficient in Small Samples; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1938. [Google Scholar]
- Russo, F.A.; Pichora-Fuller, M.K. Tune in or Tune Out: Age-Related Differences in Listening to Speech in Music. Ear Hear. 2008, 29, 746–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Relander, K.; Rama, P.; Kujala, T. Word semantics is processed even without attentional effort. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 21, 1511–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2010; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 April 2025).
- Bates, D.M.; Maechler, M. lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using S4 Classes Software. 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Tierney, A.; Kraus, N. Music training for the development of reading skills. Prog. Brain Res. 2013, 207, 209–241. [Google Scholar]
- Bosch, I.; Salimpoor, V.N.; Zatorre, R. Familiarity mediates the relationship between emotional arousal and pleasure during music listening. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 534. [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy, G.; MacDonald, R. The effects of music choice on task performance: A study of the impact of self-selected and experimenter-selected music on driving game performance and experience. Music. Sci. 2009, 13, 357–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Etaugh, C.; Michals, D. Effects on reading comprehension of preferred music and frequency of studying to music. Percept. Mot. Skills 1975, 41, 553–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, J.A.; Bidelman, G.M. Attention, musicality, and familiarity shape cortical speech tracking at the musical cocktail party. Brain Lang. 2025, 266, 105581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Janata, P.; Tomic, S.T.; Rakowski, S.K. Characterisation of music-evoked autobiographical memories. Memory 2007, 15, 845–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eastwood, J.D.; Frischen, A.; Fenske, M.J.; Smilek, D. The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 7, 482–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smallwood, J.; Fitzgerald, A.; Miles, L.K.; Phillips, L.H. Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion 2009, 9, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coulson, A.N.; Burke, B.M. Creativity in the elementary music classroom: A study of students’ perceptions. Int. J. Music. Educ. 2013, 31, 428–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slevc, R.L.; Miyake, A. Individual differences in second-language proficiency: Does musical ability matter? Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 675–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swaminathan, S.; Gopinath, J.K. Music training and second-language English comprehension and vocabulary skills in Indian children. Psychol. Stud. 2013, 58, 164–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overy, K. Dyslexia, temporal processing and music: The potential of music as an early learning aid for dyslexic children. Psychol. Music. 2000, 28, 218–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhide, A.; Power, A.; Goswami, U. A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: A comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention. Mind Brain Educ. 2013, 7, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ritter, S.M.; Ferguson, S. Happy creativity: Listening to happy music facilitates divergent thinking. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0182210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bidelman, G.M.; Feng, S. Familiar Music Reduces Mind Wandering and Boosts Behavioral Performance During Lexical Semantic Processing. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 482. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050482
Bidelman GM, Feng S. Familiar Music Reduces Mind Wandering and Boosts Behavioral Performance During Lexical Semantic Processing. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(5):482. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050482
Chicago/Turabian StyleBidelman, Gavin M., and Shi Feng. 2025. "Familiar Music Reduces Mind Wandering and Boosts Behavioral Performance During Lexical Semantic Processing" Brain Sciences 15, no. 5: 482. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050482
APA StyleBidelman, G. M., & Feng, S. (2025). Familiar Music Reduces Mind Wandering and Boosts Behavioral Performance During Lexical Semantic Processing. Brain Sciences, 15(5), 482. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15050482