Next Article in Journal
Current State-of-the-Art Animal Models of Pediatric Brain Tumors
Previous Article in Journal
Confusion Assessment Protocol: Italian Cross-Cultural Adaptation and Validation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Detection of EEG Activity in Response to the Surrounding Environment: A Neuro-Architecture Study

by
Jesús S. García-Salinas
1,*,†,
Anna Wróblewska
1,2,† and
Michal T. Kucewicz
1,3,4
1
Brain and Mind Electrophysiology Laboratory, Multimedia Systems Department, Faculty of Electronics, Telecommunications and Informatics, BioTechMed Center, Gdansk University of Technology, 80-222 Gdansk, Poland
2
Department of Environmental Design, Faculty of Architecture, Gdansk University of Technology, 80-222 Gdansk, Poland
3
Department of Physiology and Biomedical Engineering, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN 55905, USA
4
Department of Neurology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN 55905, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2025, 15(10), 1103; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101103
Submission received: 10 September 2025 / Revised: 3 October 2025 / Accepted: 4 October 2025 / Published: 14 October 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Neurotechnology and Neuroimaging)

Abstract

Background: The external environment continuously shapes human perception, often without conscious awareness. This influence can be objectively studied using non-invasive recordings of brain activity in controlled virtual reality (VR) settings. We hypothesized that brief presentations of natural features would elicit distinct neural oscillatory patterns detectable through changes in the spectral power of resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) activity in specific brain regions. Methods: To test this, participants passively viewed three minimalist VR environments—with and without biophilic elements—while their EEG was recorded. Results: Our results revealed consistent changes in spectral power, particularly suppression in the alpha band and an increase in the theta band in the occipital cortex. Conclusions: These findings support the use of resting-state EEGs in immersive VR as a promising and objective method for evaluating how specific design elements influence neural activity, offering valuable insights into the field of neuro-architecture.

1. Introduction

The human brain is in a continuous interaction with its surrounding environment, regardless of whether we are aware of it. An immense array of external stimuli affect thoughts, emotional states, and behaviors [1]. The aspects of our surroundings, designed and constructed by humans are known as the built environment, where we spend up to 87% of our lifetime [2], with little conscious awareness of its particular elements. Studying neural responses to such subconscious external stimuli can have a profound impact on architectural design and our understanding of the visual perception of spaces.
A key dimension of this interaction involves our intrinsic connection to nature. According to biophilic theory, humans are biologically predisposed to require contact with natural elements [3]. Hence, a well-designed built environment should fulfill this fundamental need, which originates from our evolutionary history and adaptation to the natural world [4,5]. When urban designs lack this connection, they can evoke solastalgia, a distressing feeling caused by the direct experience of the negative transformation or desolation of the physical environment [6,7]. The absence of natural elements in the environment has been shown to have a profound impact on well-being and emotions [8,9,10]. It has been reported that patients in hospital rooms with a view of natural surroundings not only recover faster but also require less pain medication and experience fewer post-operative complications [11]. A broad analysis of hospital rooms found that certain architectural design elements, including natural features such as bedside views of nature or images on screens and increased daylight, can alleviate patient stress and pain while decreasing the hospital stay duration [12]. Stress Reduction Theory proposes that exposure to natural environments has a stress-lowering effect [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. Moreover, Attention Restoration Theory proposes that natural elements effortlessly and involuntarily attract attention, thereby restoring cognitive functions and reducing stress [15,21,22,23]. Through questionnaire research and the measurement of subjective feelings, other studies have found a relationship between physical space, the proportions of corridors in buildings, and emotional responses [24].
Neuro-architecture is an emerging approach that explores the interaction between the environment and brain activity, and it has been used in previous studies to identify neural correlates of viewing photographs of natural scenes in forests or urban greenery [25,26,27,28]. A more systematic and immersive approach proposes the use of virtual reality (VR), which enables the investigation of neural responses to built environments with and without natural elements. Unlike static images, VR allows dynamic interaction with the environment and provides greater experimental control over environmental stimuli. The inclusion of immersive VR enhances the ability to study environmental effects in a controlled, yet highly realistic, manner that is able to induce an emotional response [29,30,31,32,33]. With the combination of EEGs and VR, our approach attempts to capture an objective measure of the neural activity associated with these psychological theories, bridging subjective reports with physiological data. By simulating built environments with different elements, researchers can systematically analyze the cognitive and emotional effects of particular architectural elements and features. This multidisciplinary approach bridges the gap between neuroscience and architecture, fostering the development of evidence-based design strategies aimed at improving human well-being.
Recent advancements in computational neuroscience have enabled the extraction of complex patterns from EEG data, thereby providing deeper insights into the neural mechanisms associated with architectural perception. The integration of physiological measures such as heart rate variability and skin conductance responses complements neuroimaging findings, offering a holistic perspective on how built environments influence mental states. By adopting an interdisciplinary approach that incorporates psychology, architecture, and neuroscience, neuro-architectural research has the potential to redefine how spaces should be designed to have a positive effect on cognitive function, mood, and overall quality of life.
Previous research on the effects of the natural environment has typically relied on questionnaires, indirect behavioral responses, or general outcomes, such as patient recovery time [11,12,34,35,36,37,38]. However, a psychological and physiological approach measuring responses to green walls in VR scenarios [31] concluded that such elements could reduce anxiety, with observed increases in alpha activity in the parietal and occipital lobes. Notably, they found that excessively large green walls may have the opposite effect, producing alpha decreases and stress in the participants. Therefore, similar elements produce different responses according to their features. In addition, visual stimulation from plants has also been associated with increased relative theta power in the occipital lobe [39], a response that has been linked to both enhanced visual processing and relaxation states.
Based on these findings, this study focused on the detection of neurophysiological changes produced by green elements in virtual architectural spaces using EEGs. We hypothesized that brief presentations of natural features can elicit distinct neural oscillatory patterns detectable through simple EEG analyses. This suggests that straightforward neurophysiological assessments can provide objective criteria to inform early-stage design decisions. Furthermore, we expect these effects to vary depending on the scale and integration of green elements within the space. By confirming these neurophysiological responses in a controlled VR environment, our work supports the use of immersive evaluations as a practical tool for architects, urban planners, and designers to assess the cognitive and emotional impacts of natural elements prior to construction.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The study involved 22 volunteer participants, 10 males and 12 females, with a mean age and standard deviation (SD) of 28.70 ± 4.04. All participants were healthy adults with no history of neurological or psychiatric brain disorders. Informed consent was obtained from all participants, ethical approval was provided by the local ethics council at the Gdansk University of Technology, and the research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines established in the Declaration of Helsinki. Exclusion criteria were considered, such as consumption of caffeinated beverages or any medication, which could potentially affect the electroencephalography signals.

2.2. Experimental Procedure

The experiments were evenly distributed across different hours of the day (10 a.m.–10 p.m.), with participants performing at different times. All sessions were conducted in a dedicated room. The participants were required to report any use of corrective eyewear to ensure optimal visual perception of the virtual environment. The use of eyeglasses was allowed, as the headgear accommodated them seamlessly without compromising the equipment setup or fidelity of the displayed virtual environment. To minimize artifacts in the EEG recordings, participants were instructed to remain seated and refrain from movement. After an adaptation period to the VR equipment of approximately 3 min, we confirmed with each participant that none of them reported symptoms of motion sickness while wearing the virtual reality goggles.
We analyzed three environments that shared a common structure and differed only in specific elements of the design or their lack (Figure 1a). They were presented through virtual reality technology while participants were sitting in a chair in a 3 m × 2.5 m × 4 m (width × height × length) room. The virtual environments were designed to have similar measurements as the real room. Environment 1 (Env1) had a gray-scale color of the base elements of the ceiling, floor, and walls, and it was free of any other elements. After immersion in Env1, the features of the virtual environment were changed. In Environment 2 (Env2), there was a window with a forest view that appeared in front of the participant, and in Environment 3 (Env3), a wall with plants was presented, which represented indoor greenery.
The procedure involved repeatedly changing the environment every 10 s, starting with alternating between Env1 and 2, or Env1 and 3, followed by a 5-s break with a black screen (Figure 1a). The procedure focused on stationary perception over exploration; no specific task was required for the participants to do; only the perceptual response to the architectural elements was the object of study. Different virtual environments were presented, with the head facing straight and with no lateral movements. The total duration of the experiment was 445 s. All results are presented as mean ± S.E.M. unless stated otherwise.

2.3. Equipment and Software

The experiment was conducted in a virtual reality (VR) environment designed to provide a highly immersive experience. Participants interacted with the VR environment using the Oculus Quest (Meta) headset with a resolution of 1832 × 1920 pixels per eye and a refresh rate of 90 Hz, ensuring smooth and realistic visual rendering. The research environment was developed in the Unity Engine, allowing precise control over experimental conditions and stimulus presentation.
EEG signals were recorded using a 19-channel eWave-24 Science Beam EEG system at a sampling rate of 500 Hz. Wet electrodes with conductive gel were placed according to the 10–20 international system at the following scalp locations: FP1, FP2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T3, T4, T5, T6, FZ, CZ, and PZ (Figure A1). A linked ear reference was used to minimize potential reference noise. Data pre-processing and analysis were performed in MATLAB 2023a (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) using the EEGLAB toolbox [40]. Figure 1b illustrates a 10-s example after artifact removal, and the subsequent feature extraction process.
Finite Impulse Response (FIR) band-pass filtering was applied to all 19 EEG channels, restricting the signal bandwidth to 1–48 Hz. This pre-processing step effectively removed low-frequency drifts and high-frequency noise, including the 50 Hz electrical line interference. Electrodes that exhibited malfunctions or disconnections during recording, an average and SD of 0.9 ± 0.86, were removed, and their signal was interpolated.
To enhance the signal quality, Independent Component Analysis (ICA) was applied to decompose the EEG data, followed by an automatic artifact detection method called Iclabel [41]. Components associated with muscle activity, eye movements, and cardiac signals were identified based on their spatial and spectral characteristics and subsequently removed, with an average and SD of 5.72 ± 1.9 rejected components. Additionally, visual inspection was conducted to ensure proper artifact removal.
The pre-processed signals were then segmented into 10-s trials. For each trial, the average power was computed within distinct EEG frequency bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), low beta (12–20 Hz), high beta (20–30 Hz), low gamma (30–39 Hz), and high gamma (39–48 Hz). Figure 1b shows an example of a topological map. These standard frequency ranges were chosen specifically for the study of cognitive functions and underlying neural activity [42], and the beta and gamma bands were subdivided to obtain a higher spectral resolution. While time–frequency methods can provide a more detailed characterization of non-stationary neural signals, the band power approach was selected in this study due to its simplicity, and its ability to facilitate comparability with previous EEG literature.
To account for inter-subject variability, the power values were normalized into z-scores across all electrodes within each frequency band, as defined in Equation (1).
Z i , f = X i , f μ f σ f
where Z i , f is the normalized power of electrode i in frequency band f, X i , f is the band power, μ f is the mean band power, and σ f is the standard deviation of the band power.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB 2023a software (MathWorks Inc.). To identify significant differences in neural activity across the environments, we used the Wilcoxon signed rank test (alpha threshold of 0.05) for each electrode and frequency band. Samples from the compared environments were paired for each participant (N = 22). The resulting values were visualized as topographical maps, remarking the electrodes with significant differences for each frequency band. To control for multiple comparisons, the Bonferroni correction was applied to the p-values obtained for each frequency band. The minimum detectable effect size (Cohen’s dz) at 80% power was estimated to be approximately 0.34, indicating that the study is sensitive to moderate or larger differences between environments.

3. Results

To identify specific EEG activity changes in response to biophilic elements, we conducted a comparative spectral power analysis across seven frequency bands, averaging the results of all subjects, during passive virtual reality viewing of three distinct environments, with and without biophilic elements (Figure 2). There was a common pattern of the low- and the high-frequency power distribution across all three environments studied. In the lower-frequency bands (delta, theta, alpha), spectral power was the highest in the occipital, central, and frontal lobes, while the parietal and temporal regions exhibited lower activity levels. In the higher-frequency bands (beta, gamma), power was relatively higher in the occipital, temporal, and frontal lobes but lower in the central, parietal, and anterior prefrontal regions.
To effectively visualize the differences in brain activity between the environments, the subtraction of band powers between environments were computed (Figure 3). The differences between Env1 (non-biophilic) and Env2, as well as between Env1 and Env3, showed a similar pattern across frequency bands. In the delta band, differences were observed primarily in the occipital and frontal lobes. In the alpha and beta bands, greater differences were found in the central and occipital lobes. In the gamma band, smaller differences were observed in the frontal, central, and occipital lobes. These findings indicate general brain regions where differences in the EEG spectral activity were localized between the environments with and without a biophilic element across various frequency bands.
Environments containing biophilic elements showed significant differences compared to the non-biophilic environment in the theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands, especially in the posterior visual areas of the occipital and parietal cortices (Figure 4a; see Table A1, Table A2 and Table A3 for electrode-specific p-values). We summarized all significant differences based on their polarity (higher or lower power), location, and frequency band at the level of specific EEG electrode contacts (Figure 4). In general, the environment without biophilic elements exhibited significantly higher alpha and beta spectral power at electrode locations within the identified posterior areas of interest. The alpha and beta power responses in the visual areas were consistently observed across the same group of electrode contacts, whereas responses in other frequency bands and regions were limited to individual electrode contacts. To ensure that these findings were not due to chance of multiple comparisons, Bonferroni correction was applied, which preserved the significant difference in occipital alpha power for Env1 when compared to Env2 and Env3 (Figure 4b).
Finally, we averaged the relative band power of all the electrodes in each of the seven areas to compare the identified alpha responses with other frequency ranges of the EEG spectrum. This activity revealed consistent patterns across cortical areas and the frequency bands (Figure 5a); the temporal areas in both hemispheres showed less power in the low-frequency bands (delta, theta) with gradually more power at higher frequencies. An opposite pattern of more power in the low-frequency bands, with gradually less power at high frequencies, was observed in the parietal and central areas. A similar pattern of higher power in the low-frequency bands dropping in the alpha, beta, and higher frequencies was consistent among the anterior frontopolar and frontocentral areas; the occipital areas were characterized by a relatively higher power across the entire spectrum (Figure 5b).
These differences are consistent with electrodes detected in statistical tests (Figure 4a,b). The identified posterior alpha power was the highest across all other EEG activities and brain regions. Switching between environments with and without a biophilic element induced the largest differences selectively in the occipital alpha activity (Figure 5b), revealing a significant suppression of power by introducing a biophilic element. Specifically, alpha activity was reduced at the O2 channel for Env1 compared to Env2 (Wilcoxon signed rank test, w = 227, z = 3.2628, p = 0.001) and for Env1 compared to Env3 (Wilcoxon signed rank test, w = 241, z = 3.7173, p = 0.0002). A similar suppression was observed in the central region at electrode channel C4, where alpha activity decreased significantly between Env1 and Env2 (Wilcoxon signed rank test, w = 25, z = −3.2953, p = 0.001). Apart from this main effect, there was also a significantly greater power in the occipital theta activity induced by introducing the biophilic elements. The theta power was higher at the same O2 channel for Env1 compared to Env2 (Wilcoxon signed rank test, w = 59, z = −2.1914, p = 0.0284) and for Env1 compared to Env3 (Wilcoxon signed rank test, w = 14, z = −3.6524, p = 0.0003). No other effects of introducing biophilic elements were observed in any frequency band or cortical area (Figure 5a). Hence, we associate the suppression of posterior alpha and induction of occipital theta EEG activities at a specific electrode channel with a visual response to the biophilic elements introduced in the virtual environment.

4. Discussion

Our study identified neural signatures of exposure to distinct environments in architectural spaces using immersive VR and EEG. By reducing the number of design features to a minimal set, we isolated the effects of the added elements on cortical activity and found that brief exposure to them produced two consistent neural responses: suppression of occipital alpha power and enhancement of occipital theta power.
The observed suppression of alpha activity in posterior cortices likely reflects increased sensory processing and attentional engagement. Alpha oscillations are widely considered to be inhibitory gating rhythms that regulate information flow, with lower alpha indicating greater cortical excitability and more active visual processing [43,44,45,46,47,48]. In our paradigm, natural features may have captured attention in an involuntary but effortless manner, consistent with Attention Restoration Theory [15,21]. The fact that alpha suppression was localized to occipital regions further supports the interpretation that these effects are driven by visual rather than general arousal mechanisms.
At the same time, the introduction of natural elements enhanced occipital theta activity. Theta rhythms are associated with the perception of novel or emotionally salient stimuli, as well as with relaxation and meditative states. This dual role aligns well with Stress Reduction Theory, which proposes that exposure to nature lowers stress and promotes positive affect [13,16]. Together, the suppression of alpha and the induction of theta point to a coordinated modulation of inhibitory and excitatory rhythms in visual networks. Previous studies have shown modulation activity in alpha and theta during exposures to nature [39,49,50].
The results suggest that neural oscillations can serve as objective biomarkers for assessing the impact of architectural elements. Posterior alpha suppression and theta enhancement may provide early indicators of how environments influence visual engagement and emotional state. This has direct applications in evidence-based design: architects and urban planners could test configurations of greenery in VR and evaluate their cognitive and affective impact before construction. In healthcare settings, integrating natural views or plant-based design features could support stress reduction and recovery, consistent with classic studies on patient outcomes [11,12].
Moreover, the paradigm used here highlights the value of immersive VR combined with EEGs for prototyping. Unlike static questionnaires or recovery measures, this approach enables controlled manipulation of design variables while directly capturing neural responses. Future studies could expand on this by incorporating other sensory modalities (sound, scent, airflow) that often accompany real-world exposure to nature and by testing longer-term exposures to better evaluate chronic restorative effects.
The current results do not provide evidence to support the notion that the effects of biophilic elements might vary depending on their scale and spatial integration within the virtual environment. The observed EEG responses were consistently associated with the presence versus absence of biophilic elements, without a clear differentiation between the two biophilic environments (Env2 and Env3). This suggests that, at least within the parameters of the environments tested, the mere presence of green elements is sufficient to elicit measurable changes in visual cortical activity, while variations in their scale or integration do not appear to produce additional or graded neurophysiological effects. It is possible that the spatial differences between Env2 and Env3 were not large enough to generate detectable EEG distinctions or that the neural markers measured here are primarily sensitive to qualitative presence rather than quantitative differences. Future studies incorporating more systematically varied scales and arrangements of biophilic elements may clarify whether these factors influence the neural responses.
Several limitations should be noted. First, our study compared environments with and without natural elements but did not include additional non-biophilic features as controls. Therefore, some of the observed neural and cognitive responses may have been influenced by novelty effects or other uncontrolled environmental variables, rather than reflecting the intrinsic properties of natural stimuli alone. Second, the brief exposure windows of 10 s were sufficient to capture immediate neural responses but are unlikely to reflect longer-term restorative or affective effects associated with prolonged interaction with biophilic environments. Moreover, the total experimental session duration of 7.4 min, while relatively short, may have contributed to attention drift or participant fatigue, as maintaining sustained engagement over several minutes without variation can reduce focus and modulate EEG measures. Third, our VR environments lacked the multisensory richness of real-world settings, and participants remained stationary, limiting ecological validity. Future studies should explore diverse biophilic configurations, incorporate multimodal cues, and allow interaction or exploration of environments. Finally, to gain a deeper understanding of the underlying brain processes, more sophisticated and robust feature extraction techniques could be implemented. While the present work relies on conventional frequency band power analysis, future studies could extend the analysis to time–frequency decompositions and nonlinear signal processing methods to better capture the spatiotemporal dynamics of EEG activity [51,52,53,54].

5. Conclusions

This study shows that brief exposure to biophilic architectural features in immersive VR elicits distinct neural responses, i.e., suppressed occipital alpha and enhanced theta activity, indicating increased visual engagement and affective modulation. By combining EEG with VR, this study demonstrates a robust framework for evidence-based architectural design, enabling an objective assessment of how environments influence cognition and emotion.
Although no differences were observed between the scales of biophilic integration, future research should explore broader design variations, longer exposures, and multimodal cues to enhance ecological validity. These findings also open promising avenues for integrating neurophysiological data into architecture and urban design. Brain–Computer Interface (BCI) technologies could enable spaces to adapt in real time to users’ cognitive and emotional states, optimizing comfort and engagement. Beyond architecture, such adaptive environments may support rehabilitation and therapy by tailoring sensory inputs to reinforce restorative brain states.
Advancing this line of research will require the identification of reliable neural markers for environmental adaptation and evaluation of the long-term outcomes of such interventions. Ultimately, neuroarchitecture has the potential to create dynamic, human-centered environments that bridge neuroscience, design, and technology.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.W. and M.T.K.; methodology, J.S.G.-S.; software, J.S.G.-S.; validation, J.S.G.-S., A.W. and M.T.K.; formal analysis, J.S.G.-S.; investigation, J.S.G.-S. and A.W.; resources, A.W.; data curation, J.S.G.-S.; writing—original draft preparation, J.S.G.-S.; writing—review and editing, J.S.G.-S., A.W. and M.T.K.; visualization, J.S.G.-S.; supervision, M.T.K.; project administration, A.W. and M.T.K.; funding acquisition, M.T.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Science Centre, Poland, grant Opus LAP number 2020/39/I/NZ4/02070.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the local ethics council at the Gdansk University of Technology.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors upon request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank ScienceBeam company for making the EEG equipment available.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A. Supplementary Tables

Table A1. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env1 and Env2. Notice that the O2 channel showed significant differences across multiple frequency bands, and that the alpha band showed the greatest number of significant instances.
Table A1. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env1 and Env2. Notice that the O2 channel showed significant differences across multiple frequency bands, and that the alpha band showed the greatest number of significant instances.
Freq. BandsChannels
Delta O2
0.013
Theta O2
0.028
F7
0.033
T5
0.024
AlphaF4
0.005
C3
0.007
C4
0.001
P3
0.026
O1
0.008
O2
0.001
F8
0.036
T5
0.018
T6
0.002
CZ
0.003
Low Beta P3
0.013
T3
0.036
T4
0.039
High Gamma O2
0.046
Table A2. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env1 and Env3. The O2 channel presented significant differences across multiple frequency bands, and the alpha band showed the greatest number of significant instances.
Table A2. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env1 and Env3. The O2 channel presented significant differences across multiple frequency bands, and the alpha band showed the greatest number of significant instances.
Freq. BandsChannels
DeltaFP1
0.011
O2
0.006
Theta C3
0.026
C4
0.049
O2
0.0002
T5
0.0424
AlphaF3
0.031
F4
0.039
P4
0.028
O1
0.028
O2
0.0002
T6
0.024
Low Beta P3
0.006
P4
0.008
O1
0.022
O2
0.003
T3
0.0495
High Beta O2
0.039
Table A3. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env2 and Env3. The theta band exhibited multiple electrodes showing significant activity, including both O1 and O2.
Table A3. Summary of significant difference p-values found between Env2 and Env3. The theta band exhibited multiple electrodes showing significant activity, including both O1 and O2.
Freq. BandsChannels
Delta T4
0.039
ThetaC3
0.031
O1
0.031
O2
0.018
F7
0.045
T4
0.033
CZ
0.049
High Beta O2
0.039

Appendix B. Supplementary Figures

Figure A1. EEG electrode placement. Diagram showing the distribution of the 19 wet electrodes positioned according to the international 10–20 system.
Figure A1. EEG electrode placement. Diagram showing the distribution of the 19 wet electrodes positioned according to the international 10–20 system.
Brainsci 15 01103 g0a1

References

  1. Di Pompeo, I.; D’Aurizio, G.; Burattini, C.; Bisegna, F.; Curcio, G. Positive mood induction to promote well-being and health: A systematic review from real settings to virtual reality. J. Environ. Psychol. 2023, 91, 102095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Klepeis, N.; Nelson, W.; Ott, W.; Robinson, J.; Tsang, A.; Switzer, P.; Behar, J.; Hern, S.; Engelmann, W. The National Human Activity Pattern Survey (NHAPS): A resource for assessing exposure to environmental pollutants. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2001, 11, 231–252. Available online: http://www.nature.com/articles/7500165 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  3. Salingaros, N.; Masden, K. Chapter 5: Neuroscience, the Natural Environment, and Building Design. In Biophilic Design: The Theory, Science and Practice of Bringing Buildings to Life; John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2008; pp. 59–83. [Google Scholar]
  4. Chang, C.-C.; Cox, D.T.C.; Fan, Q.; Nghiem, T.; Tan, C.; Oh, R.; Lin, B.; Shanahan, D.; Fuller, R.; Gaston, K.; et al. People’s desire to be in nature and how they experience it are partially heritable. PLoS Biol. 2022, 20, e3001500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barbiero, G.; Berto, R. Biophilia as Evolutionary Adaptation: An Onto- and Phylogenetic Framework for Biophilic Design. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 700709. Available online: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700709/full (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Albrecht, G. Solastalgia. Altern. J. (AJ)-Canada’s Environ. Voice 2006, 32, 34–36. [Google Scholar]
  7. Sanders, S.; Deming, A.; Ray, S.; Brorby, T.; Passarello, E.; Atkinson, J.; Ray, J.; Bogard, P.; Wilkinson, M.; Shukla, P.; et al. Solastalgia: An Anthology of Emotion in a Disappearing World; University of Virginia Press: Charlottesville, VA, USA, 2023; Available online: http://muse.jhu.edu/pub/191/edited%5C_volume/book/109723 (accessed on 3 October 2025).
  8. Ulrich, R. Natural Versus Urban Scenes. Environ. Behav. 1981, 13, 523–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mayer, F.; Frantz, C.; Bruehlman-Senecal, E.; Dolliver, K. Why Is Nature Beneficial? Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 607–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ancora, L.; Blanco-Mora, D.; Alves, I.; Bonifácio, A.; Morgado, P.; Miranda, B. Cities and neuroscience research: A systematic literature review. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 983352. Available online: http://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.983352/full (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  11. Ulrich, R. View Through a Window May Influence Recovery from Surgery. Science 1984, 224, 420–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Ulrich, R.; Zimring, C.; Zhu, X.; DuBose, J.; Seo, H.; Choi, Y.; Quan, X.; Joseph, A. A Review of the Research Literature on Evidence-Based Healthcare Design. HERD Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2008, 1, 61–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ulrich, R.; Simons, R.; Losito, B.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272494405801847 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  14. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989; Volume 7, Available online: https://books.google.com/books/about/The%5C_Experience%5C_of%5C_Nature.html?hl=&id=7l80AAAAIAAJ (accessed on 3 October 2025).
  15. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15, 169–182. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0272494495900012 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  16. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and Health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hartig, T.; Mang, M.; Evans, G. Restorative Effects of Natural Environment Experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wells, N.; Evans, G. Nearby Nature. Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 311–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Grahn, P.; Stigsdotter, U. Landscape planning and stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2003, 2, 1–18. Available online: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1618866704700199 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  20. Herzog, T.; Black, A.; Fountaine, K.; Knotts, D. Reflection and attentional recovery as distinctive benefits of restorative environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1997, 17, 165–170. Available online: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272494497900517 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  21. Berman, M.; Jonides, J.; Kaplan, S. The Cognitive Benefits of Interacting With Nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hartig, T.; Evans, G.; Jamner, L.; Davis, D.; Gärling, T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 109–123. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272494402001093 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  23. Berto, R. Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 249–259. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272494405000381 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  24. Presti, P.; Ruzzon, D.; Avanzini, P.; Caruana, F.; Rizzolatti, G.; Vecchiato, G. Measuring arousal and valence generated by the dynamic experience of architectural forms in virtual environments. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 13376. Available online: http://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-022-17689-9 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Martínez-Soto, J.; Gonzales-Santos, L.; Pasaye, E.; Barrios, F. Exploration of neural correlates of restorative environment exposure through functional magnetic resonance. Intell. Build. Int. 2013, 5, 10–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Henderson, J.; Larson, C.; Zhu, D. Cortical activation to indoor versus outdoor scenes: An fMRI study. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 179, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kim, T.; Jeong, G.; Baek, H.; Kim, G.; Sundaram, T.; Kang, H.; Lee, S.; Kim, H.; Song, J. Human brain activation in response to visual stimulation with rural and urban scenery pictures: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Sci. Total Environ. 2010, 408, 2600–2607. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0048969710001695 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  28. Jiang, X.; Hu, Y.; Larsen, L.; Chang, C.; Sullivan, W. Impacts of urban green infrastructure on attentional functioning: Insights from an fMRI study. Front Psychol 2023, 14, 1047993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Burattini, C.; Bisegna, F.; D’Aurizio, G.; Curcio, G. The effect of an audio-video stimulation on emotions: A virtual reality study. In Proceedings of the 2023 Immersive and 3D Audio: From Architecture to Automotive (I3DA), Bologna, Italy, 5–7 September 2023; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Burattini, C.; D’Aurizio, G.; Broszio, K. Environmental psychology and health: Research in VR and real settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2024, 100, 102478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Yeom, S.; Kim, H.; Hong, T. Psychological and physiological effects of a green wall on occupants: A cross-over study in virtual reality. Build. Environ. 2021, 204, 108134. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0360132321005357 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  32. Kim, N.; Gero, J. Neurophysiological Responses to Biophilic Design: A Pilot Experiment Using VR and EEG. In Design Computing And Cognition’22; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2023; pp. 235–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Yin, J.; Arfaei, N.; MacNaughton, P.; Catalano, P.; Allen, J.; Spengler, J. Effects of biophilic interventions in office on stress reaction and cognitive function: A randomized crossover study in virtual reality. Indoor Air 2019, 29, 1028–1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Jung, D.; Kim, D.; Kim, N. Bringing nature into hospital architecture: Machine learning-based EEG analysis of the biophilia effect in virtual reality. J. Environ. Psychol. 2023, 89, 102033. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0272494423000816 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  35. Higuera-Trujillo, J.; Millán, C.; Aviñó, A.; Rojas, J. Multisensory stress reduction: A neuro-architecture study of paediatric waiting rooms. Build. Res. Inf. 2020, 48, 269–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Song, R.; Chen, Q.; Zhang, Y.; Jia, Q.; He, H.; Gao, T.; Qiu, L. Psychophysiological restorative potential in cancer patients by virtual reality (VR)-based perception of natural environment. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 1003497. Available online: http://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1003497/full (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lei, Q.; Yuan, C.; Lau, S. A quantitative study for indoor workplace biophilic design to improve health and productivity performance. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 324, 129168. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0959652621033540 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  38. Yeom, S.; Kim, H.; Hong, T.; Park, H.; Lee, D. An integrated psychological score for occupants based on their perception and emotional response according to the windows’ outdoor view size. Build. Environ. 2020, 180, 107019. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0360132320303991 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  39. Jeong, J.; Park, S. Physiological and Psychological Effects of Visual Stimulation with Green Plant Types. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165027003003479 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  41. Pion-Tonachini, L.; Kreutz-Delgado, K.; Makeig, S. ICLabel: An automated electroencephalographic independent component classifier, dataset, and website. NeuroImage 2019, 198, 181–197. Available online: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811919304185 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  42. Buzsáki, G. Rhythms of the Brain; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006; Volume 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Klimesch, W.; Sauseng, P.; Hanslmayr, S. EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 2007, 53, 63–88. Available online: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S016501730600083X (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  44. Klimesch, W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 606–617. Available online: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661312002434 (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  45. Michelmann, S.; Griffiths, B.; Hanslmayr, S. The role of alpha and beta oscillations in the human EEG during perception and memory processes. In The Oxford Handbook of EEG Frequency; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2022; pp. 202–219. [Google Scholar]
  46. Jensen, O.; Mazaheri, A. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 186. Available online: http://www.frontiersin.org/journals/human-neuroscience/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186 (accessed on 3 October 2025).
  47. Magosso, E.; Ricci, G.; Ursino, M. Alpha and theta mechanisms operating in internal-external attention competition. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2021, 20, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ricci, G.; De Crescenzio, F.; Santhosh, S.; Magosso, E.; Ursino, M. Relationship between electroencephalographic data and comfort perception captured in a Virtual Reality design environment of an aircraft cabin. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 10938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chen, Z.; He, Y.; Yu, Y. Attention restoration during environmental exposure via alpha-theta oscillations and synchronization. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 68, 101406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Grassini, S.; Segurini, G.; Koivisto, M. Watching Nature Videos Promotes Physiological Restoration: Evidence From the Modulation of Alpha Waves in Electroencephalography. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 871143. Available online: http://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.871143/full (accessed on 3 October 2025). [CrossRef]
  51. Wang, F.; Ke, H.; Ma, H.; Tang, Y. Deep Wavelet Temporal-Frequency Attention for nonlinear fMRI factorization in ASD. Pattern Recognit. 2025, 165, 111543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Morales, S.; Bowers, M.E. Time-frequency analysis methods and their application in developmental EEG data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2022, 54, 101067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Murugappan, M.; Rizon, M.; Nagarajan, R.; Yaacob, S.; Hazry, D.; Zunaidi, I. Time-Frequency Analysis of EEG Signals for Human Emotion Detection. In 4th Kuala Lumpur International Conference on Biomedical Engineering 2008; Abu Osman, N.A., Ibrahim, F., Wan Abas, W.A.B., Abdul Rahman, H.S., Ting, H.-N., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; pp. 262–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Ignacio, P.; Shealy, T. Effects of biophilic restorative experiences on designers’ bodies, brains, and minds. Proc. Des. Soc. 2023, 3, 1565–1574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental procedure. (a) Participants experienced three virtual environments presented sequentially, each varying in the presence of biophilic elements. The experimental protocol consisted of alternating 10-s exposures to the different environments, separated by 5-s black screen intervals, for a total duration of 445 s (7.4 min). Participants remained stationary and engaged in passive viewing, with no tasks assigned during the session. (b) Example pre-processed EEG signals are shown during a 10-s segment. Signals were transformed to the frequency domain by extracting band power. Later on, a comparison across environments using a signed rank test revealed significant differences in specific electrodes.
Figure 1. Experimental procedure. (a) Participants experienced three virtual environments presented sequentially, each varying in the presence of biophilic elements. The experimental protocol consisted of alternating 10-s exposures to the different environments, separated by 5-s black screen intervals, for a total duration of 445 s (7.4 min). Participants remained stationary and engaged in passive viewing, with no tasks assigned during the session. (b) Example pre-processed EEG signals are shown during a 10-s segment. Signals were transformed to the frequency domain by extracting band power. Later on, a comparison across environments using a signed rank test revealed significant differences in specific electrodes.
Brainsci 15 01103 g001
Figure 2. Distinct band power patterns emerge in lower- and higher-frequency bands. Low- frequency bands exhibit higher power in the central frontal than the occipital regions, whereas high-frequency bands show increased power in lateral frontal, temporal, and parietal areas. Although this pattern was consistent across environments, statistical analysis (Figure 4) revealed significant differences.
Figure 2. Distinct band power patterns emerge in lower- and higher-frequency bands. Low- frequency bands exhibit higher power in the central frontal than the occipital regions, whereas high-frequency bands show increased power in lateral frontal, temporal, and parietal areas. Although this pattern was consistent across environments, statistical analysis (Figure 4) revealed significant differences.
Brainsci 15 01103 g002
Figure 3. Comparison of band power differences between environments. The power in different frequency bands was compared by subtracting the values between environments. Similar differences were observed when comparing Env1 (non-biophilic) with Env2 and Env3 (biophilic).
Figure 3. Comparison of band power differences between environments. The power in different frequency bands was compared by subtracting the values between environments. Similar differences were observed when comparing Env1 (non-biophilic) with Env2 and Env3 (biophilic).
Brainsci 15 01103 g003
Figure 4. Alpha and beta spectral power in visual and parietal areas reflects responses to environments with biophilic elements. (a) Maps show electrode locations where a significant difference was found between environmental transitions ( p < 0.05 ). Columns represent frequency bands, while rows correspond to different environment comparisons based on Wilcoxon signed rank tests across all subjects. Two clusters of alpha and beta power responses emerge in the posterior visual areas. Highlighted electrode positions indicate significantly higher spectral power between specific environment pairs (color-coded). (b) The right occipital lobe maintained significance in the alpha band after Bonferroni correction.
Figure 4. Alpha and beta spectral power in visual and parietal areas reflects responses to environments with biophilic elements. (a) Maps show electrode locations where a significant difference was found between environmental transitions ( p < 0.05 ). Columns represent frequency bands, while rows correspond to different environment comparisons based on Wilcoxon signed rank tests across all subjects. Two clusters of alpha and beta power responses emerge in the posterior visual areas. Highlighted electrode positions indicate significantly higher spectral power between specific environment pairs (color-coded). (b) The right occipital lobe maintained significance in the alpha band after Bonferroni correction.
Brainsci 15 01103 g004
Figure 5. Introduction of biophilic elements selectively suppresses alpha and induces theta spectral activities in the occipital cortex. (a) Summary of the mean power-in-band averaged across electrodes in the same area channels reveals characteristic patterns for the posterior, central, temporal, and frontal lobes in response to viewing the studied environments. (b) The greatest response was observed selectively in the visual areas of the occipital cortex with a significant suppression of the alpha and an induction of the theta EEG activities. (* indicates p < 0.05 .)
Figure 5. Introduction of biophilic elements selectively suppresses alpha and induces theta spectral activities in the occipital cortex. (a) Summary of the mean power-in-band averaged across electrodes in the same area channels reveals characteristic patterns for the posterior, central, temporal, and frontal lobes in response to viewing the studied environments. (b) The greatest response was observed selectively in the visual areas of the occipital cortex with a significant suppression of the alpha and an induction of the theta EEG activities. (* indicates p < 0.05 .)
Brainsci 15 01103 g005
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

García-Salinas, J.S.; Wróblewska, A.; Kucewicz, M.T. Detection of EEG Activity in Response to the Surrounding Environment: A Neuro-Architecture Study. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101103

AMA Style

García-Salinas JS, Wróblewska A, Kucewicz MT. Detection of EEG Activity in Response to the Surrounding Environment: A Neuro-Architecture Study. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(10):1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101103

Chicago/Turabian Style

García-Salinas, Jesús S., Anna Wróblewska, and Michal T. Kucewicz. 2025. "Detection of EEG Activity in Response to the Surrounding Environment: A Neuro-Architecture Study" Brain Sciences 15, no. 10: 1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101103

APA Style

García-Salinas, J. S., Wróblewska, A., & Kucewicz, M. T. (2025). Detection of EEG Activity in Response to the Surrounding Environment: A Neuro-Architecture Study. Brain Sciences, 15(10), 1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101103

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop