Next Article in Journal
The Expression of Affective Temperaments in Cystic Fibrosis Patients: Psychopathological Associations and Possible Neurobiological Mechanisms
Next Article in Special Issue
Maternal Variability of Amplitudes of Frequency Fluctuations Is Related to the Progressive Self–Other Transposition Group Intervention in Autistic Children
Previous Article in Journal
Role of Immersive Virtual Reality in Motor Behaviour Decision-Making in Chronic Pain Patients
Previous Article in Special Issue
Perceiving Oneself to Be Integrated into the Peer Group: A Protective Factor against Victimization in Children with Learning Disabilities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Avoidance Is Associated with Decreased Brain Responsiveness to Threat Distractors under High Perceptual Load

1
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Center, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
2
Department of Neuroradiology, University of Leipzig Medical Center, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
3
Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, 55122 Mainz, Germany
4
Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Haifa 3100000, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(4), 618; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040618
Submission received: 22 February 2023 / Revised: 31 March 2023 / Accepted: 3 April 2023 / Published: 5 April 2023

Abstract

:
Cognitive coping strategies to deal with anxiety-provoking events have an impact on mental and physical health. Dispositional vigilance is characterized by an increased analysis of the threatening environment, whereas cognitive avoidance comprises strategies to inhibit threat processing. To date, functional neuroimaging studies on the neural underpinnings of these coping styles are scarce and have revealed discrepant findings. In the present study, we examined automatic brain responsiveness as a function of coping styles using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We administered a perceptual load paradigm with contemptuous and fearful faces as distractor stimuli in a sample of N = 43 healthy participants. The Mainz Coping Inventory was used to assess cognitive avoidance and vigilance. An association of cognitive avoidance with reduced contempt and fear processing under high perceptual load was observed in a widespread network including the amygdala, thalamus, cingulate gyrus, insula, and frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital areas. Our findings indicate that the dispositional tendency to divert one’s attention away from distressing stimuli is a valuable predictor of diminished automatic neural responses to threat in several cortical and subcortical areas. A reduced processing in brain regions involved in emotion perception and attention might indicate a potential threat resilience associated with cognitive avoidance.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Anxiety is an emotional state that is characterized by high physiological arousal, vigilance, and the subjective experience of distress and apprehension, prompted by distant and unpredictable threats [1,2]. In general, anxiety is considered as an adaptive emotion that serves the maintenance of safety and well-being [3]. However, when feelings of anxiety become persistent and excessive, individuals may have a higher risk for cardiovascular diseases [4] or may even be diagnosed with an anxiety disorder when symptoms lead to functional impairments in daily life [5]. Coping strategies in response to stressful events have been implicated in the pathogenesis of mental and physical disorders (e.g., [6,7,8]). Therefore, coping skills are potential targets for prevention or intervention programs [9]. There are inter-individual differences in the dispositional use of coping strategies to deal with anxiety-inducing situations. Krohne [10] has suggested in his model of coping modes (MCM) for threatening situations two dimensions of dispositional coping behavior: vigilance and cognitive avoidance. Vigilance is characterized by an intense search for information related to the threatening event. In contrast, cognitive avoidance denotes the disposition to inhibit the processing of threatening information by directing attention away from threat cues that can be of external or internal nature. Factor analyses have confirmed the assumption that vigilant and cognitive avoidant strategies are moderately (negatively) related, but constitute independent dimensions of dispositional coping with anxiety [11,12]. In the MCM, Krohne [10] has claimed that anxiety-inducing incidents are characterized by the presence of aversive stimulation and ambiguity, which can trigger emotional arousal and experiences of uncertainty regarding the occurrence of threats. Vigilance is a coping strategy that is supposed to reduce uncertainty and the probability of bad surprises. Cognitive avoidance aims at the reduction of intense anxious arousal by shielding the individual from aversive stimulation. In the MCM, it is suggested that individuals with a low tolerance for experienced uncertainty are inclined to make use of a vigilant coping style. Thus, vigilant individuals monitor their environment intensely to be prepared for the occurrence of potentially threatening information. On the other hand, individuals that are highly susceptible for anxious arousal are supposed to avoid danger cues. Individuals scoring high in vigilance but low in cognitive avoidance have been designated as sensitizers. Individuals who consistently employ cognitive avoidance but not vigilance have been designated as repressors [11]. There is evidence for an overlap between the coping style classifications according to Krohne [10] and according to Weinberger et al. [13]; see Egloff and Hock [14] and Krohne et al. [11]. The simultaneous utilization of scales to assess social desirability and trait anxiety has been proposed by Weinberger et al. [13] to classify repression and sensitization based on median splits or cut-off scores. Within the framework of Weinberger et al.’s [13] theory of coping strategies, it is assumed that repressors report low scores in anxiety, as they avoid the awareness of their own anxiety, while they display high repressive defensiveness. In response to stress, repressors are characterized by discrepancies among low self-reported distress but high levels of autonomic arousal. Despite some differences in the theoretical foundations, Krohne’s [10] and Weinberger et al.’s [13] methods appear to assess related constructs, in particular with respect to repression and sensitization. Thus, findings based on both assessment procedures might be comparable [14], but see Kohlmann [15] for contradictory findings. The classificatory system of Weinberger et al. [13] is category-based, which has been criticized (e.g., [16]), whereas Krohne et al. [11] have provided with the Mainz Coping Inventory (MCI), a dimensional measure to assess cognitive avoidance and vigilance.
Neuroimaging research on vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping styles in the framework of Krohne [10] is scarce and revealed heterogeneous findings. Most studies have investigated brain activation as a function of coping styles during threat processing by comparing individuals categorized as repressors with individuals categorized as sensitizers. This categorical approach provided interesting insights regarding neural differences between repressors and sensitizers. During the passive viewing of visible or briefly displayed emotional faces, repressors have exhibited heightened responsivity to fearful faces in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and in the temporo-occipital visual system, relative to sensitizers [17]. On the other hand, sensitizers have demonstrated exaggerated responsivity to angry faces in the frontal cortex and increased amygdalar reactivity in response to visible fearful faces. During an explicit emotion processing task, where very briefly presented facial emotions had to be labeled, repressors have shown stronger activity than sensitizers in frontal and temporo-parietal areas in response to angry and fearful faces [18]. Similar findings have been reported for the explicit threat-evaluation of visible emotional faces [19]. Here, repressors exhibited heightened activity in response to angry but not fearful faces in the temporo-parietal and frontal cortex, compared to sensitizers. Stronger activations in repressors may indicate an intensified processing of specific threatening stimuli, but may also represent increased attentional control or emotion regulation efforts to enable task performance (see also [17,18,19]). There is also evidence for stronger activations in repressors compared to sensitizers in the amygdala and insula during fear conditioning with electrical stimulation [20]. Higher activations in these emotion-related brain areas were accompanied by stronger responses in the ventral ACC and ventromedial PFC, brain regions that have been implicated in emotion evaluation, emotion-related learning, and emotion regulation [21,22]. In sum, studies using categorical approaches partly point to heightened reactivity in prefrontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital areas in repressors relative to sensitizers. Higher responsivity to threat appears to depend strongly on the emotional quality of the stimulus, as angry faces produce more homogeneous findings than fearful faces.
Vigilance appears to be positively related to trait anxiety, whereas cognitive avoidance shows a negative correlation with anxiety [11,15,23,24]. However, none of the reported imaging studies on coping styles have considered a potential influence of anxiety.
Given the background information that vigilant and cognitive avoidant strategies are not conceptualized as opposite poles of a continuum, but as independent dimensions of dispositional coping [11,12], it is surprising that only one study used a dimensional approach to predict brain responsiveness to emotional faces by both coping styles, each independently. Leehr et al. [25] have used in a large sample an automatic emotion processing task, where individuals had to match the identity of threatening or neutral faces. Thus, the emotional quality of the faces was not relevant for task performance. Here, no relation among cognitive avoidance and brain responsiveness to fearful and angry faces was observed. A cerebral hyper-responsiveness in high cognitive avoidance could not be confirmed. High vigilance was associated with diminished activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus in response to angry, but not fearful, faces. A recent study on brain structural correlates of vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping styles linked volumetric alterations in the thalamus to both strategies to manage anxiety-provoking situations [24]. The thalamus, along with the amygdala, has been implicated in an automatic alerting system when faced with potential threat-related cues [26], in which the thalamus sends coarse information of potential danger signals to the amygdala (e.g., [27,28]). Günther et al. [24] found high vigilance to be related to volumetric increases in the thalamus, whereas reduced thalamic gray matter volume was found in cognitive avoidance. The heterogeneity of previous findings might partly be attributed to different methodologies (e.g., extreme group comparisons or dimensional approaches) and variations in applied experimental tasks in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (e.g., controlled or automatic emotion processing with varying stimulus durations). The inconsistent results point out the importance of further research.
Faces and their emotional content appear to be processed fast and efficiently by the human visual system [29,30]. In general, threat-related stimuli were shown to have an impact on improved behavioral control capabilities [31] and to activate a distributed brain network including the amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal and temporal gyrus; see [32] for an overview. There is evidence that emotional stimuli capture attention involuntarily [33,34] and that their valences are evaluated automatically [35]. However, the automatic processing of emotionally salient stimuli appears to depend on the availability of sufficient attentional resources (e.g., [36,37]). According to the Load Theory ([38,39], see [40] for an overview), the processing of task-irrelevant distractors is prevented when high task demands or a large amount of task-relevant information leave no spare perceptual capacity. In this case, attention to threat distractors is inhibited due to a lack of processing resources. Interestingly, brain responsiveness to emotional face distractors under different perceptual-load conditions has been shown to be modulated by non-clinical [37] and clinical anxiety [41].
To our knowledge, no previous study investigated the effects of attentional load on the neural processing of threat distractors as a function of dispositional coping styles. In the present study, functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans were selected from healthy individuals with varying degrees of vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping strategies while they performed a perceptual load task with contemptuous, fearful, and neutral faces. Fearful faces are considered as biologically salient stimuli that signal potential threats in the environment [42], whereas contempt has been characterized as a hostile emotion [43], which can imply disapproval and rejection [44]. Previous neuroimaging studies, which used a dimensional approach [24,25], pointed to associations between coping styles and brain functional and structural alterations in the ACC and the thalamus. Thus, the ACC and thalamus were chosen as our regions of interest (ROIs). We expected increased thalamic and reduced anterior cingulate responsiveness to threat faces in vigilance, and decreased thalamic reactivity in cognitive avoidance. Due to heterogeneous results from earlier studies with differing experimental designs, we used an exploratory whole-brain approach to examine the relationship between vigilant/avoidant coping and brain responsiveness under low and high perceptual load.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Forty-five healthy volunteers participated in this study. All participants had to be native German speakers with a normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Participants were recruited via public notices in canteens and libraries and online advertisement in social networks. Exclusion criteria for study participation were a history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, MRI contraindications, head trauma involving loss of consciousness, and left-handedness. To exclude potential diagnoses of past or current Axis I disorders, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID-I; [45]) was administered.
Two participants had to be excluded, as they were extreme scorers in overall hit rates (>4 SD below the mean), which was mainly due to omission errors indicating an inattentive task performance. The final sample included N = 43 subjects (22 women), with a mean age of 23.81 years (SD = 3.59) and a mean school education of 12.09 years (SD = 0.48). When all tasks were completed, participants received financial compensation.

2.2. Psychometric Measures

Cognitive avoidance and vigilance were assessed by the Mainz Coping Inventory (MCI; 11.23). The MCI is as a stimulus-response inventory that consists of 80 items. Eight anxiety-evoking scenarios are described, including ego threat (e.g., giving a speech) and physical threat (e.g., an encounter with dubious people at night), and for each scenario, five vigilant strategies, such as “anticipation of negative events” and “information search” (e.g., “I think about what questions might be asked after the speech” or “I watch the dubious people closely”) and five cognitive avoidant strategies, such as “re-interpretation” and “attentional diversion” (e.g., “I prefer to talk with friends about something other than the speech”) are administered. For each scenario, participants are instructed to state on a true-false scale which of the mentioned strategies they would prefer. Across all situations and items, sum scores are calculated separately for the vigilance and cognitive avoidance scales.
Levels of trait anxiety were measured with the trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; [46]). Questionnaire characteristics of the final sample and correlations between the psychometric measures are presented in Table 1. Vigilance and cognitive avoidance showed a moderate negative relationship. Moreover, trait anxiety was positively correlated with vigilance and negatively correlated with cognitive avoidance.

2.3. fMRI Experiment: Perceptual Load Task

The automatic emotional face processing task, with a low and high perceptual load condition, was adapted from Bishop et al. [37]. Stimuli consisted of photographs of 24 actors (12 men) picturing either fear, contempt, or neutral facial expressions, chosen from the Radboud Faces database [47]. Six additional actors depicting neutral faces were used for an initial practice task. Subjects were told that they would perform a letter search task and were instructed to respond as accurately as possible. We were primarily interested in brain responsiveness to emotional expressions, not reaction times. Conscious awareness of an erroneous response appears to elicit activity in the ACC (e.g., [48]). To avoid interferences with error-related brain responsiveness, we aimed at a low number of overall error trials and did explicitly request accurate, but not necessarily fast, responses from the participants. Responses were given on a fiber optic response pad in each hand via button presses with the left and right index finger.
The duration of each trial was 3 s and it started with a fixation cross that was shown for 500 ms, followed by 1000 ms of face and letter presentation; see Figure 1. The emotional or neutral face stimulus was a task-irrelevant distractor that was superimposed by a string of six letters. The participants had to identify the target letters X and A. In the low perceptual load condition, the string was entirely composed of target letters (e.g., six As). In the high perceptual load condition, a single target letter was randomly mixed with non-target letters (T, H, U, M, W), increasing attentional search requirements. A gray screen where the assignment of the letter responses to the buttons was depicted followed for 1.5 s.
Depicted is the sequence of events within trials of the perceptual load task in the low load condition (top illustration) and high load condition (lower illustration). Distractor stimuli were contemptuous, fearful, or neutral faces. Targets were the letters X or A.
The task comprised 24 blocks (4 per condition) of 6 trials. Trials within a block were randomized with respect to the target letter, but were constant regarding the load condition (low vs. high) and facial emotional expression (fearful, contempt, neutral). We balanced the presentation frequencies of actors across the experiment. Each block lasted for 18 s and was followed by a 12 s blank screen. Two fixed counterbalanced sequences were chosen for the trials to avoid stimulus order effects. Presentation® software was used for stimulus presentation and to record responses.

2.4. MRI Acquisition and Preprocessing

Structural and functional MR images were selected using a 3 T scanner (Magnetom Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 20-channel coil. Structural images were obtained with a T1-weighted 3D MP-RAGE [49] with the following imaging parameters: TI 900 ms, TR 1900 ms, TE 2.65 ms, flip angle 9°, spatial resolution of 0.8 × 0.8 × 1 mm3, and two averages. Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast sensitive images were collected using T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (matrix 642; resolution 3.5 × 3.5 × 3.5 mm3; TR 2.54 s; TE 30 ms; flip angle 90°; interleaved acquisition of 40 slices along the AC-PC plane; 237 images). To preprocess and analyze MRI data SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/, accessed on 14 November 2013) was used. The first four functional volumes were discarded to allow longitudinal magnetization to reach equilibrium. Preprocessing included motion-correction, slice time-correction, and co-registration. Anatomical images were segmented, including normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. The normalization parameters were then applied to the functional EPI series (resulting in a re-sampled voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm3). A temporal high-pass filter (128 s) was applied. Functional data were smoothed (Gaussian kernel size = 6 mm).

2.5. Data Analyses

Mean accuracy was high with 97% (SD = 0.03%), and overall hit rates were not significantly correlated with the vigilance scale (MCI-VIG) or the cognitive avoidance scale (MCI-CAV), (r(41) = 0.25, p = 0.11 and r(41) = −0.07, p = 0.67, respectively). Coping styles were not related to effects of emotion on accuracy (hit rate neutral minus hit rate emotion) in the different perceptual load conditions (see Supplementary Table S1). A 2 (perceptual load condition: low vs. high) × 3 (emotion condition: fear, contempt, neutral) analysis of variance with repeated measures and hit rates as dependent variables revealed no significant main effects for load and emotion, or interactions (all ps > 0.21). Thus, hit rates were not influenced by the cognitive load condition or emotion condition, probably due to ceiling effects in task performance.
Functional MRI data were analyzed by modeling the onset and duration of 18 s for each block. Regressors were convolved with a hemodynamic response function for the six conditions (contempt-low load, contempt-high load, fear-low load, fear-high load, neutral-low load, neutral-high load). First-level t-contrasts were calculated for the low load condition by contrasting the contempt and fear condition with the neutral one, separately. The same was done for the high load condition, where contempt and fear were each contrasted with the neutral condition. These contrasts were chosen to determine whether activations were clearly triggered by the emotional content of facial expressions, under low and high load separately. First-level t-contrasts (and following second-level main effect analyses) were also calculated for the load factor, where the low load condition was contrasted to the high load condition of the same emotion (e.g., contempt under low load vs. contempt under high load). These contrasts allow conclusions about activations elicited by task demands under a certain emotional condition and are reported in Table S2 in the Supplementary Materials.
For the second-level analyses, one-sample t-tests were performed to determine main effects of contemptuous expressions (vs. neutral ones) under low load and under high load, and of fearful expressions (vs. neutral ones) under low load and under high load. Contrast images were entered into regression models with the respective individual MCI-VIG and MCI-CAV scores as regressors of interest. One regression model was calculated per coping style and per contrast. Due to the evidence that trait anxiety is associated with vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping strategies, STAI scores and sex were also entered in these models to regress out a possible influence.
Exploratory whole-brain analyses were conducted with a voxel-wise threshold at p < 0.001 (uncorrected) and an additional cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05, family-wise-error (FWE) corrected. ROI analyses were carried out for the bilateral thalamus and ACC. To create an anatomically defined mask, the WFU Pickatlas [50] was used (according to [51]). For ROI analyses, the statistical threshold was set to p = 0.05, FWE-corrected.

3. Results

3.1. Neural Main Effects

3.1.1. Main Effects of Threatening Faces under Low Perceptual Load

Across all participants, contemptuous faces (compared to neutral faces) that were processed under low perceptual load conditions significantly evoked activation in the left superior and bilateral inferior parietal lobule (BA40), the right middle temporal and fusiform gyrus, the right inferior frontal gyrus (BA9), and the thalamus see Table 2. Fearful faces produced no significant brain activations relative to neutral faces under low perceptual load.

3.1.2. Main Effects of Threatening Faces under High Perceptual Load

Contemptuous (vs. neutral) faces produced no significant brain activations under high load conditions. Fearful faces, compared to neutral faces, significantly evoked activations in the left inferior frontal gyrus, extending to the precentral gyrus, in the bilateral superior temporal gyrus, and the cerebellum; see Table 2.

3.2. Relationship among Brain Activation to Threat Faces and Coping Styles

3.2.1. Vigilance

ACC and thalamus ROI-based analyses and whole-brain analyses showed no significant associations between a vigilant coping style and activations in response to contemptuous (vs. neutral) or fearful (vs. neutral) faces under low or high load conditions.

3.2.2. Cognitive Avoidance

Exploratory whole-brain regression analyses and thalamus ROI-analyses with the cognitive avoidance scale did not reveal associations with brain reactivity to contemptuous (vs. neutral) or fearful (vs. neutral) faces under the low load condition.
In thalamus ROI-based regression analyses under high load conditions, cognitive avoidance was negatively correlated with responsiveness in the thalamus to contemptuous (vs. neutral) and fearful (vs. neutral) faces: peak voxel xyz: −6 −10 16, cluster size: 2, T-score = 3.79, pFWE = 0.03, and peak voxel xyz: −3, −13, 3, cluster size: 3, T-score = 4.25, pFWE = 0.01, each respectively.
In exploratory whole-brain regression analyses for contemptuous faces in the high load condition (see Table 3 and Figure 2), cognitive avoidance showed a negative correlation with activity in large clusters including the left postcentral gyrus, extending to the bilateral precentral (BA6), middle and inferior frontal gyrus (BA8); the bilateral insula, and bilateral superior temporal gyrus; the bilateral lingual gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, and calcarine sulcus (primary visual cortex); the precuneus, supramarginal and inferior parietal gyrus, and amygdala and thalamus. More clusters were revealed in the right anterior and middle cingulate gyrus (BA32), extending to the superior medial frontal cortex (BA8 and 9), in the bilateral supplementary motor area (BA6), extending to the medial frontal and middle cingulate gyrus, and also in the right postcentral gyrus, extending to the inferior parietal gyrus.
During the processing of fearful (vs. neutral) faces under high load, cognitive avoidance was significantly and negatively associated with brain activity in a large cluster including the right insula and subcentral gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, and precentral gyrus, and bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus, thalamus, and bilateral lingual gyrus, cuneus, and precuneus. Additional clusters showing a negative correlation were also revealed in the postcentral gyrus, in the fusiform gyrus, and the bilateral anterior cingulate gyrus.
Following a reviewer’s suggestion, we calculated further regression analyses with the respective vigilance or cognitive avoidance scale as additional covariate to control for. The result pattern did not substantially change, but the overall number of activated voxels was slightly reduced. When additionally controlling for a potential influence of vigilant coping style, cognitive avoidance was no longer related to diminished activity in the anterior cingulate gyrus in response to fearful faces.

4. Discussion

With the present study, we aimed to examine the relationship between coping strategies in anxiety-provoking situations and automatic brain responsiveness to threatening facial expressions in case of low and high task demands. For the first time, a perceptual load task with contemptuous and fearful faces was administered in a non-clinical sample of young adults with varying degrees of vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping styles. The processing of threatening faces can be assumed to be automatic in our used paradigm, since the facial expressions were task-irrelevant distractor stimuli [52]. We aimed to shed light on coping styles as predictors of automatic brain reactivity to threat stimuli under low and high attentional load, after controlling for a potential influence of trait anxiety. In the low perceptual load condition, contemptuous faces, but not fearful faces, elicited activations in the parietal, temporal, and inferior frontal cortex. These brain regions have been previously shown to be activated in response to negative pictures during low perceptual load [53]. During low perceptual load, coping styles did not influence brain activity in response to contemptuous or fearful faces. Under high perceptual load, fearful faces, but not contemptuous faces, elicited increased activity in frontal and temporal areas, and in the cerebellum. Interestingly, temporal and frontal activation was also modulated by coping style. Individuals with a disposition to cognitively avoid anxiety-inducing information showed decreased brain reactivity to contempt and fear in several areas including the superior temporal gyrus, the anterior and posterior insula, the thalamus, the amygdala, the parieto-occipital lobe (supramarginal and inferior parietal gyrus, precuneus, cuneus), the anterior, middle and posterior cingulate gyrus, and pre- and postcentral gyrus. Additionally, reduced activations in cognitive avoiders have been observed during the processing of contemptuous faces in the superior and middle frontal and medial frontal gyrus. Despite a moderate negative correlation between vigilance and cognitive avoidance, only the avoidant coping style contributed to the prediction of automatic brain responsiveness to threat under high attentional load. Contrary to findings from Leehr et al. [25], our hypothesis of reduced anterior cingulate responsiveness in vigilance was not confirmed. Only individuals who tend to inhibit the processing of distressing external or internal stimuli and divert their attention away from them showed reduced activity in a widespread neural network implicated in emotion and attention. This reduced responsiveness in cognitive avoidance was only observed during high attentional load and was not influenced by trait anxiety.
As a subcortical brain structure, the thalamus is involved in the early visual encoding of salient stimuli and plays an important role in the selection of information from the environment that can be relevant for further processing [54,55]. The thalamus has connections with the prefrontal and temporo-parietal cortices and with the amygdala [56,57]. The amygdala plays a key role in the fast detection of potential threats, and is involved in the recruitment of attentional resources and guidance of attention in the presence of salient stimuli [54,58,59]. A subcortical route has been suggested through which the amygdala receives direct sensory input from the thalamus [27,54,57]. In contrast, Pessoa and Adolphs [60] have proposed that the thalamus receives multimodal information from cortical regions and conveys biologically significant information to cortical regions, which activate the amygdala.
A previous study demonstrated a reduced thalamic gray matter density in cognitive avoidance [24]. Reduced thalamic and amygdalar responsiveness in individuals scoring high in cognitive avoidance may indicate a lower sensitivity to distracting threat stimuli and a lower capacity for the maintenance of attention to these stimuli. Of note, in the present study, findings in the thalamus were restricted to a relatively small cluster and should be considered as preliminary.
High cognitive avoidance was associated with reduced reactivity in visual processing areas (i.e., cuneus, lingual gyrus, see [61]) that have also been implicated in emotional face processing [62,63]. Diminished reactivity to threat distractors in cognitive avoiders was also observed in the inferior parietal cortex and supramarginal gyrus. These parietal brain areas appear to be involved in emotion regulation, such as reappraisal and suppression [22,64,65], probably by directing attention to relevant stimuli (e.g., [64]). Indeed, the ventral regions of the parietal cortex along with the precuneus (e.g., [66]) have been suggested to play a role in visuo–spatial attention [67], particularly in involuntary and bottom-up driven shifts of attention [68]. This proposed function is in line with increased activity in these brain areas during the non-conscious or automatic perception of fearful faces [69]. Additionally, the supramarginal gyrus appears to be recruited during the judgment of facial emotions [70]. Further, the superior temporal gyrus is supposed to be involved in the visual analyses and encoding of emotional facial expressions (e.g., [30,62,71]), and activity within this area has been linked to fear and anxiety [32].
It is conceivable that the visual processing and encoding of threat-related distractors and their capability to capture attention under high load is diminished in cognitive avoidance. Blunted responsiveness in the pre- and postcentral gyrus (i.e., the premotor and primary somatosensory cortex) may also indicate a reduced perception and recognition of threat faces in avoiders, since both brain regions have been implicated in the affective evaluation of facial expressions [30,71]. We have also observed in cognitive avoidance a reduced reactivity in the posterior cingulate gyrus. The posterior cingulate gyrus has been discussed as part of the default mode network [72] and appears to be involved in the evaluation of the affective valence of stimuli [73], in self-related processing, and in social evaluation (e.g., see [74] for an overview). Particularly, deactivations in the posterior cingulate gyrus have been described as a neural correlate of task-related attention [72]. Thus, decreased responsivity may indicate a more external focus on task demands and more efficient cognitive functioning in individuals with a disposition to cognitively avoid anxiety- inducing stimuli. However, no evidence emerged in our study that coping styles were associated with improved or impoverished task performance in case of low or high perceptual load. Thus, a more efficient processing style and a reduced encoding of threat distractors on the neural level in avoidant coping style did not exert an influence on the behavioral performance.
Individuals who scored high in cognitive avoidance also responded less to contemptuous and fearful faces in the posterior insula. The posterior insula has been ascribed to various functions, such as sensory-motor processing and the collection of interoceptive, emotional, and environmental data [75]. Menon and Uddin [76] have proposed a basic function of the posterior insula in the regulation of physiological reactivity, but it has also been suggested that the posterior insula engages in primary interoceptive processes [77,78] and in the representation of aversive bodily and emotional stages [79]. Activity within the posterior insula has also been associated with the judgment of emotions in faces [80] and attention to the emotional relevance of stimuli [81]. In addition, contemptuous faces elicited less activation in cognitive avoiders in the anterior insula, which has been implicated in attention, salience evaluation, emotional awareness, and in the integration of interoceptive information [75,77]. Reduced insular responsiveness in high cognitive avoidance may represent a less internally orientated processing of emotional or bodily states, which were induced by threat faces, or reduced attention to their relevance. This interpretation falls in line with reduced activity in the posterior cingulate and supramarginal gyrus, which have also been implicated in attention to one’s own emotional states [82].
Another brain region where individuals with a cognitive avoidant coping style manifested less activity in response to contemptuous faces included the dorsal anterior and middle cingulate gyrus, and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (PFC). The dorsal ACC and dorsomedial PFC have been suggested to support attention processes and performance monitoring and to play a role in voluntary emotion regulation [83] and conflict monitoring [84]. There is also convincing evidence that the dorsal ACC and dorsomedial PFC are engaged in fear appraisal and the generation of fear responses (e.g., [84,85]), in the evaluation of negative valences [86], and the anticipation of unpredictable threats [87]. Earlier findings point to a role of the dorsal ACC in subliminal face processing [88] and in cardiovascular responsivity to emotional face stimuli [89] and stressors [90]. Higher activity in the dorsal ACC and dorsomedial PFC has been linked to the processing of disapproving facial expressions in individuals with high rejection sensitivity [91] and to the experience of distress during social rejection [92]. Thus, individuals with a disposition to avoid threat-related information might demonstrate a lower sensitivity and autonomic responsiveness to facial expressions of contempt, which imply disapproval and rejection [44]. It is also conceivable that reduced activity in these brain areas indicates less effort to regulate emotion perception or to monitor emotional conflicts. Individuals with an avoidant regulation style might show an impaired ability to disengage attention and regulate the processing of task-irrelevant threat distractors under high load. However, in this case, one would expect heightened activations in emotion-related brain areas to accompany decreased emotion regulation efforts. An alternative interpretation of reduced ACC and PFC responsiveness to contempt might be a lower demand for emotional conflict monitoring or emotion regulation due to the diminished processing of threat distractors in subcortical and primary visual areas in cognitive avoidance.
Taken together, according to our results, individuals who are inclined to withdraw their attention from anxiety-inducing information and to inhibit their further processing manifest decreased activity in a brain network implicated in the processing of emotionally relevant information. Our findings suggest reduced visual processing and encoding of distracting and threatening social stimuli, and less attention allocation and weaker emotional responsiveness to them. However, this reduced sensory analysis of threat signals and diminished emotional responsiveness in cognitive avoidance was only observed during high attentional load. Leehr et al. [25] have found no relationship among cognitive avoidant coping style and automatic brain responsiveness to emotional faces in a task with relatively low cognitive demands. This is in line with our null results in the low perceptual load condition. These data suggest that coping styles only modulate the processing of threatening information when perceptual resources are limited. One may speculate that coping-related inter-individual differences in threat encoding occur at a very early pre-attentive level, with a reduced distractor encoding in low (relative to high) cognitive avoidance at this processing stage. However, under the condition of spare attentional capacity (i.e., low load), salient distractors may be encoded at later processing stages in all individuals, irrespective of their dispositional use of coping strategies. Our results appear contradictory to findings by Paul et al. [18] and Rauch et al. [19], which pointed to enhanced processing of threat faces in prefrontal and temporo-parietal areas in repressors, relative to sensitizers. Several methodological differences might have contributed to the discrepancies. Importantly, earlier tasks required the participants to attend to and evaluate emotional expressions, whereas in our task emotional expressions served as task-irrelevant distractor stimuli. Dimensional approaches to investigate neural correlates of coping styles might also produce different results than category-based approaches where high scorers in cognitive avoidance and vigilance are compared to each other. In general, findings on functional substrates of strategies to cope with anxiety-provoking situations remain inconsistent and highlight the importance of future research. Further investigations appear worthwhile, since coping styles can have important implications for physical and mental health.
Schwerdtfeger and Rathner [93] suggested that repression constitutes a rather maladaptive strategy to cope with aversive events. According to the authors, a strategy to deny problems that require a solution or to divert one’s attention away from them may circumvent adaptive problem-focused approaches. Grant et al. [94] have provided evidence for a relationship among cognitive avoidance in response to stressful life events and anxiety. However, Coifman et al. [95] and Bonanno et al. [96] have reported protective effects of repressive coping on mental health, when individuals are confronted with adverse life events. It has been stated that cognitive avoidance serves the purpose of decreasing anxious arousal and should be preferred by people who have a low tolerance for intense emotional states [97]. Indeed, some studies have suggested a higher sensitivity and autonomic responsivity in the face of threats in repression (e.g., [20,98,99]), but other studies could not confirm this assumption [100,101]. It appears that repression and cognitive avoidance in response to stressful events are broadly defined constructs in the literature, comprising strategies such as denial, resignation, avoiding thoughts about problems, and avoiding the awareness of one’s own anxiety, but also attentional diversion, re-interpretation, and accentuating positive aspects of the situations [13,97,102]. These constructs vary significantly in their operationalization. In several studies, different coping questionnaires [20,94,98] were used, or repression has been operationalized as a discrepancy score between self-reported perception of distress and actual physiological responses (e.g., [95,99]), or as a combination of low self-reported trait anxiety and high defensiveness [100,101]. Within Krohne’s [10] MCM, cognitive avoidance and vigilance strategies are elaborated in the context of anxiety-inducing events with varying degrees of controllability. Cognitive functions that are subsumed under cognitive avoidance are, for example, attentional diversion, re-interpretation, denial, minimization, and self-enhancement (see [97]). Importantly, cognitive avoidance according to Krohne [10] is conceptually distinct from (behavioral) avoidance of anxiety-inducing situations, which is a common symptom in clinical anxiety that perpetuates the disorder [5]. Vigilance comprises strategies such as information search, anticipation of negative events, planning, and situation control [97]. It is plausible that the success of each strategy and its long-term consequences on physical and mental health depend on the degree to which threat situations can be controlled. Cognitive avoidance has been suggested as an appropriate strategy when dealing with uncontrollable threats [97]. In contrast, individuals who employ a vigilant coping style are inclined to have an intolerance of uncertainty, which has been suggested as a transdiagnostic causal mechanism of anxiety-related conditions [103].
Whereas earlier findings pointed to enhanced processing of emotional faces in a frontal and temporo-parietal brain network in healthy individuals with a disposition to employ a cognitive avoidant coping style relative to sensitization (e.g., [18]), our results indicate a diminished capacity to perceive and attend to threat distractors in cognitive avoidance. This relationship was not influenced by trait anxiety. Thus, dispositional strategies to cope with anxiety-evoking situations and anxious arousal in a cognitive avoidant manner can explain incremental proportions of variance in automatic brain activity elicited by threat faces.
Our findings of reduced activation in cognitive avoidance might have relevance within the framework of non-clinical and clinical studies on anxiety. Increased activation in the dorsolateral PFC, posterior cingulate, parietal, and temporo-occipital areas have been implicated in an anxious temperament and in social anxiety disorder [104,105,106,107]. Furthermore, in contrast to healthy controls, patients with panic disorder, specific phobias, and social anxiety disorder appear to exhibit elevated activity within the anterior and midcingulate gyrus in response to feared stimuli [108,109,110]. This hyperactivity was shown to be resolved after psychotherapy [110]. Our finding of reduced automatic brain responsiveness to negative distractor stimuli might indicate a more favorable processing style in cognitive avoidance and might be a resilience factor. However, it remains to be clarified in future studies whether a cognitive avoidant strategy that relies on attention allocation away from anxiety-inducing situations and from bodily responses to them, as well as on denial, minimization, reappraisal, and the accentuation of positive aspects of a threat situation can be clearly assigned as an adaptive or maladaptive way of coping. It remains speculative whether cognitive avoidance increases the risk for physical and mental health issues or can be considered as a protective factor. Our results indicate a neural response pattern to emotional stimuli that appears incongruous to brain activations that have been observed in pathological anxiety.
Our study has several limitations. We have investigated young, healthy, and well- educated participants. Our results cannot be generalized to other populations. In our experiment, 24 trials were presented per condition. An increased trial number may enhance the statistical power. However, one has to consider potential neural habituation effects with an increased task length and repetition of stimuli [111]. With a sample size of N = 43 we were only able to detect moderate and large effects. To test their validity, our findings need to be replicated in larger samples and merged in meta-analyses. We operationalized vigilant and cognitive avoidant coping by means of the Mainz Coping Inventory [11,23]. Future studies on physiological or neural responsiveness and health outcomes that are moderated by coping styles might benefit from the simultaneous application of different measures and operationalization of cognitive avoidance. The cross-sectional design of our study does not allow speculations about the direction of the effects. Longitudinal studies may investigate whether altered neural reactivity is a mechanism in the manifestation of inter-individual differences in coping styles or might be the consequences of down-regulated perception and attention in individuals who prefer cognitive avoidant anxiety regulation styles in their daily lives.

5. Conclusions

In sum, we have demonstrated in individuals with a disposition to cognitively avoid anxiety-inducing information reduced reactivity to distracting threat faces in a widespread subcortical and cortical brain network that is involved in attention and emotion perception.
It might be concluded from our findings that individuals who employ a cognitive avoidant regulation style have a less sensitive threat detection system when attentional resources are limited. The diminished threat processing may indicate a potential resilience against transient danger signals in the environment and may be a neural basis of inter-individual differences in the vulnerability to mental disorders.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci13040618/s1, Table S1: Correlations between coping styles, trait anxiety, and emotional accuracy effects (hit rate neutral minus hit rate emotion) under low and high perceptual load, Table S2: Perceptual load main effects: Brain regions showing different activation under the high load (vs. low load) condition.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.G., A.K., K.-T.H., B.E., D.L. and T.S.; Methodology, V.G. and H.O.-S.; Software, V.G.; Validation, V.G.; Formal Analysis, V.G.; Investigation, M.S., J.P., C.W. and S.E.; Resources, A.K. and K.-T.H.; Data Curation, V.G.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, V.G.; Writing—Review and Editing, B.E., H.O.-S., D.L. and T.S.; Visualization, V.G.; Supervision, T.S.; Project Administration, V.G.; Funding Acquisition, V.G. and T.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation DFG to VG (GU 2231/2-1, project number: 496949003).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Medical Faculty of the University of Leipzig (reference number: 396/18-ek, date: 20 November 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge support from the University of Leipzig within the program of Open Access Publishing.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Davis, M.; Walker, D.L.; Miles, L.; Grillon, C. Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: Role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010, 35, 105–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Lang, P.J.; Davis, M.; Öhman, A. Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. J. Affect. Disord. 2000, 61, 137–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sylvers, P.; Lilienfeld, S.O.; LaPrairie, J.L. Differences between trait fear and trait anxiety: Implications for psychopathology. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 31, 122–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Suls, J. Toxic affect: Are anger, anxiety, and depression independent risk factors for cardiovascular disease? Emotion Rev. 2018, 10, 6–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
  6. Taylor, S.E.; Stanton, A.L. Coping resources, coping processes, and mental health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2007, 3, 377–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Mund, M.; Mitte, K. The costs of repression: A meta-analysis on the relation between repressive coping and somatic diseases. Health Psychol. 2012, 31, 640–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Myers, L.B. The importance of the repressive coping style: Findings from 30 years of research. Anxiety Stress Coping 2010, 23, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Collins, S.; Woolfson, L.M.; Durkin, K. Effects on coping skills and anxiety of a universal school-based mental health intervention delivered in Scottish primary schools. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2014, 35, 85–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Krohne, H.W. Vigilance and cognitive avoidance as concepts in coping research. In Attention and Avoidance; Krohne, H.W., Ed.; Hogrefe & Huber: Seattle, WA, USA, 1993; pp. 19–50. [Google Scholar]
  11. Krohne, H.W.; Egloff, B.; Varner, L.J.; Burns, L.R.; Weidner, G.; Ellis, H.C. The assessment of dispositional vigilance and cognitive avoidance: Factorial structure, psychometric properties, and validity of the Mainz Coping Inventory. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2000, 24, 297–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Krohne, H.W.; Schmukle, S.C.; Burns, L.R.; Egloff, B.; Spielberger, C.D. The measurement of coping in achievement situations: An international comparison. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2001, 30, 1225–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Weinberger, D.A.; Schwartz, G.E.; Davidson, R.J. Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles: Psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological responses to stress. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1979, 88, 369–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Egloff, B.; Hock, M. A comparison of two approaches to the assessment of coping styles. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1997, 23, 913–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kohlmann, C.-W. Rigid and flexible modes of coping: Related to coping style? Anxiety Stress Coping 1993, 6, 107–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gebhardt, C.; Rose, N.; Mitte, K. Fact or artefact: An item response theory analysis of median split based repressor classification. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2014, 19, 36–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Rauch, A.V.; Ohrmann, P.; Bauer, J.; Kugel, H.; Engelien, A.; Arolt, V.; Heindel, W.; Suslow, T. Cognitive coping style modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy humans: A 3-T fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 2526–2535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Paul, V.G.; Rauch, A.V.; Kugel, H.; Ter Horst, L.; Bauer, J.; Dannlowski, U.; Ohrmann, P.; Lindner, C.; Donges, U.-S.; Kersting, A.; et al. High responsivity to threat during the initial stage of perception in repression: A 3 T fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012, 7, 980–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Rauch, A.V.; Ter Horst, L.; Paul, V.G.; Bauer, J.; Dannlowski, U.; Konrad, C.; Ohrmann, P.; Kugel, H.; Egloff, B.; Arolt, V.; et al. Influence of repressive coping style on cortical activation during encoding of angry faces. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e112398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Klucken, T.; Kruse, O.; Schweckendiek, J.; Stark, R. Increased skin conductance responses and neural activity during fear conditioning are associated with a repressive coping style. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Stevens, F.L.; Hurley, R.A.; Taber, K.H. Anterior cingulate cortex: Unique role in cognition and emotion. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Goldin, P.R.; McRae, K.; Ramel, W.; Gross, J.J. The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 577–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Egloff, B.; Krohne, H.W. The measurement of vigilance and cognitive avoidance: Investigations with the “Angstbewältigungsinventar” (ABI). Diagnostica 1998, 44, 189–200. [Google Scholar]
  24. Günther, V.; Jahn, S.; Webelhorst, C.; Bodenschatz, C.M.; Bujanow, A.; Mucha, S.; Kersting, A.; Hoffmann, K.-T.; Egloff, B.; Lobsien, D.; et al. Coping with anxiety: Brain structural correlates of vigilance and cognitive avoidance. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 869367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Leehr, E.J.; Redlich, R.; Zaremba, D.; Dohm, K.; Böhnlein, J.; Grotegerd, D.; Kähler, C.; Repple, J.; Förster, K.; Opel, N.; et al. Structural and functional neural correlates of vigilant and avoidant regulation style. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 258, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Liddell, B.J.; Brown, K.J.; Kemp, A.H.; Barton, M.J.; Das, P.; Peduto, A.; Gordon, E.; Williams, L.M. A direct brainstem-amygdala-cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage 2005, 24, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. LeDoux, J.E. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underspinnings of Emotional Life; Simon & Schuster Paperbacks: New York, NY, USA, 1996; ISBN 9780684836591. [Google Scholar]
  28. Vuilleumier, P.; Armony, J.L.; Driver, J.; Dolan, R.J. Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 624–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Herrmann, M.J.; Ehlis, A.-C.; Muehlberger, A.; Fallgatter, A.J. Source localization of early stages of face processing. Brain Topogr. 2005, 18, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Adolphs, R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2002, 12, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Battaglia, S.; Cardellicchio, P.; Di Fazio, C.; Nazzi, C.; Fracasso, A.; Borgomaneri, S. Stopping in (e)motion: Reactive action inhibition when facing valence-independent emotional stimuli. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 998714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kirby, L.A.J.; Robinson, J.L. Affective mapping: An activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 2017, 118, 137–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sawada, R.; Sato, W. Emotional attention capture by facial expressions. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 14042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Dolan, R.J.; Vuilleumier, P. Amygdala automaticity in emotional processing. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2003, 985, 348–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Murphy, S.T.; Zajonc, R.B. Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 64, 723–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Okon-Singer, H.; Tzelgov, J.; Henik, A. Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. Emotion 2007, 7, 147–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Bishop, S.J.; Jenkins, R.; Lawrence, A.D. Neural processing of fearful faces: Effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 1595–1603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Lavie, N. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1995, 21, 451–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lavie, N. Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 19, 143–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Brockhoff, L.; Schindler, S.; Bruchmann, M.; Straube, T. Effects of perceptual and working memory load on brain responses to task-irrelevant stimuli: Review and implications for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 135, 104580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wheaton, M.G.; Fitzgerald, D.A.; Phan, K.L.; Klumpp, H. Perceptual load modulates anterior cingulate cortex response to threat distractors in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 101, 13–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Wieser, M.J.; Keil, A. Fearful faces heighten the cortical representation of contextual threat. NeuroImage 2014, 86, 317–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Izard, C.E. The Psychology of Emotions; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  44. Aleman, A.; Swart, M. Sex differences in neural activation to facial expressions denoting contempt and disgust. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e3622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Wittchen, H.; Wunderlich, U.; Gruschwitz, S.; Zaudig, M. SKID-I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  46. Laux, L.; Glanzmann, P.; Schaffner, P.; Spielberger, C.D. State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). Manual; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  47. Langner, O.; Dotsch, R.; Bijlstra, G.; Wigboldus, D.H.J.; Hawk, S.T.; van Knippenberg, A. Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cogn. Emot. 2010, 24, 1377–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Edwards, B.G.; Calhoun, V.D.; Kiehl, K.A. Joint ICA of ERP and fMRI during error-monitoring. NeuroImage 2012, 59, 1896–1903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Mugler, J.P.; Brookeman, J.R. Three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging (3D MP RAGE). Magn. Reason. Med. 1990, 15, 152–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Maldjian, J.A.; Laurienti, P.J.; Kraft, R.A.; Burdette, J.H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 1233–1239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N.; Landeau, B.; Papathanassiou, D.; Crivello, F.; Etard, O.; Delcroix, N.; Mazoyer, B.; Joliot, M. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage 2002, 15, 273–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Moors, A. Automaticity: Componential, causal, and mechanistic explanations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016, 67, 263–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Okon-Singer, H.; Mehnert, J.; Hoyer, J.; Hellrung, L.; Schaare, H.L.; Dukart, J.; Villringer, A. Neural control of vascular reactions: Impact of emotion and attention. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 4251–4259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Tamietto, M.; de Gelder, B. Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 697–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Saalmann, Y.B.; Kastner, S. Cognitive and perceptual functions of the visual thalamus. Neuron 2011, 71, 209–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Saalmann, Y.B. Intralaminar and medial thalamic influence on cortical synchrony, information transmission and cognition. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Abivardi, A.; Bach, D.R. Deconstructing white matter connectivity of human amygdala nuclei with thalamus and cortex subdivisions in vivo. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017, 38, 3927–3940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Davis, M.; Whalen, P.J. The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Mol. Psychiatry 2001, 6, 13–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Phelps, E.A.; LeDoux, J.E. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron 2005, 48, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Pessoa, L.; Adolphs, R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 773–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Cohen, R. Cuneus. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Kreutzer, J., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2011; ISBN 978-3-319-56782-2. [Google Scholar]
  62. Engell, A.D.; Haxby, J.V. Facial expression and gaze-direction in human superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 3234–3241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Nomi, J.S.; Scherfeld, D.; Friederichs, S.; Schäfer, R.; Franz, M.; Wittsack, H.-J.; Azari, N.P.; Missimer, J.; Seitz, R.J. On the neural networks of empathy: A principal component analysis of an fMRI study. Behav. Brain Funct. 2008, 4, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Ochsner, K.N.; Silvers, J.A.; Buhle, J.T. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012, 1251, E1–E24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Vanderhasselt, M.-A.; Kühn, S.; de Raedt, R. ‘Put on your poker face’: Neural systems supporting the anticipation for expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 8, 903–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Cavanna, A.E.; Trimble, M.R. The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 2006, 129, 564–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Numssen, O.; Bzdok, D.; Hartwigsen, G. Functional specialization within the inferior parietal lobes across cognitive domains. Elife 2021, 10, e63591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Shomstein, S. Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: Top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Fan, J.; Gu, X.; Liu, X.; Guise, K.G.; Park, Y.; Martin, L.; Marchena, A.; de Tang, C.Y.; Minzenberg, M.J.; Hof, P.R. Involvement of the anterior cingulate and frontoinsular cortices in rapid processing of salient facial emotional information. NeuroImage 2011, 54, 2539–2546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Adolphs, R.; Spezio, M. Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. Prog. Brain Res. 2006, 156, 363–378. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  71. Said, C.P.; Haxby, J.V.; Todorov, A. Brain systems for assessing the affective value of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 1660–1670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Leech, R.; Sharp, D.J. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 2014, 137, 12–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Maddock, R.J.; Garrett, A.S.; Buonocore, M.H. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: FMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2003, 18, 30–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Brewer, J.A.; Garrison, K.A.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. What about the "self" is processed in the posterior cingulate cortex? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Cauda, F.; Costa, T.; Torta, D.M.E.; Sacco, K.; D’Agata, F.; Duca, S.; Geminiani, G.; Fox, P.T.; Vercelli, A. Meta-analytic clustering of the insular cortex: Characterizing the meta-analytic connectivity of the insula when involved in active tasks. NeuroImage 2012, 62, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Menon, V.; Uddin, L.Q. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010, 214, 655–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  77. Craig, A.D.B. How do you feel—Now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Uddin, L.Q.; Nomi, J.S.; Hébert-Seropian, B.; Ghaziri, J.; Boucher, O. Structure and function of the human insula. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 34, 300–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Gehrlach, D.A.; Dolensek, N.; Klein, A.S.; Roy Chowdhury, R.; Matthys, A.; Junghänel, M.; Gaitanos, T.N.; Podgornik, A.; Black, T.D.; Reddy Vaka, N.; et al. Aversive state processing in the posterior insular cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2019, 22, 1424–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Kurth, F.; Zilles, K.; Fox, P.T.; Laird, A.R.; Eickhoff, S.B. A link between the systems: Functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010, 214, 519–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Straube, T.; Miltner, W.H.R. Attention to aversive emotion and specific activation of the right insula and right somatosensory cortex. NeuroImage 2011, 54, 2534–2538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Terasawa, Y.; Fukushima, H.; Umeda, S. How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 598–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Phillips, M.L.; Ladouceur, C.D.; Drevets, W.C. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: Implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 2008, 13, 829, 833–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Heilbronner, S.R.; Hayden, B.Y. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A bottom-up view. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 39, 149–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  85. Etkin, A.; Egner, T.; Kalisch, R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Gerber, A.J.; Posner, J.; Gorman, D.; Colibazzi, T.; Yu, S.; Wang, Z.; Kangarlu, A.; Zhu, H.; Russell, J.; Peterson, B.S. An affective circumplex model of neural systems subserving valence, arousal, and cognitive overlay during the appraisal of emotional faces. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 2129–2139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  87. Alvarez, R.P.; Kirlic, N.; Misaki, M.; Bodurka, J.; Rhudy, J.L.; Paulus, M.P.; Drevets, W.C. Increased anterior insula activity in anxious individuals is linked to diminished perceived control. Transl. Psychiatry 2015, 5, e591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Brooks, S.J.; Savov, V.; Allzén, E.; Benedict, C.; Fredriksson, R.; Schiöth, H.B. Exposure to subliminal arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular cortex and primary visual cortex: A systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. NeuroImage 2012, 59, 2962–2973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Critchley, H.D.; Rotshtein, P.; Nagai, Y.; O’Doherty, J.; Mathias, C.J.; Dolan, R.J. Activity in the human brain predicting differential heart rate responses to emotional facial expressions. NeuroImage 2005, 24, 751–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  90. Gianaros, P.J.; Jennings, J.R.; Sheu, L.K.; Derbyshire, S.W.G.; Matthews, K.A. Heightened functional neural activation to psychological stress covaries with exaggerated blood pressure reactivity. Hypertension 2007, 49, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  91. Burklund, L.J.; Eisenberger, N.I.; Lieberman, M.D. The face of rejection: Rejection sensitivity moderates dorsal anterior cingulate activity to disapproving facial expressions. Soc. Neuro. Sci. 2007, 2, 238–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Eisenberger, N.I.; Lieberman, M.D.; Williams, K.D. Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science 2003, 302, 290–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Schwerdtfeger, A.R.; Rathner, E.-M. The ecological validity of the autonomic-subjective response dissociation in repressive coping. Anxiety Stress Coping 2016, 29, 241–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Grant, D.M.; Wingate, L.R.; Rasmussen, K.A.; Davidson, C.L.; Slish, M.L.; Rhoades-Kerswill, S.; Mills, A.C.; Judah, M.R. An examination of the reciprocal relationship between avoidance coping and symptoms of anxiety and depression. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2013, 32, 878–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Coifman, K.G.; Bonanno, G.A.; Ray, R.D.; Gross, J.J. Does repressive coping promote resilience? Affective-autonomic response discrepancy during bereavement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 745–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bonanno, G.A.; Keltner, D.; Holen, A.; Horowitz, M.J. When avoiding unpleasant emotions might not be such a bad thing: Verbal-autonomic response dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 975–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Krohne, H.W. The concept of coping modes: Relating cognitive person variables to actual coping behavior. Adv. Behav. Res. Ther. 1989, 11, 235–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Kohlmann, C.-W.; Weidner, G.; Messina, C.R. Avoidant coping style and verbal-cardiovascular response dissociation. Psychol. Health 1996, 11, 371–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Howard, S.; Myers, L.B.; Hughes, B.M. Repressive coping and cardiovascular reactivity to novel and recurrent stress. Anxiety Stress Coping 2017, 30, 562–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Brosschot, J.F.; Janssen, E. Continuous monitoring of affective-autonomic response dissociation in repressers during negative emotional stimulation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1998, 25, 69–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Jørgensen, M.M.; Zachariae, R. Repressive coping style and autonomic reactions to two experimental stressors in healthy men and women. Scand. J. Psychol. 2006, 47, 137–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Moos, R.H.; Holahan, C.J. Dispositional and contextual perspectives on coping: Toward an integrative framework. J. Clin. Psychol. 2003, 59, 1387–1403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Rosser, B.A. Intolerance of uncertainty as a transdiagnostic mechanism of psychological difficulties: A systematic review of evidence pertaining to causality and temporal precedence. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2019, 43, 438–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Brühl, A.B.; Delsignore, A.; Komossa, K.; Weidt, S. Neuroimaging in social anxiety disorder—A meta-analytic review resulting in a new neurofunctional model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 47, 260–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Etkin, A.; Klemenhagen, K.C.; Dudman, J.T.; Rogan, M.T.; Hen, R.; Kandel, E.R.; Hirsch, J. Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron 2004, 44, 1043–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Fu, X.; Taber-Thomas, B.C.; Pérez-Edgar, K. Frontolimbic functioning during threat-related attention: Relations to early behavioral inhibition and anxiety in children. Biol. Psychol. 2017, 122, 98–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Heitmann, C.Y.; Feldker, K.; Neumeister, P.; Brinkmann, L.; Schrammen, E.; Zwitserlood, P.; Straube, T. Brain activation to task-irrelevant disorder-related threat in social anxiety disorder: The impact of symptom severity. Neuroimage Clin. 2017, 14, 323–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Blair, K.S.; Geraci, M.; Korelitz, K.; Otero, M.; Towbin, K.; Ernst, M.; Leibenluft, E.; Blair, R.J.R.; Pine, D.S. The pathology of social phobia is independent of developmental changes in face processing. Am. J. Psychiatry 2011, 168, 1202–1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  109. Feldker, K.; Heitmann, C.Y.; Neumeister, P.; Brinkmann, L.; Bruchmann, M.; Zwitserlood, P.; Straube, T. Cardiorespiratory concerns shape brain responses during automatic panic-related scene processing in patients with panic disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2018, 43, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  110. Lipka, J.; Hoffmann, M.; Miltner, W.H.R.; Straube, T. Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on brain responses to subliminal and supraliminal threat and their functional significance in specific phobia. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 76, 869–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Wright, C.I.; Fischer, H.; Whalen, P.J.; McInerney, S.C.; Shin, L.M.; Rauch, S.L. Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 2001, 12, 379–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. FMRI paradigm.
Figure 1. FMRI paradigm.
Brainsci 13 00618 g001
Figure 2. Results from whole-brain regression analyses with cognitive avoidance predicting decreased brain responsiveness to contemptuous and fearful faces under high load. (A) Sagittal and coronal images in neurological orientation showing the negative relationship between cognitive avoidance and responsivity to contemptuous (vs. neutral) faces under high perceptual load. Depicted are activation clusters in the right superior medial frontal and cingulate gyrus (BA32), supplemental motor area (BA6), precuneus, cuneus, and lingual gyrus (top left), in the right precentral (BA4), inferior frontal gyrus, and parietal and superior temporal lobes (top middle), and in the insula, postcentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, left amygdala, and supplementary motor area (top right). (B) Sagittal, coronal, and axial images showing the negative correlation between cognitive avoidance and activation in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces under high perceptual load. Depicted are activation clusters in the precentral, supramarginal, and superior temporal gyrus (lower left), in the insula, subcentral gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus (lower middle), and in the posterior insula, superior temporal gyrus, calcarine, and middle occipital gyrus (lower right). The voxel-wise threshold was set to p = 0.001 (uncorrected) with a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05, FWE-corrected. Color bar: t-values.
Figure 2. Results from whole-brain regression analyses with cognitive avoidance predicting decreased brain responsiveness to contemptuous and fearful faces under high load. (A) Sagittal and coronal images in neurological orientation showing the negative relationship between cognitive avoidance and responsivity to contemptuous (vs. neutral) faces under high perceptual load. Depicted are activation clusters in the right superior medial frontal and cingulate gyrus (BA32), supplemental motor area (BA6), precuneus, cuneus, and lingual gyrus (top left), in the right precentral (BA4), inferior frontal gyrus, and parietal and superior temporal lobes (top middle), and in the insula, postcentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, left amygdala, and supplementary motor area (top right). (B) Sagittal, coronal, and axial images showing the negative correlation between cognitive avoidance and activation in response to fearful (vs. neutral) faces under high perceptual load. Depicted are activation clusters in the precentral, supramarginal, and superior temporal gyrus (lower left), in the insula, subcentral gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus (lower middle), and in the posterior insula, superior temporal gyrus, calcarine, and middle occipital gyrus (lower right). The voxel-wise threshold was set to p = 0.001 (uncorrected) with a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05, FWE-corrected. Color bar: t-values.
Brainsci 13 00618 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations (df = 41) between psychometric measures.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations (df = 41) between psychometric measures.
CopingTrait Anxiety
M (SD)MCI-CAVSTAI
MCI-VIG22.00 (6.80)−0.41 **0.35 *
MCI-CAV23.75 (5.11)-−0.36 *
STAI-trait35.05 (6.82)--
* p < 0.05; ** <0.01 (two-tailed) MCI = Mainz Coping Inventory; VIG = Vigilance scale, CAV = Cognitive avoidance scale; STAI = State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
Table 2. Threat main effects: Brain regions showing increased activation in response to threatening (vs. neutral) faces under low and high perceptual load.
Table 2. Threat main effects: Brain regions showing increased activation in response to threatening (vs. neutral) faces under low and high perceptual load.
HemispherePeak T-ValuePeak-Level ClusterCluster-LevelPeak MNI
puncorrectedSize (Voxels)pFWExyz
Low perceptual load--------
Contempt > neutral--------
Superior extending to inferior parietal lobule R5.61<0.0011830.00442−5555
Middle temporal extending to fusiform gyrusR5.23<0.001536<0.00157−46−5
Inferior frontal gyrusR5.33<0.0012780.003452622
Inferior parietal lobule (BA40)L5.10<0.0011830.004−42−5255
Inferior temporal gyrusL4.57<0.0011710.005−42−49−17
Thalamus (ROI)L3.69<0.00110.049−281
Fear > neutral--------
---------
High perceptual load--------
Contempt > neutral- ------
---------
Fear > neutral- ------
Inferior frontal extending to precentral gyrusL4.73<0.0012300.003−511719
Superior temporal gyrusR4.89<0.0012670.001−57−4313
Superior temporal gyrus L5.33<0.0012170.004−57−4613
CerebellumR4.62<0.001419<0.00142−67−29
Neuroanatomical labels, hemisphere, peak voxel t- and p-values, cluster extent, cluster-level p-values, and coordinates in MNI space are presented. Activation clusters are yielded by one-sample t-tests and are significant at p = 0.001 (uncorrected) and a cluster-level threshold of pFWE < 0.05.
Table 3. Brain regions showing a negative correlation with cognitive avoidant coping style in response to threatening faces under high perceptual load.
Table 3. Brain regions showing a negative correlation with cognitive avoidant coping style in response to threatening faces under high perceptual load.
HemispherePeak T-ValuePeak-Level ClusterCluster-LevelPeak MNI
puncorrectedSize (Voxels)pFWExyz
Contempt (vs. neutral)--------
Left postcentral gyrus, extending to the following regions:L/R6.29<0.0015770<0.001−63−1016
Bilateral precentral (BA6), superior, and middle frontal gyrus (BA8)L/R5.18<0.001--−51−116
Bilateral posterior and anterior insulaL/R6.14<0.001--42−44
Bilateral lingual and parahippocampal gyrus, calcarine sulcus and precuneusL/R5.16<0.001--−18−551
Bilateral superior temporal gyrusL/R5.38<0.001--−48−3719
Bilateral supramarginal and inferior parietal gyrusL/R4.67<0.001--−63−1937
AmygdalaL/R3.46<0.001--−27−7−11
ThalamusL3.79<0.001--−6−1016
Dorsal anterior and middle cingulate gyrus (BA32), extending to medial frontal cortex (BA6,8,9)R4.59<0.0012140.00293528
Bilateral supplementary motor area (BA6), extending to the left medial frontal and middle cingulate gyrus (BA31)L/R4.51<0.0012210.001−12−752
Right postcentral gyrus extending to inferior parietal gyrusR4.13<0.0011580.00833−4058
Fear (vs. neutral)
Subcentral gyrus, extending to the following regions:L/R5.67<0.0013634<0.00145−1922
Bilateral superior temporal gyrus L/R4.69<0.001--45−10−2
Posterior insulaR4.81<0.001--45−101
Precentral gyrusR4.68<0.001--
Bilateral posterior cingulateL/R3.77<0.001--12−4028
Bilateral lingual gyrus, cuneus and precuneusL/R5.05<0.001--15−43−5
AmygdalaR3.49<0.001--27−4−11
ThalamusL/R4.25<0.001--−3−13−2
Bilateral anterior cingulate gyrus (BA25, 32)L/R4.75<0.0013530.001−317−5
Fusiform gyrusL4.22<0.001307<0.001−36−46−23
Postcentral gyrusR4.49<0.0011610.00812−4373
Postcentral extending to superior temporal gyrusL4.41<0.0011100.03−36−1928
Neuroanatomical labels, hemisphere, peak voxel t- and p-values, cluster extent, cluster-level p-values, and coordinates in MNI space are presented. Activation clusters are yielded by regression analyses and are significant at p = 0.001 (uncorrected) and a cluster-level threshold of pFWE < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Günther, V.; Strukova, M.; Pecher, J.; Webelhorst, C.; Engelmann, S.; Kersting, A.; Hoffmann, K.-T.; Egloff, B.; Okon-Singer, H.; Lobsien, D.; et al. Cognitive Avoidance Is Associated with Decreased Brain Responsiveness to Threat Distractors under High Perceptual Load. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040618

AMA Style

Günther V, Strukova M, Pecher J, Webelhorst C, Engelmann S, Kersting A, Hoffmann K-T, Egloff B, Okon-Singer H, Lobsien D, et al. Cognitive Avoidance Is Associated with Decreased Brain Responsiveness to Threat Distractors under High Perceptual Load. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(4):618. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040618

Chicago/Turabian Style

Günther, Vivien, Mariia Strukova, Jonas Pecher, Carolin Webelhorst, Simone Engelmann, Anette Kersting, Karl-Titus Hoffmann, Boris Egloff, Hadas Okon-Singer, Donald Lobsien, and et al. 2023. "Cognitive Avoidance Is Associated with Decreased Brain Responsiveness to Threat Distractors under High Perceptual Load" Brain Sciences 13, no. 4: 618. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040618

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop