Next Article in Journal
Behavioral Phenotype in Heterozygous DAT Rats: Transgenerational Transmission of Maternal Impact and the Role of Genetic Asset
Next Article in Special Issue
Development of Landmark Use for Navigation in Children: Effects of Age, Sex, Working Memory and Landmark Type
Previous Article in Journal
Exploring the Effects of Brain Stimulation on Musical Taste: tDCS on the Left Dorso-Lateral Prefrontal Cortex—A Null Result
Previous Article in Special Issue
Spatial Learning in a Virtual Environment: The Role of Self-Efficacy Feedback and Individual Visuospatial Factors
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Application of Real and Virtual Radial Arm Maze Task in Human

1
Department of Social and Developmental Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Humanities, University of Naples “Federico II”, 80133 Naples, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(4), 468; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040468
Submission received: 25 January 2022 / Revised: 23 March 2022 / Accepted: 29 March 2022 / Published: 31 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Application of Virtual Reality in Spatial Memory)

Abstract

:
Virtual Reality (VR) emerges as a promising technology capable of creating different scenarios in which the body, environment, and brain are closely related, proving enhancements in the diagnosis and treatment of several spatial memory deficits. In recent years, human spatial navigation has increasingly been studied in interactive virtual environments. However, navigational tasks are still not completely adapted in immersive 3D VR systems. We stipulate that an immersive Radial Arm Maze (RAM) is an excellent instrument, allowing the participants to be physically active within the maze exactly as in the walking RAM version in reality modality. RAM is a behavioral ecological task that allows the analyses of different facets of spatial memory, distinguishing declarative components from procedural ones. In addition to describing the characteristics of RAM, we will also analyze studies in which RAM has been used in virtual modality to provide suggestions into RAM building in immersive modality.

1. Introduction

Virtual reality (VR) has been traditionally defined as “interactive, virtual image displays enhanced by special processing and by non-visual display modalities … to convince users that they are immersed in a synthetic space” [1]. Nowadays, VR emerges as a promising technology useful in different fields of clinical applications [2,3]. In particular, by means of creating different scenarios in which the body, environment, and brain are closely related, VR facilitates the improvement and/or the recovery of spatial cognitive abilities, thus offering a great advantage in diagnosis and treatment of several spatial memory deficits [4,5,6,7,8,9]. The key strength of VR for screening spatial abilities refers to the capacity to mimic real-world tasks showing a correlation between real and virtual environments [10]. The simulated environment can be easily manipulated, facilitating experimental tasks that are difficult to implement in real-world settings. Other benefits of VR regard the possibility to participate in potentially dangerous tasks, such as moving in a complex environment or driving a car, in a controlled ecological setting [11], thus allowing to perform a task safely [12,13,14,15,16].
Several clinical evidences had documented the use of VR for screening spatial memory impairments in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer Disease (AD) [3,11,14,17,18,19,20]. Moreover, VR technology had been implemented in the rehabilitation training of spatial abilities on patients with cognitive-spatial impairments, thus demonstrating how it represents valid support to the main methods of intervention [21].
The technological progress of VR has expanded the range of tools and types of questions on spatial learning, adapting most of the standard spatial tasks to the virtual version. The Radial Arm Maze (RAM), developed by Olton and Samuelson (1976) [22] and quickly adapted in humans, is a high ecological spatial task, firstly used in a real environment and subsequently in the virtual one.
The continuous evolution of VR devices has brought new challenges for researchers who implemented the standard tools to assess spatial abilities with new technological devices. Focusing on the RAM task, it is necessary to carry out an overview to understand how technology has necessitated an adaptation of RAM used for screening and rehabilitation of spatial abilities.
The purpose of the present study is to narratively review the literature dealing with the RAM task, both in its classic and virtual adaptations. With this aim, firstly, the traditional RAM paradigms and some of their applications in different clinical settings will be illustrated. Subsequently, the studies that have used the RAM task in virtual environments will be discussed to provide a careful reflection as to the potential and development of completely immersive RAM versions, which are still little used.

2. The Evolution of Virtual Reality Technology

The concept of VR was formulated in 1960 by computer scientist Ivan Sutherland, who created the Ultimate Display, the first head-mounted device. Although several definitions of VR have been formulated, the main features of the VR system are the immersion, the sense of the presence in a simulated environment, and the interaction with that environment [23,24,25]. In particular, the immersion depends on the characteristics of the VR system to simulate real interactions and stimuli in a virtual environment [25]. In scientific literature are reported three different types of VR systems that provide different degrees of immersion:
Non-immersive VR systems include the development of a 2D virtual environment projected on a computer screen, such as serious videogames. In this condition, using a PC monitor, keyboard, and a mouse, but also joysticks or gamepads, the subject performing a spatial task can be forced to view the scene using a route perspective [26].
Immersive VR systems provide a complete simulated experience using several devices, such as head-mounted displays, audio, and haptic devices. This technology enhances the stereoscopic view of the 3D simulated environment through the movement of the user’s head [27], and offers 360° audio and visual stimuli to perceive the user’s interaction as real [28,29].
Semi-immersive VR systems have some features in common with the two systems mentioned above. This technology includes a stereo image of a 3D simulated environment viewed on a PC monitor using a perspective projection linked to the head position of the observer [27,30].
Other features of VR, as mentioned before, regard the sense of presence and interaction with the virtual environment. The sense of presence regards the sensation and feelings of the users to be physically present in the virtual environment, having the opportunity to interact and react with the stimuli as if the user was in the real world [27].
Although the concept of VR was formulated in 1960, over the last 25 years, several scholars explored the utility, effects, and applications of this technology [27]. In the first phase, studies of VR referred to the computer graphics field. Successively, the researchers extended VR technology to several disciplines, representing a valuable tool for psychologists and neuroscientists [27].
In most recent years, the amount of research into psychological health and neuroscience has increased, demonstrating the validity of the VR tools. A recent review conducted by Cipresso and colleagues showed that VR had been widely used in navigation studies [27]. The Nobel prize winner Edvard Moser highlighted the potential of VR that can be implemented in navigational studies [31]. This evidence shows that the development and implementation of VR systems in research and clinical practice could provide a valuable contribution to assessing spatial abilities.

3. Main Spatial Tasks Suitable for Virtual Environments

Experimental psychological research offers several navigational tasks to study spatial abilities. Nevertheless, few studies adapted the classical spatial experimental paradigms using VR technology. In this contest, spatial cognition research has distinguished itself for developing the virtual versions of the Morris Water Maze (MWM) and the Radial Arm Maze (RAM), the behavioral tasks most used in the study of spatial function [32,33,34,35]. Both tasks were used in the animal cognition field. Only in most recent years, they are becoming part of the evaluation tools in research involving human participants to study the development of spatial abilities, or their functioning in specific clinical populations [13,36,37,38,39].
The MWM adapted to humans consists of a large circular pool in which the subject has to find a hidden object, while the RAM is formed by a central holding area from which a number of identical arms radiate. Although both tasks have a unique contribution in analyzing spatial memory, compared to MWM, RAM offers the great advantage of being administered with several paradigms that investigate the different faces of the spatial memory process, providing more opportunities to evaluate the spatial working memory component compared to the real and virtual pool [35]. In addition, while in the MWM task, the subject may perform an infinite number of path trajectories, whereas in the RAM task, the choices of the entries are constrained, thus facilitating the identification of the strategy put into action. Moreover, thanks to the many parameters that can be calculated, RAM is a sensitive and ecological tool for the diagnosis and study of spatial deficit. However, it should be noted that many studies analyzing spatial abilities in humans have used new apparatuses including both MWM and RAM tasks, i.e., [36] have developed new navigational paradigms to compare the spatial abilities in real and virtual modality [37] alongside other research.
Despite these advantages, the use of RAM in VR technology is still too limited and not completely adapted in immersive VR systems.

4. Method

A comprehensive search within the literature was used to detect the available studies related to the use of RAM in the real and virtual modalities in the human sample. We carried out searches in PubMed/Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, and Pubmed databases in the date range from 1976 (date of the first RAM study) to December 2021. Search terms included a combination of “Radial Maze”, “Arm Maze”, “Radial Arm Maze”. To avoid animal studies, articles containing the following keywords have been excluded: “Mice”, “Rat”, “Rodent”, “Gerbil”, “Pigeon”, “Fish”. Once the main articles were identified, to supplement the aforementioned search terms, a second search was carried out using the citations within each article.
Only articles in peer-reviewed journals and written in the English language were included.
Studies that arose from the search terms were assessed for further evaluation via abstract review, and duplicates were removed. In addition, the reference lists of the full-text articles were manually checked to identify other studies that were cross-referenced, in order to find further existing articles on the use of RAM. As a result, we detected in total 433 potentially relevant articles. After screening the titles and abstracts, a total of 352 articles were eliminated. The remaining 81 studies were examined by full-text review, and 16 of them were further excluded until they reached 65 studies in total. All the excluded studies did not meet the eligibility criteria. Figure 1 is reported the PRISMA flow diagram regarding the search process [38].
The high number of studies excluded in the screening phase is due to the broad use of RAM in animal research. The eligibility criteria referred to spatial abilities being evaluated using RAM. Specifically, we have selected only studies covering the different facets of spatial memory processes. The 16 studies were excluded in the eligibility phase because they focused on other processes, such as attention or perception. Various data were extracted from each study, including the number and age of participants, type of population, RAM paradigm, and real or virtual environment (Table 1 and Table 2).

5. Radial Arm Maze Task (RAM)

5.1. Free-Choice and Forced-Choice Version

The Radial Arm Maze (RAM) task, developed by Olton and Samuelson (1976) to assess the spatial abilities in rodents, is also used in several studies on children [42,45,46,47,51,55] and adults [48,49,50].
RAM consists of a central holding area from which several identical arms, commonly eight, radiate, and the task’s difficulty depends on the number of them. There is a hidden reward at the end of each arm, generally a coin or a little toy for children.
Different RAM paradigms take into account the environmental cues and the kind of spatial memory process to be investigated. Generally, it is possible to distinguish two main classic paradigms: free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions (Figure 2).
In the free-choice version, the subjects have to take all the rewards and know that the arms are rewarded only once (declarative rule). To solve the task without errors, the subject has to make use of mnesic and mapping abilities, as well as proficient explorative strategies [46,98,101]. Carrying out the RAM in several trials, these competencies can be learned during the various phases of the task.
For many years, the free-choice RAM version has been considered appropriate for evaluating the correct functioning of working-term memory abilities by detecting the number of errors (e.g., returning to arms already visited). However, some authors observed that the longest sequence of correctly visited arms, corresponding to spatial span parameter, can also depend on the type of strategy put into action to explore the maze, suggesting the potential that RAM offers to evaluate procedural memory processes [102,103]. In addition, employing different parameters, the free-choice RAM version allows to efficaciously study the explorative strategies used by the subject. For example, it is possible to analyze if he/she visits a specific sequence of arms or always beginning a run from the same arm (“praxic” strategy), or if he/she solves the task by referring to specific environmental stimuli (“taxic” strategy), or finally, if he/she exploits mapping abilities to build a cognitive spatial map (“place” strategy) [53,101,104]. To refer to these different strategies, numerous terms, sich as “motor”, “cue”, or “relational” strategies, respectively, have been used in other studies [46,101].
Therefore, to distinguish explorative from working mnesic components, it is possible to use the forced-choice RAM version. In this protocol, each trial consists of two phases. In the first phase, the subjects have to collect only four rewards, while the remaining ones are inaccessible. In the second phase, he/she has to collect the rewards of the four arms not visited in the first phase. Success depends on remembering the arms visited in the first phase (rather than putting into action particular search patterns), thus emphasizing working memory requirements. Putting into action a specific exploratory strategy is avoided using different angles to separate the opened arms (i.e., arms 2,4,5,8). Therefore, although both paradigms investigate spatial memory processes, they analyze different aspects of these processes.
Both versions of RAM can be cued or uncued [46,56]. In the cued RAM version, each arm is made physically distinct by visual stimulus at its end. In the uncued RAM version, visited arms can be remembered by the subjects in relation to their spatial relationship to distal extra maze cues. It is easy to understand that when using the cued RAM version, the subject is forced to apply a taxic strategy to solve the task, while an allocentric strategy in the uncued RAM version is more appropriate. Again, the type of paradigm allows to investigate different aspects of spatial exploration.
Therefore, the choice of a specific RAM paradigm depends on the type of study objective to be achieved, and the normotypical and clinical population to be studied. For example, for children around the age of four years, who have not yet developed short-term memory processes, the use of free choice may be more appropriate.
In Table 3, the main parameters analyzed in free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions are reported to emphasize the different faces of memory components that can be studied throughout real and virtual RAM tasks.
It is appropriate to note that in free-choice and forced-choice RAM versions, the subject walks around the maze, and this promotes the integration of the mechanisms that link perception to action. This feature suggests that RAM is a complete task as it allows to also analyze perceptive and motor processes. Furthermore, the exploration of an environment through moving in it accelerates the spatial learning processes, allowing the formation of a spatial cognitive map [105], thus indicating RAM as a tool for devising virtual personalized neurorehabilitation training, as is already being done with other experimental protocols [106].

5.2. Table RAM and Visuospatial Peripersonal Abilities

The RAM is cataloged among large-scale behavioral tasks since it is a walking task. The subjects are inside the maze and see it from the inside, thus promoting an allocentric and egocentric encoding. The participant is compelled to build a spatial cognitive map of RAM to orient and move himself/herself in it. In this way, the declarative competence of the environment is probably built through procedural competence [103]. Recently, Foti and collaborators have developed a RAM table version that allows studying the visuospatial peripersonal abilities through body–objects interaction [54]. In fact, in this table RAM version, the participant is forced to explore the portion of space accessible with the limbs in order to resolve the task, which was presented to children as the “Ladybug game”. The child had to move the older sister ladybug, placed on the central platform, to find its sisters hidden inside the caps at the end of each arm [54]. The child is seated in front of the RAM and has visual access to the maze in all its completeness. Seeing it from above, it is likely that the construction of the spatial cognitive map may be facilitated because declarative knowledge is promptly formed. In addition, recent scientific literature reported that tactile and visual stimuli inside the peripersonal space elicit stronger processing and induce a powerful multilevel activation [107], inducing an integration of perceptive, motor, and cognitive processes. When we see an object and recognize its function, we also know how to grasp it, preparing ourselves for the action to be enacted upon it. These characteristics related to the process that links the perception to the action suggest the table RAM is an advantageous tool to improve peripersonal spatial abilities. Furthermore, in this RAM table version, the two RAM paradigms, free choice and forced choice, were administered. However, this time it is necessary to point out that the free choice paradigm served as habituation to the setting. In contrast, the forced choice paradigm constituted the experimental part of the study. The reason for this is easy to understand, as on a small scale, free exploration is elementary, even for children. In the future, it may be helpful to administer free choice to populations with marked cognitive deficits, such as in neglect syndrome.
In this line of thinking, it is interesting to note that another group of researchers has used a small-scale RAM model to investigate the age at which children begin to integrate the increasing flexibility in the conjoint use of egocentric and allocentric frames of reference [41], obtaining data comparable to those of classic neuropsychological spatial tests, such as Corsi Block task or block construction [42,53], indicating also the reliability of this ecological task. In the past, O’Connor and Glassman used a radial maze analog drawn on paper to study short-term memory [48], first suggesting the RAM as a tabletop tool.

6. Applications of RAM Task in Real Environment

As described above, RAM is a behavioral ecological task on a large and small scale that allows the analyses of different facets of spatial memory. In humans, several clinical and psychological studies have extensively used the walking RAM version for analyzing the navigational abilities in individuals with typical development (TD) and the spatial deficit in specific clinical populations.
In the late ‘80s, walking RAM was used in children to study spatial memory and understand from what age it could be administered [45,47]. These studies have shown that even preschool infants can walk in RAM. However, the variable dimensions regarding length and number of arms and the experimental setting have confused the results. About ten years after these pioneering studies, Overmann and colleagues developed a RAM built to human scale in which children were tested without explicit verbal instructions and with a longitudinal procedure for up to 16 consecutive weekdays, using free-choice and forced-choice versions [46]. In a sense, Overmann’s study confirms the precedents, even though it aimed to observe the development of mapping abilities rather than evaluate the age of administration of RAM. Successively, other behavioral studies on TD children were carried out employing both versions of the walking RAM to investigate the ontogenesis of spatial competencies and eventually gender differences [42,43,44,55] as well, so as to better characterize the spatial deficit in adolescents with Williams and Prader-Willi syndromes [52,53] and to evaluate the spatial orientation of intrauterine growth retarded children [51]. All these studies have shown how RAM can analyze the development of a process (spatial abilities) and highlight the presence and severity of a spatial deficit.
Recently, the walking RAM task has also been used to compare learning by observation to learning by doing in TD children [40]. In this study, the authors have made clear that the observation of the correct explorative strategy showed by the experimenter promotes the development of spatial declarative and procedural knowledge, thus suggesting the RAM task as a useful tool for improving and facilitating spatial memory. In particular, the authors highlighted that the observation of a correct exploration strategy, such as the entry into the adjacent arms, induces an early development of the spatial cognitive map in the observing child. This study suggests that RAM can also be an educational tool to facilitate and accelerate learning processes.
Even in adults, the first studies that used the walking RAM date back to the 1980s. In some of these, participants’ performances were compared to those of the rats in analog mazes [48]. Successively, the RAM task was mainly used to study human navigation behavior in health and clinical populations [48,49,50,55,56,57], highlighting once again how RAM can be used for diagnostic purposes. Recently, a RAM version has been also used to evidence physical activity effects on spatial abilities [39]. In fact, by comparing the performance of athletes with those of a sedentary group, it was possible to highlight how physical exercise improves spatial memory.
However, in these studies, the behavioral procedure is not always comparable. For example, in some of them, it is preferred to use the free choice version with only part of the arms baited [96], or to insert specific cue intramaze, or change the starting arm [56]. As already pointed out, the choice of one or the other version of the RAM task depends on the age of the participants and on the type of memory process to be studied.
Although these differences make the results confusing and not homogeneous, they demonstrate once again the extent to which RAM task is a flexible tool that can be easily adapted to the type of spatial process to be investigated and the type of deficit to be rehabilitated.

7. Potentiality and Applications of RAM Task in Virtual Environment

The RAM task is a highly ecological test because it is administered outside hospital environments and experimental settings of research laboratories. Aside for a few examples, RAM is a large-scale task that is presented as a game, especially in children. When considering the different RAM paradigms and versions, overall, on the one hand, they have favored objectives and reliable results, also correlating to aseptic paper and pencil tests. However, on the other hand, their design has hampered RAM use, as it is very expensive to assemble them in real environments. Furthermore, as RAM tasks are very often performed outdoors and generally last a few days, they are also affected by weather conditions. All these difficulties may explain why the RAM task is only partially used in humans compared to its extensive application in animal research and the numerous evidences in the implementation in virtual modality (Table 2) made possible by VR technology progress. As has been already pointed out, VR offers several advantages, such as the possibility to evaluate people in complete safety [11,12]. Another possible advantage consists of manipulating the environment, for example, making it increasingly complex or easier to explore, thus allowing for more personalization as well as a more interactive subject-environment. In addition, the changes that can be made in virtual modality allow to specifically investigate the type of strategy used by the participant to solve the task. While in real RAM version, for example, it is not certain whether the participant has oriented himself/herself according to the external cues, which, although kept under control cannot be stable (for example, a strong wind, variable brightness, etc.). In virtual RAM version, it is possible to modify the surrounding landmarks and keep other conditions constant, analyzing the procedural competences in more detail. Despite this, most of the studies that have used RAM in virtual modality have adopted the forced-choice version of the task (Table 2), which allows analyzing working and short-term memory processes rather than the type of strategy used by the subject. A possible explanation could be that the free choice version is apparently easier than the forced-choice one, and since the participants were mainly young adults, the researchers believed it more useful to administer a RAM version emphasizing working memory requirements.
Other potential advantages relate to the fact that the digitized versions of the RAM task can be easily shared by several groups of researchers, and that the data obtained can be entered into scientific databases. With the aim of eventually implementing rehabilitative intervention, it could be possible to imagine a sort of “videogame training” that the patient can perform inside his/her home when motivated to do so.
The present review has evidenced 46 papers concerning virtual RAM task as the method chosen to investigate different aspects of human behavior, not only related to analyzing the spatial abilities. In most of them, a non-immersive VR modality of RAM task has been used.
To our knowledge, the first evidence of a virtual RAM task goes back to the Iaria et al. study in 2003. The authors created an eight-arm radial maze with a central starting location. The maze was surrounded by a landscape (mountains and sunset), two trees, and a short wall located between the landscape and the tree. At the end of each arm, there was a staircase leading to the location where an object could be picked up in some of the arms. The participants were young, healthy adults who used a keypad to move in any direction [34]. Successively, joysticks were also used to navigate through the virtual RAM, but the subjects were always seated in front of the computer [33,91,92,93,94,96,97,98,99,100]. In 2012, a study of 599, including TD children and younger to older healthy adults, demonstrated the virtual RAM task to be a useful tool with which to investigate the changes in exploratory strategies over life span [87], confirming the results of the studies conducted with real RAM, but adding valuable information as to environmental factors that can modulate the development of navigational strategies. In the last twenty years, studies with virtual RAM tasks have greatly increased, and more and more evidence also relates to clinical populations and children, as well as the suggestion of new paradigms of task-based RAM [74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,83,88,94,95,96]. For example, Marsh and colleagues (2015) had administered a virtual eight RAM version during fMRI scanning of adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) in order to study the functioning of mesolimbic and striatal areas involved in reward-based spatial learning [79]. Furthermore, other authors investigated navigational strategies in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) children [78]. The use of virtual RAM in these clinical populations suggests how it is suitable for individuals exhibiting behavioral alterations.
Recently, a digitalized version of the RAM task was also used to investigate the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the spatial exploration in Italian University students, allowing to evidence an increase in pseudoneglect through analysis of the lateralization of the first explored arm [60]. However, many studies use RAM in non-immersive modality to evaluate spatial abilities [61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73].
To date, only two studies reported the virtual RAM task in full immersive modality [58,59]. In particular, Kim et al. have developed a virtual RAM task with a head-mounted display to produce information about travel distance and head movement, demonstrating that this virtual task was just as competent as the walking task one in measuring spatial learning and memory [58]. More recently, Ben-Zeev and colleagues have produced a virtual RAM task in which the subjects wore specific virtual reality goggles as a display that enabled them to see the room in a first-person perspective, as well as a rotating tool of the view, due to its capability to translate head movements in real-time as shifts of the viewpoint [59].
From the analysis of the studies carried out, it is clear that most of them use the forced choice paradigm (Table 2), and this observation deserves careful consideration. Once again, the two paradigms allowed to evaluate different facets of spatial memory. Still, the forced choice method is more sensitive to the short memory components, and is also more challenging to perform. However, in virtual modality, it is easier to modify scenarios by reducing (or increasing) the complexity of the task. Perhaps this could be why forced choice in virtual modality is more frequent than free choice.

8. Conclusion and Future Perspectives

The present review highlights the potential of the RAM task to study spatial abilities in real and virtual environments. Furthermore, the analysis of the scientific literature has shown an increase in studies with virtual RAM tasks.
However, almost all of these tasks are based on non-immersive methodology that offers several advantages, but does have some limitations. It is important to underline the benefits of using non-immersive virtual RAM tasks, as the experimenter can easily modify the scenario. Moreover, using non-immersive VR, it is possible to build virtual environments that can be seen by the subject from a monitor, forcing both a view from above (i.e., the map of a city) and a view from the inside (i.e., using a third-person perspective in which the subject sees his/her or another virtual body moving in a videogame), promoting thus the building of the spatial cognitive map influencing egocentric and allocentric encoding [63].
In relation to limits that non-immersive VR has, being a simple video viewing experience, it cannot permit the experience of feeling like an integral part of a context [28]. Thus, RAM exploration is not entirely comparable to real exploration in the maze, and care should be taken when evaluating patients with a spatial deficit [10]. However, these limits can be overcome using immersive VR. Compared to non-immersive VR, immersive VR leverages the vestibular and proprioceptive systems to further engage the participant, and this involvement seems to be related to spatial memory recall [108]. Recently, it has been demonstrated that walking induces oscillations, peaking within a lower frequency band in the hippocampus, which correlate to spatial memory processing [109], thus suggesting immersive VR is a suitable technology for promoting spatial memory. Therefore, using full immersive VR, it is possible to stress procedural and declarative components of spatial memory processes [82,84,110], which are equally necessary for efficient exploration and construction of the cognitive spatial map [42,85,101]. Just like in a real environment, through full immersive VR, the subject explores the virtual scenario considering the position of the virtual environmental cues and his/her own position to these (declarative competence). Simultaneously, he/she can move in the virtual environment to reach (or avoid) specific objects, putting into action an exploration strategy (procedural competence) [54]. These observations encourage careful consideration in promoting therapeutic strategies for improving and recovering spatial abilities through full immersive virtual navigation training, and in this context, the RAM task reveals itself to be a suitable tool for use in neurorehabilitation. However, it is important to emphasize that there are also other spatial tasks adapted in virtual modality, such as the virtual MWM or the virtual boxes room task [106,111], and that new research has to be conducted in order to find which procedure is more useful for the assessment and rehabilitation of spatial memory in different pathologies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.M. and F.L.; methodology, T.P., A.C. and F.A.; formal analysis, T.P.; data curation, T.P. and L.M.; writing—original draft preparation, T.P.; writing—review and editing, T.P., L.M., A.C., F.A. and F.L.; supervision, L.M. and F.L.; funding acquisition, L.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by funding from the Department of Humanities, University of Naples Federico II (Fondi ricerca dipartimentale 30% and 70% 2020/2021) to L.M.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available under request to the corresponding author Tommaso Palombi ([email protected]).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ellis, S.R. What Are Virtual Environments? IEEE Comput. Graph. Appl. 1994, 14, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chirico, A.; Lucidi, F.; De Laurentiis, M.; Milanese, C.; Napoli, A.; Giordano, A. Virtual Reality in Health System: Beyond Entertainment. A Mini-Review on the Efficacy of VR during Cancer Treatment. J. Cell Physiol. 2016, 231, 275–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Chirico, A.; Maiorano, P.; Indovina, P.; Milanese, C.; Giordano, G.G.; Alivernini, F.; Iodice, G.; Gallo, L.; De Pietro, G.; Lucidi, F.; et al. Virtual reality and music therapy as distraction interventions to alleviate anxiety and improve mood states in breast cancer patients during chemotherapy. J. Cell Physiol. 2020, 235, 5353–5362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Brooks, B.M. Route learning in a case of amnesia: A preliminary investigation into the efficacy of training in a virtual environment. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 1999, 9, 63–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Hofmann, M.; Rösler, A.; Schwarz, W.; Muller-Spahn, F.; Krauchi, K.; Hock, C.; Seifiritz, E. Interactive computer-training as a therapeutic tool in Alzheimer’s disease. Compr. Psychiatry 2003, 44, 213–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Optale, G.; Urgesi, C.; Busato, V.; Marin, S.; Piron, L.; Priftis, K.; Gamberini, L.; Capodieci, S.; Bordin, A. Controlling memory impairment in elderly adults using virtual reality memory training: A randomized controlled pilot study. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 2010, 24, 348–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Jekel, K.; Damian, M.; Wattmo, C.; Hausner, L.; Bullock, R.; Connelly, P.; Dubois, B.; Eriksdotter, M.; Ewers, M.; Graessel, E.; et al. Mild cognitive impairment and deficits in instrumental activities of daily living: A systematic review. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2015, 7, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Claessen, M.H.G.; Visser-Meily, J.M.A.; De Rooij, N.K.; Postma, A.; Van Der Ham, I.J.M. A Direct Comparison of Real-World and Virtual Navigation Performance in Chronic Stroke Patients. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2016, 22, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Montana, J.I.; Tuena, C.; Serino, S.; Cipresso, P.; Riva, G. Neurorehabilitation of spatial memory using virtual environments: A systematic review. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 1516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Jonson, M.; Avramescu, S.; Chen, D.; Alam, F. The Role of Virtual Reality in Screening, Diagnosing, and Rehabilitating Spatial Memory Deficits. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 628818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Plancher, G.; Tirard, A.; Gyselinck, V.; Nicolas, S.; Piolino, P. Using virtual reality to characterize episodic memory profiles in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Influence of active and passive encoding. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 592–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Maguire, E.A.; Nannery, R.; Spiers, H.J. Navigation around London by a taxi driver with bilateral hippocampal lesions. Brain 2006, 129, 2894–2907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hort, J.; Laczó, J.; Vyhnálek, M.; Bojar, M.; Bureš, J.; Vlček, K. Spatial navigation deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 4042–4047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Cushman, L.A.; Stein, K.; Duffy, C.J. Detecting navigational deficits in cognitive aging and Alzheimer disease using virtual reality. Neurology 2008, 71, 888–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. van der Ham, I.J.M.; Faber, A.M.E.; Venselaar, M.; van Kreveld, M.J.; Löffler, M. Ecological validity of virtual environments to assess human navigation ability. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Cogné, M.; Taillade, M.; N’Kaoua, B.; Tarruella, A.; Klinger, E.; Larrue, F.; Sauzéon, H.; Joseph, P.A.; Sorita, E. The contribution of virtual reality to the diagnosis of spatial navigation disorders and to the study of the role of navigational aids: A systematic literature review. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2017, 60, 164–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Weniger, G.; Ruhleder, M.; Lange, C.; Wolf, S.; Irle, E. Egocentric and allocentric memory as assessed by virtual reality in individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 518–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chirico, A.; Giovannetti, T.; Neroni, P.; Simone, S.; Gallo, L.; Galli, F.; Giancamilli, F.; Predazzi, M.; Lucidi, F.; De Pietro, G.; et al. Virtual Reality for the Assessment of Everyday Cognitive Functions in Older Adults: An Evaluation of the Virtual Reality Action Test and Two Interaction Devices in a 91-Year-Old Woman. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Davis, R.; Ohman, J.M.; Weisbeck, C. Salient cues and wayfinding in Alzheimer’s disease within a virtual senior residence. Environ. Behav. 2017, 49, 1038–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Montenegro, J.M.F.; Argyriou, V. Cognitive evaluation for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease based on turing test and virtual environments. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 173, 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Dovis, S.; Van der Oord, S.; Wiers, R.W.; Prins, P.J.M. Improving Executive Functioning in Children with ADHD: Training Multiple Executive Functions within the Context of a Computer Game. A Randomized Double- Blind Placebo Controlled Trial. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0121651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Olton, D.S.; Samuelson, R.J. Remembrance of placed passed: Spatial memory in rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. 1976, 2, 97–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bailenson, J.N.; Yee, N.; Merget, D.; Schroeder, R. The effect of behavioral realism and form realism of real-time avatar faces on verbal disclosure, nonverbal disclosure, emotion recognition, and copresence in dyadic interaction. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 2006, 15, 359–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Andersen, S.M.; Thorpe, J.S. An IF-THEN theory of personality: Significant others and the relational self. J. Res. Pers. 2009, 43, 163–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Slater, M. Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2009, 364, 3549–3557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Marková, H.; Laczó, J.; Andel, R.; Hort, J.; Vlček, K. Perspective taking abilities in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 284, 229–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cipresso, P.; Giglioli, I.A.C.; Raya, M.A.; Riva, G. The past, present, and future of virtual and augmented reality research: A network and cluster analysis of the literature. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. García-Betances, R.I.; Arredondo Waldmeyer, M.T.; Fico, G.; Cabrera-Umpiérrez, M.F. A succinct overview of virtual reality technology use in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. D’Cunha, N.M.; Nguyen, D.; Naumovski, N.; McKune, J.A.; Kellett, J.; Georgousopoulou, N.E.; Frost, J.; Isbel, S. A mini-review of virtual reality-based interventions to promote well-being for people living with dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Gerontology 2019, 65, 430–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ware, C.; Arthur, K.; Booth, K.S. Fish tank virtual reality. In Proceedings of the INTERACT’93 and CHI’93 Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, (Amsterdam: ACM), New Orleans, LA, USA, 24–29 April 1993; pp. 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Minderer, M.; Harvey, C.D.; Donato, F.; Moser, E.I. Neuroscience: Virtual reality explored. Nature 2016, 533, 324–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Cornwell, B.R.; Johnson, L.L.; Holroyd, T.; Carver, F.W.; Grillon, C. Human hippocampal and parahippocampal theta during goal-directed spatial navigation predicts performance on a virtual Morris water maze. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 5983–5990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Bohbot, V.D.; Iaria, G.; Petrides, M. Hippocampal function and spatial memory: Evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. Neuropsychology 2004, 18, 418–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Iaria, G.; Petrides, M.; Dagher, A.; Pike, B.; Bohbot, V.D. Cognitive strategies dependent on the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in human navigation: Variability and change with practice. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 5945–5952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Patel, S.A.; Frick, K.M.; Newhouse, P.A.; Astur, R.S. Estradiol effects on spatial memory in women. Behav. Brain Res. 2022, 417, 113592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Leplow, B.; Lehnung, M.; Pohl, J.; Herzog, A.; Ferstl, R.; Mehdorn, M. Navigational place learning in children and young adults as assessed with a standardized locomotor search task. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 299–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Leon, I.; Tascòn, L.; Ortells-Pareja, J.J.; Cimadevilla, J.M. Virtual reality assessment of walking and non-walking space in men and women with virtual reality-based tasks. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0204995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffman, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Serra, L.; Raimondi, S.; di Domenico, C.; Maffei, S.; Lardone, A.; Liparoti, M.; Sorrentino, P.; Caltagirone, C.; Petrosini, L.; Mandolesi, L. The beneficial effects of physical exercise on visuospatial working memory in preadolescent children. AIMS Neurosci. 2021, 8, 496–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Foti, F.; Martone, D.; Orrù, S.; Montuori, S.; Imperlini, E.; Buono, P.; Petrosini, L.; Mandolesi, L. Are young children able to learn exploratory strategies by observation? Psychol. Res. 2018, 82, 1212–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Moraleda, E.; Broglio, C.; Rodríguez, F.; Gómez, A. Development of different spatial frames of reference for orientation in small-scale environments. Psicothema 2013, 25, 468–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mandolesi, L.; Petrosini, L.; Menghini, D.; Addona, F.; Vicari, S. Children’s radial arm maze performance as a function of age and sex. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2009, 27, 789–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Foreman, N.; Gillett, R.; Jones, S. Choice autonomy and memory for spatial locations in six-year-old children. Br. J. Psychol. 1994, 85, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Foreman, N.; Warry, R.; Murray, P. Development of reference and working spatial memory in preschool children. J. Gen. Psychol. 1990, 117, 267–276. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. Foreman, N.P.; Arber, M.; Savage, J. Spatial memory in preschool infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 1984, 17, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Overman, W.H.; Pate, B.J.; Moore, K.; Peuster, A. Ontogeny of place learning in children as measured in the radial arm maze, Morris search task, and open field task. Behav. Neurosci. 1996, 110, 1205–1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Aadland, J.; Beatty, W.W.; Maki, R.H. Spatial memory of children and adults assessed in the radial maze. Dev. Psychobiol. 1985, 18, 163–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. O’Connor, R.C.; Glassman, R.B. Human performance with a seventeen-arm radial maze analog. Brain Res. Bull. 1993, 30, 189–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Glassman, R.B.; Garvey, K.J.; Elkins, K.M.; Kasal, K.L.; Couillard, N.L. Spatial working memory score of humans in a large radial maze, similar to published score of rats, implies capacity close to the magical number 7 ± 22. Brain Res. Bull. 1994, 34, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Glassman, R.B.; Leniek, K.M.; Haegerich, T.M. Human working memory capacity is 7 ± 2 in a radial maze with distracting interruption: Possible implication for neural mechanisms of declarative and implicit long-term memory. Brain Res. Bull. 1998, 47, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Leitner, Y.; Heldman, D.; Harel, S.; Pick, C.G. Deficits in spatial orientation of children with intrauterine growth retardation. Brain Res. Bull. 2005, 67, 13–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Foti, F.; Menghini, D.; Petrosini, L.; Valerio, G.; Crinò, A.; Vicari, S.; Grimaldi, T.; Mandolesi, L. Spatial competences in Prader-Willi syndrome: A Radial Arm Maze study. Behav. Genet. 2011, 41, 445–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Mandolesi, L.; Addona, F.; Foti, F.; Menghini, D.; Petrosini, L.; Vicari, S. Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: A radial arm maze study. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2009, 27, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Foti, F.; Sorrentino, P.; Menghini, D.; Montuori, S.; Pesoli, M.; Turriziani, P.; Vicari, S.; Petrosini, L.; Mandolesi, L. Peripersonal Visuospatial Abilities in Williams Syndrome Analyzed by a Table Radial Arm Maze Task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Bertholet, L.; Escobar, M.T.; Depré, M.; Chavan, C.F.; Giuliani, F.; Gisquet-Verrier, P.; Preissmann, D.; Schenk, F. Spatial radial maze procedures and setups to dissociate local and distal relational spatial frameworks in humans. J. Neurosci. Methods 2015, 253, 126–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Palermo, L.; Foti, F.; Ferlazzo, F.; Guariglia, C.; Petrosini, L. I find my way in a maze but not in my own territory! Navigational processing in developmental topographical disorientation. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bohbot, V.D.; Jech, R.; Ruèicka, E.; Nadel, L.; Kanilna, M.; Stepankova, K.; Bures, J. Rat Spatial Memory Tasks Adapted for Humans: Characterization in Subjects with Intact Brain and Subjects with Medial Temporal Lobe Lesions. Physiol. Res. 2002, 51 (Suppl. S1), S49–S56. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kim, H.; Park, J.Y.; Kim, K.K. Spatial learning and memory using a radial arm maze with a head-mounted display. Psychiatry Investig. 2018, 15, 935–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Ben-Zeev, T.; Weiss, I.; Ashri, S.; Heled, Y.; Ketko, I.; Yanovich, R.; Okun, E. Mild Physical Activity Does Not Improve Spatial Learning in a Virtual Environment. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 584052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Somma, F.; Bartolomeo, P.; Vallone, F.; Argiuolo, A.; Cerrato, A.; Miglino, O.; Mandolesi, L.; Zurlo, M.C.; Gigliotta, O. Further to the left: Stress-induced increase of spatial pseudoneglect during the covid-19 lockdown. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 573846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Taheri Gorji, H.; Leocadi, M.; Grassi, F.; Galati, G. The art gallery maze: A novel tool to assess human navigational abilities. Cogn. Process. 2021, 22, 501–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Rechtman, E.; Curtin, P.; Papazaharias, D.M.; Renzetti, S.; Cagna, G.; Peli, M.; Levin-Schwartz, Y.; Placidi, D.; Smith, D.R.; Lucchini, R.G.; et al. Sex-specific associations between co-exposure to multiple metals and visuospatial learning in early adolescence. Transl. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Sodums, D.J.; Bohbot, V.D. Negative correlation between grey matter in the hippocampus and caudate nucleus in healthy aging. Hippocampus 2020, 30, 892–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Dahmani, L.; Courcot, B.; Near, J.; Patel, R.; Amaral, R.S.C.; Chakravarty, M.M.; Bohbot, V.D. Fimbria-Fornix Volume Is Associated With Spatial Memory and Olfactory Identification in Humans. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2020, 13, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Goodman, J.; McClay, M.; Dunsmoor, J.E. Threat-induced modulation of hippocampal and striatal memory systems during navigation of a virtual environment. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2020, 168, 107160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Yang, Y.; Merrill, E.C.; Wang, Q. Children’s response, landmark, and metric strategies in spatial navigation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 181, 75–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Caplan, J.B.; Legge, E.L.G.; Cheng, B.; Madan, C.R. Effectiveness of the method of loci is only minimally related to factors that should influence imagined navigation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2019, 72, 2541–2553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Aumont, É.; Arguin, M.; Bohbot, V.; West, G.L. Increased flanker task and forward digit span performance in caudate-nucleus-dependent response strategies. Brain Cogn. 2019, 135, 103576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Aumont, É.; Blanchette, C.A.; Bohbot, V.D.; West, G.L. Caudate nucleus-dependent navigation strategies are associated with increased risk-taking and set-shifting behavior. Learn. Mem. 2019, 26, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Raiesdana, S. Modeling the interaction of navigational systems in a reward-based virtual navigation task. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2018, 17, 45–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Dahmani, L.; Patel, R.M.; Yang, Y.; Chakravarty, M.M.; Fellows, L.K.; Bohbot, V.D. An intrinsic association between olfactory identification and spatial memory in humans. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 4162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Aumont, É.; Bohbot, V.D.; West, G.L. Spatial learners display enhanced oculomotor performance. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2018, 30, 872–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Konishi, K.; Joober, R.; Poirier, J.; MacDonald, K.; Chakravarty, M.; Patel, R.; Breitner, J.; Bohbot, V.D. Healthy versus entorhinal cortical atrophy identification in asymptomatic APOE4 carriers at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018, 61, 1493–1507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Bauer, J.A.; Claus Henn, B.; Austin, C.; Zonic, S.; Fedrighi, C.; Cagna, G.; Placidi, D.; White, R.F.; Yang, Q.; Coulle, B.A.; et al. Manganese in teeth and neurobehavior: Sex-specific windows of susceptibility. Environ. Int. 2017, 108, 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Wilkins, L.K.; Girard, T.A.; Herdman, K.A.; Antonovaa, E.; Dawson, G.R.; Dourish, C.T.; Craig, K.J.; Simmons, A.; Wilcock, G.K.; McCulloch, E.; et al. Hippocampal activation and memory in schizophrenia depend on spatial strategy use in a virtual maze. Psychiatry Res.-Neuroimaging 2017, 268, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Cyr, M.; Wang, Z.; Tau, G.Z.; Zhao, G.; Friedl, E.; Stefan, M.; Terranova, K.; Marsh, R. Reward-Based Spatial Learning in Teens With Bulimia Nervosa. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 962–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Migo, E.M.; O’Daly, O.; Mitterschiffthaler, M.; Antonova, E.; Dawson, G.R.; Dourish, C.T.; Craig, K.J.; Simmons, A.; Wilcock, G.K.; McCulloch, E.; et al. Investigating virtual reality navigation in amnestic mild cognitive impairment using fMRI. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2016, 23, 196–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Robaey, P.; McKenzie, S.; Schachar, R.; Boivin, M.; Bohbot, V.D. Stop and look! Evidence for a bias towards virtual navigation response strategies in children with ADHD symptoms. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 298, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Marsh, R.; Tau, G.Z.; Wang, Z.; Huo, Y.; Liu, G.; Hao, X.; Packard, M.J.; Peterson, B.S.; Simpson, B. Reward-based spatial learning in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2015, 172, 383–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Lee, J.Y.; Kho, S.; Yoo, H.B.; Park, S.; Choi, J.S.; Kwon, J.S.; Cha, K.R.; Jung, H.Y. Spatial memory impairments in amnestic mild cognitive impairment in a virtual radial arm maze. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2014, 10, 653–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Pirogovsky, E.; Holden, H.M.; Jenkins, C.; Peavy, G.M.; Salmon, D.P.; Galasko, D.R.; Gilbert, P.E. Temporal sequence learning in healthy aging and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Exp. Aging Res. 2013, 39, 371–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Konishi, K.; Bohbot, V.D. Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2013, 5, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Wilkins, L.K.; Girard, T.A.; Konishi, K.; King, M.; Herdman, K.A.; King, J.; Christensen, B.; Bohbot, V.D. Selective deficit in spatial memory strategies contrast to intact response strategies in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders tested in a virtual navigation task. Hippocampus 2013, 23, 1015–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Konishi, K.; Etchamendy, N.; Roy, S.; Marighetto, A.; Rajah, N.; Bohbot, V.D. Decreased functional magnetic resonance imaging activity in the hippocampus in favor of the caudate nucleus in older adults tested in a virtual navigation task. Hippocampus 2013, 23, 1005–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Andersen, N.E.; Dahmani, L.; Konishi, K.; Bohbot, V.D. Eye tracking, strategies, and sex differences in virtual navigation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2012, 97, 81–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Braun, J.M.; Lucchini, R.; Bellinger, D.C.; Hoffman, E.; Nazzaro, M.; Smith, D.R.; Wright, R.D. Predictors of virtual radial arm maze performance in adolescent Italian children. Neurotoxicology 2012, 33, 1203–1211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Bohbot, V.D.; McKenzie, S.; Konishi, K.; Fouquet, C.; Kurdi, V.; Schachar, R.; Boivin, M.; Robaey, P. Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: Evidence for changes across the life span. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2012, 4, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Spieker, E.A.; Astur, R.S.; West, J.T.; Griego, J.A.; Rowland, L.M. Spatial memory deficits in a virtual reality eight-arm radial maze in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2012, 135, 84–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Schwabe, L.; Bohbot, V.D.; Wolf, O.T. Prenatal stress changes learning strategies in adulthood. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 2136–2143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Etchamendy, N.; Konishi, K.; Pike, G.B.; Marighetto, A.; Bohbot, V.D. Evidence for a virtual human analog of a rodent relational memory task: A study of aging and fMRI in young adults. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 869–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Bohbot, V.D.; Gupta, M.; Banner, H.; Dahmani, L. Caudate nucleus-dependent response strategies in a virtual navigation task are associated with lower basal cortisol and impaired episodic memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011, 96, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Banner, H.; Bhat, V.; Etchamendy, N.; Joober, R.; Bohbot, V.D. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism is associated with reduced functional magnetic resonance imaging activity in the hippocampus and increased use of caudate nucleus-dependent strategies in a human virtual navigation task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011, 33, 968–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Marsh, R.; Hao, X.; Xu, D.; Wang, Z.; Duan, Y.; Liu, J.; Kangarlu, A.; Martinez, D.; Garcia, F.; Tau, G.; et al. A virtual reality-based FMRI study of reward-based spatial learning. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 2912–2921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  94. Goodrich-Hunsaker, N.J.; Hopkins, R.O. Spatial memory deficits in a virtual radial arm maze in amnesic participants with hippocampal damage. Behav. Neurosci. 2010, 124, 405–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Pirogowsky, E.; Goldstein, J.; Peavy, G.; Jacobson, M.W.; Corey-Bloom, J.; Gilbert, P.E. Temporal order memory deficits prior to clinical diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 662–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Rahman, Q.; Koerting, J. Sexual orientation-related differences in allocentric spatial memory tasks. Hippocampus 2008, 18, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Bohbot, V.D.; Lerch, J.; Thorndycraft, B.; Iaria, G.; Zijdenbos, A.P. Gray matter differences correlate with spontaneous strategies in a human virtual navigation task. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 10078–10083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Levy, L.J.; Astur, R.S.; Frick, K.M. Men and women differ in object memory but not performance of a virtual radial maze. Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 119, 853–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Astur, R.S.; Tropp, J.; Sava, S.; Constable, R.T.; Markus, E.J. Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 151, 103–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Astur, R.S.; Taylor, L.B.; Mamelak, A.N.; Philpott, L.; Sutherland, R.J. Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav. Brain Res. 2002, 132, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. O’Keefe, J.; Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  102. Mandolesi, L.; Leggio, M.G.; Graziano, A.; Neri, P.; Petrosini, L. Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: Involvement in procedural and working memory components. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001, 14, 2020–2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Mandolesi, L.; Leggio, M.G.; Spirito, F.; Petrosini, L. Cerebellar contribution to spatial event processing: Do spatial procedures contribute to formation of spatial declarative knowledge? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003, 18, 2618–2626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Jarrard, L.E. On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat. Behav. Neural Biol. 1993, 60, 9–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Farran, E.K.; Purser, H.R.M.; Courbois, Y.; Ballé, M.; Sockeel, P.; Mellier, D.; Blades, M. Route knowledge and configural knowledge in typical and atypical development: A comparison of sparse and rich environments. J. Neurodev. Disord. 2015, 7, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  106. McLaren-Gradinaru, M.; Burles, F.; Dhillon, I.; Retsinas, A.; Umiltà, A.; Hannah, J.; Dolhan, K.; Iaria, G. A Novel Training Program to Improve Human Spatial Orientation: Preliminary Findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Serino, A. Peripersonal space (PPS) as a multisensory interface between the individual and the environment, defining the space of the self. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019, 99, 138–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Krokos, E.; Plaisant, C.; Varshney, A. Virtual memory palaces: Immersion aids recall. Virtual Real. 2019, 23, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Bohbot, V.D.; Copara, M.S.; Gotman, J.; Ekstrom, A.D. Low-frequency theta oscillations in the human hippocampus during real-world and virtual navigation. Nat. Commun. 2017, 8, 14415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Astur, R.S.; St. Germain, S.A.; Baker, E.K.; Calhoun, V.; Pearlson, G.D.; Constable, R.T. fMRI hippocampal activity during a virtual radial arm maze. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 2005, 30, 307–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Thornberry, C.; Cimadevilla, J.M.; Commins, S. Virtual Morris water maze: Opportunities and challenges. Rev. Neurosci. 2021, 32, 887–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.
Brainsci 12 00468 g001
Figure 2. Free-choice (a) and forced-choice (b) paradigms are represented.
Figure 2. Free-choice (a) and forced-choice (b) paradigms are represented.
Brainsci 12 00468 g002
Table 1. Studies carried out using RAM in real environment.
Table 1. Studies carried out using RAM in real environment.
AuthorsSample CharacteristicsRAM ParadigmArms
Serra et al., 2021 [39]Children (healthy)Forced-choice/
Table RAM
8
N = 28;
Age: m = 8.1, SD = 1.1
Foti et al., 2018 [40]Children (healthy)Free-choice8
N = 36;
Age: m = 5.3, SD = 0.2
Moraleda et al., 2013 [41]Children (healthy)Free-choice/
Table RAM
4
N = 48;
Reported age range
from 6 to 10 years
Mandolesi et al., 2009 [42]Children (healthy)Free-choice
Forced-choice
8
N = 90;
Reported age range
from 3 to 8 years
Foreman et al., 1994 [43]Children (healthy)Free-choice/
Forced-choice
12
N = 28;
Reported age: m = 6, SD = 4.5
Foreman et al., 1990 [44]Children (healthy)Forced-choice10
N = 18;
Age: m = 3.3, SD: Not reported
Foreman et al., 1984 [45]Children (healthy)Free-choice8
N = 10;
Reported age range
from 2 to 4 years
Overman et al., 1996 [46]Children/young adults (healthy)Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 43;
Children reported age range
from 20 to 151 months;
Young adults age range
from 17 to 21 years
Aadland et al., 1985 [47]Children/young adults (healthy)Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 146;
Reported age range
from 18 to 71 months
O’Connor & Glassman,
1993 [48]
Young adults (healthy)Free-choice
(drawn on
paper)
17
N = 15;
Age: Not reported
Glassman et al., 1994 [49]Young adults (healthy)Free-choice17/13
N = 57;
Age: Not reported
Glassman et al., 1998 [50]Young adults/adults (healthy)Free-choice8
N = 21;
Reported age range
from 18 to 35 years
Leitner et al., 2005 [51]Children
(Intrauterine growth retardation)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 28;
Reported age: 6 years,
SD: Not reported
Foti et al., 2011 [52]Children/adolescents
(Prader-Willi syndrome and
Williams syndrome)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 24;
PWS Mental age: m = 6.0, SD = 0.5;
WS Mental age: m = 6.0, SD = 0.3
Mandolesi et al., 2009 [53]Children/Adolescents
(Williams syndrome)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 14;
Mental age: m = 6.2, SD = 1.4
Foti et al., 2020 [54]Adolescents
(Williams syndrome)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 15;
Mental age: m = 18.1, SD = 5.2
Bertholet et al., 2015 [55]Young adults (Intellectual
disability and healthy)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 107;
Age: m = 22.8, SD = 0.7
Palermo et al., 2014 [56]Adults
(Developmental topographical
disorientation)
Free-choice/
Forced-choice
8
N = 2; patient 1: reported age 29 years;
patient 2: reported age 38 years
Bohbot et al., 2002 [57]Adults
(Temporal lesions and healthy)
Free-choice8
N = 52;
Age: m = 38, SD = 1.3
Abbreviations: m, mean; SD, standard deviation; RAM, Radial Arm Maze; PWS, Prader-Willi Syndrome; WS, Williams Syndrome.
Table 2. Studies carried out using RAM in virtual environment.
Table 2. Studies carried out using RAM in virtual environment.
AuthorsSample CharacteristicsVirtual ModalityRAM ParadigmArms
Kim et al., 2018 [58]Young adults (healthy)ImmersiveForced-choice8
N = 80;
Age: m = 23.2 SD = 2.4
Ben-Zeev et al., 2020 [59]Young adults (healthy)ImmersiveFree-choice8
N = 40;
Age: m = 25.5, SD = 1.6,
Patel et al., 2021 [35]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 86;
Age: m = 19.0, SD = 1.0
Somma et al., 2021 [60]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 47;
Age: m = 20, SD = 1.0
Taheri Gorji et al., 2021 [61]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice6
N = 42;
Age: m = 24.4, SD = 2.4
Rechtman et al., 2020 [62]Children
(exposed to manganese)
Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 188;
Age: m = 12.01, SD = 0.9
Sodums & Bohbot, 2020 [63]Elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice12
N = 39;
Age: m = 64.6, SD = 4.1
Dahmani et al., 2020 [64]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 55;
Age: m = 22.9, SD = 3.5
Goodman et al., 2020 [65]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 62;
Age: m = 23, SD = 6.3
Yang et al., 2019 [66]Children/young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveFree-choice6
N = 83;
Children: reported age range from 6 to 10 years;
Young adults: reported age range from 18 to 22 years
Caplan et al., 2019 [67]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 173;
Age: m = 19.5, SD = 2.5
Aumont et al., 2019 [68]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 50;
Age: m = 23.4, SD = 4.1
Aumont et al., 2019 [69]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 53;
Age: m = 23.9, SD = 4.4
Raiesdana, 2018 [70]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 8;
Age: m = 22.1, SD = 2.3
Dahmani et al., 2018 [71]Young adults/adult (healthy/focal lesion to the frontal lobe)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 78;
Young adults:
Age: m = 22.9, SD = 3.5;
Patients:
group 1:
Age m = 56.6, SD = 16.2;
group 2:
Age m = 60, SD = 6.7
Aumont et al., 2018 [72]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 50;
Age: m = 23.4, SD = 4.1
Konishi et al., 2018 [73]Elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice12
N = 66;
Age: m = 66.1, SD = 4.5
Bauer et al., 2017 [74]Children (exposed to manganese)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 142;
Age: m = 12.4, SD = 0.9
Wilkins et al., 2017 [75]Adults
(Schizophrenia)
Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 16;
Age: m = 44.4, SD = 6.1;
Cyr et al., 2016 [76]Adolescents
(Bulimia nervosa)
Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 27;
Age: m = 16.6 SD = 1.5
Migo et al., 2016 [77]Elderly people
(Mild cognitive impairment)
Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 8;
Age: m = 69.6, SD = 5.8
Robaey et al., 2016 [78]Children (ADHD)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 223;
Age: m = 8.4, SD = 0.1
Marsh et al., 2015 [79]Adults
(Obsessive compulsive disorder)
Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 33;
Age: m = 29.4, SD = 8.1
Lee et al., 2014 [80]Elderly people
(Alzheimer’s Disease,
Mild cognitive impairment)
Non-immersiveForced-choice6
N = 40;
AD Age: m = 72.4, SD = 5.6; MCI: Age m = 70.7, SD = 5
Pirogovsky et al., 2013 [81]Elderly people
(Mild Cognitiv Impairment)
Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 10;
Age: m = 76.8, SD = 2.3
Konishi & Bohbot, 2013 [82]Elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 45;
Age: m = 64.3, SD = 4
Wilkins et al., 2013 [83]Adults (Schizophrenia)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 17;
Age: m = 44.9, SD = 4.8;
Age: m = 39.6, SD = 9.8
Konishi et al., 2013 [84]Young adults/elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice12
N = 52;
Young adults:
Age: m = 23.8, SD = 3.8;
Elderly people:
Age: m = 64.2, SD = 4.7
Andersen et al., 2012 [85]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 7;
Age: m = 28.1, SD = 5.6
Braun et al., 2012 [86]Children (exposed to manganese)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 255;
Age: m = 13, SD = 0.9
Bohbot et al., 2012 [87]Children/young adults/elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 599;
Children:
Age: m = 8.4, SD = 0.1;
Young adults:
Age: m = 25.6, SD = 4.6; Elderly people:
Age: m = 65.6, SD = 5.6
Spieker et al., 2012 [88]Adults (Schizophrenia)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 33;
Age: m = 40.0, SD = 11.9
Schwabe et al., 2012 [89]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 60;
Age: m = 24.4, SD = 0.4
Etchamendy et al., 2012 [90]Young adults/elderly people (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice12
N = 55;
Young adults:
Age: m = 25.1, SD = 4.1;
Elderly people:
Age: m = 66.9, SD = 7.9
Bohbot et al., 2011 [91]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 66;
Age: m = 21.6, SD = 0.81;
Banner et al., 2011 [92]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 106;
Age: m = 23.4, SD = 1.1
Marsh et al., 2010 [93]Adults (healthy)Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 25;
Age: m = 32.5, SD = 7.6
Goodrich-Hunsaker & Hopkins, 2010 [94]Adults
(Hippocampal damage)
Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 5;
Age: m = 45.6, SD = 9.4
Pirogovsky et al., 2009 [95]Adults
(Huntington’s disease)
Non-immersiveFree-choice8
N = 18;
Age: m = 46.4, SD = 2.1
Rahman & Koerting, 2008 [96]Young adults/adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 140; Reported age range
from 19 to 45 years
Bohbot et al., 2007 [97]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 30;
Age: m = 27.9, SD = 4.1
Levy et al., 2005 [98]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice12
N = 55; Reported age range
from 18 to 30 years
Astur et al., 2004 [99]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 13; Reported age range
from 18 to 30 years
Astur et al., 2002 [100]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 61;
Age: m = 19.4, SD = 4.8
Bohbot et al.,2004 [33]Adults (medial temporal lobe resections)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 15;
Age: m = 42.5, SD = 8.7
Iaria et al., 2003 [34]Young adults (healthy)Non-immersiveForced-choice8
N = 50;
Age: m = 27.7, SD = 4.7
Abbreviations: m, mean; SD, standard deviation; RAM, Radial Arm Maze; MCI, Mild Cognitive Impairment; AD, Alzheimer’s Disease.
Table 3. Illustration of the main parameters used to analyze the performances in RAM task.
Table 3. Illustration of the main parameters used to analyze the performances in RAM task.
Free-Choice RAM VersionForced-Choice RAM Version
(Referred to the Second Phase of the Task)
Total time to complete the entire task
Time to reach each reward
Total time to complete the second phase of the task
Latency to select the first armLatency to select the first arm
Total entries (arms correct and incorrect visited)Total entries (arms correct and incorrect visited)
Distance travelledDistance travelled
Movement speedErrors
Frequency of successes/Percentage of correct visits/Search efficiencyAcross-phase errors
Errors/Error-free trialsWithin-phase errors
The longest sequence of correctly visited armsThe longest sequence of correctly visited arms
Percentage of angles turned (45°, 90°, 135°, 180° or 360°)/Angle change/Strategy fixation
Perseverations (consecutive entries into the same arm or the re-entries into a fixed sequence of arms)
Declarative mastery
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Palombi, T.; Mandolesi, L.; Alivernini, F.; Chirico, A.; Lucidi, F. Application of Real and Virtual Radial Arm Maze Task in Human. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040468

AMA Style

Palombi T, Mandolesi L, Alivernini F, Chirico A, Lucidi F. Application of Real and Virtual Radial Arm Maze Task in Human. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(4):468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040468

Chicago/Turabian Style

Palombi, Tommaso, Laura Mandolesi, Fabio Alivernini, Andrea Chirico, and Fabio Lucidi. 2022. "Application of Real and Virtual Radial Arm Maze Task in Human" Brain Sciences 12, no. 4: 468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12040468

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop