Next Article in Journal
Numerical Simulation on Backfilling of Buried Pipes Using Controlled Low Strength Materials
Next Article in Special Issue
Linking the Sensory Taste Properties of Chocolate-Based Biscuits to Consumers’ Emotions: A Cross-Cultural Study
Previous Article in Journal
Steady State and Transient Vibration Analysis of an Exponentially Graded Rotor Bearing System Having a Slant Crack
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effect of Scents on Gazing Behavior and Choice

1
Institute of Agribusiness, Hungarian University of Agriculture and Life Sciences, Villányi út 29-31, H-1118 Budapest, Hungary
2
Institute of Food Science and Technology, Hungarian University of Agriculture and Life Sciences, Villányi út 29-31, H-1118 Budapest, Hungary
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(14), 6899; https://doi.org/10.3390/app12146899
Submission received: 10 June 2022 / Revised: 30 June 2022 / Accepted: 5 July 2022 / Published: 7 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sensory Characteristics and Consumers Acceptance of Food Products)

Abstract

:
Environmental stimuli can have a significant impact on our decisions. Elements of the store atmosphere, such as music, lights and smells, all have effects on choices, but these have been only vaguely investigated. In the present study, we aim to uncover the effect of strawberry scent on the gazing behavior and choices of the 62 recruited participants. A static eye-tracker was used to study the effect of scent, released by a diffuser. In total, 31 participants completed the study under odorless conditions, while another 31 participants had strawberry fragrance sprayed into the air. The objectives of the study were (1) to determine whether the most gazed-upon product in each of the four categories (chocolate, tea, muesli bar, yoghurt) was chosen, (2) whether the presence of the strawberry scent influenced consumer decision making, i.e., whether the strawberry scent influenced more people to choose strawberry-flavored products, and (3) to introduce the application of a fast and easy-to-use technique for the qualitative analysis of strawberry aroma present in the air during eye-tracking measurements. The results show that (1) participants chose the product they had studied the longest, for all four categories, and (2) the presence or absence of the scent had no significant effect on choice, with the same frequencies of choosing each product in the two conditions regardless of the flavor of the products.

1. Introduction

Even though consumers’ decisions are routine, the stimuli they encounter in the shopping environment can change their initial goals and preferences. People want to get through their daily shopping as quickly as possible, especially at the beginning and end of the shopping day. A good measure of consumers’ visual attention is achieved using eye-tracking. It can be observed, for example, whether shoppers look specifically at the products they have planned to buy or also at other product categories [1].
Producers want customers to choose their products. In many cases, customers choose a product not because of what is inside, but because of the way it is packaged or displayed on the shelf, or in advertisements on the Internet or on TV [2,3]. Neuromarketing experts are trying to identify these influencing factors, for which there are already many methods and tools [4]. Several tools are available for producers to drive consumer attention, e.g., packaging, ads, lights, sounds and scents. The use of smells have been identified as a promising area, as olfactory cues have been shown to have a significant effect on our everyday life [5].

1.1. Environmental Scents

As previous studies have shown, the sense of smell plays an important role in the perception of taste and the acceptance of food [3,4]. It is important to note that there are individual differences in odor sensitivity. Previous research has revealed that factors such as age [6,7], hunger/thirst [8], personality traits [9], as well as environmental factors [10] contribute to differences in odor sensitivity. Environmental scents also influence consumer behavior in a number of ways [11]. A pleasant smell enhances the evaluation of the product and the retailer, causing a change in purchasing behavior (longer stay, better mood, memories and purchase) [12]. It also affects body states and decision making. Gagarina and Pikturniené [13] identified the relationship between environmental odors and decision-making heuristics when it comes to risk. They manipulated the type (vanilla vs. peppermint) and intensity of ambient fragrance concentrates sprayed in the room. The risk was perceived as significantly lower in the high-intensity scent condition, compared to unscented or lightly scented environments. The results are consistent with the reported properties of peppermint scent, as it increases alertness, captures attention and speeds up physiological processes.
Managers use environmental scents as an important strategic element in different service environments, especially food-related scents. Biswas and Szocs investigated the impact of food-related environmental scents on food purchase/choice of children and adults. The results show that prolonged (more than two minutes) exposure to an environmental scent associated with enjoyable foods (e.g., cookie scent) leads to a lower purchase of unhealthy foods than no environmental scent or an environmental scent associated with non-enjoyable foods (e.g., strawberry scent). The effects appear to be driven by cross-modal sensory compensation, whereby prolonged exposure to a pleasurable/rewarding food odor causes pleasure in the reward circuit, which in turn reduces the desire to consume the pleasurable food [14]. Sensory marketing, which is defined as “marketing that engages the consumers’ senses and affects their perception, judgment and behavior” [15], can be used to create subliminal triggers that characterize consumers’ perceptions of abstract concepts. Among the elements of the shop atmosphere, there has been research on music [16,17,18], smells [19,20,21] and color [22,23].

1.2. Gazing Behavior

Eye tracking has long been a widely applied technique to study visual attention in individuals, with the most common method being pupil-centered corneal reflectance (PCCR). Eye-trackers track all of the eye movements of a participant, i.e., where the person looks and for how long, where they looked first and how much time they spend looking at that particular product. There have been many studies on the “love at first sight” choice. Some have argued that choice can be predicted by which product the subject fixated on first, but Danner and colleagues have refuted this claim [24]. With an eye-tracker, we can measure the followings: gaze direction and gaze point, detection of eye presence, eye position, eye identification, eyelid closure and pupil dilation and size [25].
In their 2013 paper, Orquin and Mueller Loose [26] reviewed studies on eye movements in decision making and compare their observations with theoretical predictions about the role of attention in decision making. An important conclusion of this review is that attention plays an active role in decision making. In contrast to the assumption of passive information acquisition, attention has been shown to be driven not only by information demands, but also by bottom-up processes and interactions with working memory. The final decision is shaped by complex interactions between stimuli, attentional processes, working memory and preferences, and it is, therefore, reasonable to conclude that attention plays a constructive role in decision-making. It can also be inferred that the cognitive processes that drive eye movements during decision making are not consistently different from those in similar tasks. Attentional processes, such as top-down or bottom-up attention capture or learning effects on fixations, play similarly important roles in decision making, problem solving, visual search and scene viewing. Rojas and colleagues focused their research on using eye-tracking as a method to understand children’s consumer preferences. The results support the use of visual attention measures as an implicit tool to analyze children’s decision making and preferences. Additionally, their results also revealed that stimuli determine the largest amount of fixations and that viewing times are different across genders [27].
When working with advertisements, it has been shown that “sniffing a scent while viewing a print ad diverts visual attention (total gaze duration) to a visual object in the ad when the object is semantically congruent with the scent, controlling for total time spent looking at the ad” [20]. This effect was more pronounced in direct scenarios (e.g., lemon scent and lemon picture) compared to indirect ones (citrus scent and cleanser).
Several studies have demonstrated that visual cues affect olfactory performance [28,29,30]. For example, when an odor is correctly paired with a color, participants perceive it as more pleasant than when they are incorrectly paired [31]. It was also shown that looking at congruent pictures increased the intensity of odors and made them more pleasant than incongruent pairing [32]. In addition, several studies have investigated the ability of odors to modulate the behavior of participants in response to visual stimuli [33,34,35,36].
Morquecho-Campos and colleagues [37] investigated how subliminal exposure to odors influences the choice of snacks with similar taste characteristics and whether this is modulated by visual attention. The test used odors that were associated with salty/savory or sweet food. Their results showed that the participants fixated first on the product that could be paired with the particular odor: if they smelled salty/savory, most people fixated on salty/savory snacks first, and vice versa for sweet snacks. However, they fixated on sweet snacks more often and for longer periods of time, and chose them more often, regardless of which odorant was released. Participants spent more time looking at incongruent snack products during the first fixation. These results suggest that scent affects visual attention but does not influence final choice.

1.3. Odor Perception

The olfactory sensory receptors are located in the posterior-anterior part of the nasal cavity, embedded in the olfactory bulb. The projection from the olfactory bulb runs directly into the olfactory cortex. It is probably in this part of the cortex that olfactory sensations are conscious. From here, the information is transmitted to the orbitofrontal cortex and directly to parts of the limbic system, one of which is responsible for the affective effects of olfactory sensations and is involved in behavioral responses. This very direct connection suggests that the “decoding” of the stimulation of primary sensory neurons takes place in the olfactory bulb itself [38]. It has been shown that odorants can affect behavioral performance through their stimulatory or sedative properties on attention and arousal [39] and can modify mood and emotional states, and thereby, affect information processing [40].
In recent decades, there has been increasing interest in cross-modal integration, focusing on the links between hearing and vision. The cross-modal interaction between olfaction and vision is observed in two opposing ways: the effect of visual inputs on odor processing and the effect of odors on visual perception [41]. There are studies suggesting that the perception of odors can be strongly influenced by vision. In such studies, participants are most often required to perform one or more olfactory tasks (e.g., hedonic appraisal or identification) while exposed to a visual stimulus [42].
In Gottfried and Dolan’s study, participants were shown a picture while being sprayed with a fragrance. The associative relationship between odor and image was either congruent (image and odor are paired) or incongruent (image and odor are not paired). Odor perception was easier for congruent trials than for incongruent trials. In comparison, congruent and incongruent pairs differentially activated the anterior hippocampus, which is thought to be involved in relational processing between multiple sensory sources and in mediating the activation of cross-modal semantic representations [43].

1.4. Measurement of Aromas

Strawberry aroma is not a distinct, specific aroma, rather a collection of different compounds, each perceived as strawberry but in a different way [44]. Interestingly, over 360 volatile compounds have already been identified in strawberries [45]. A complex mixture of esters, aldehydes, ketones, alcohols, terpenes, furanones and sulfur compounds determines the perception of strawberry aroma. Even though many of the components are present in low concentrations, they have a significant influence on strawberry aroma. Esters have been identified as the most prevalent class of these compounds, both quantitatively and qualitatively, with over 131 esters identified in strawberry aroma [46]. For example, Fan and colleagues’ [47] study on esters, especially methyl and ethyl esters, accounts for 25–90% of the total amount of volatiles in strawberry and contribute fruity aromas. Out of esters, there are several volatile compounds with different concentrations that impact the development of the strawberry aroma, containing a variety of aromatic notes, such as caramel, fruity, sour, flour, buttery and grassy [48].
Several different techniques have been used to measure volatiles in foods. Starowitz [49] stated that the most commonly used techniques for fraction separation are solid-phase microextractions (SPME) and stir bar sorptive extractions (SBSE), while the analysis of active compounds is usually conducted using gas chromatography with olfactometry detectors (GC-O) or solvent-assisted flavor evaporation (SAFE). Volatiles are mostly separated on GC systems, while detection is done by mass spectrometry for chemical compound identification. However, HS-SPME in conjunction with GC-MS has been proven to be one of the most helpful tools in detecting volatile components of various food aromas [50].

1.5. Hypotheses

Based on the above, the hypotheses of the present study are:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
In each of the four categories (chocolate, tea, muesli bars, yoghurt), participants choose the product they have looked at most often.
Hypothesis 1 (H2).
Strawberry scent influences consumer decision making.
Hypothesis 1 (H3).
Rapid GC-MS analysis of essential oils in the air supports eye-tracking studies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Location

The measurement was carried out at the Buda Campus of the Hungarian University of Agricultural and Life Sciences, in an approximately 18 m2 room located in a quiet, but central part of the campus. The room was 6 m long and 3 m wide, with 2.60 m inner height. This was a positive factor, as it was easier to recruit participants for the study, yet it could be carried out without disturbance. The table with the computer was placed in the middle of the room. The light in the room was provided by an LED panel (6500 K, 1600 lm) mounted on the ceiling above the table. Since the research was conducted to investigate the influence of strawberry scent on decision making, a pleasant strawberry scent was sprayed into the air using MAYAM elements essential oil and a Sencor SHF 920BL (Ricany, Czech Republic) humidifier. The size of the room allowed for a uniform distribution of the odor and easy control of its intensity. The level of strawberry fragrance was continuous and adjusted to be pleasant, yet not overpowering. Details of the preparation of the aroma solution are presented in Section 2.6.2.

2.2. Participants

The subjects of the study were recruited on the Buda Campus. The total number of volunteering participants was 70, mean age of the odorless group is 22.85 years (SD = 6.55) and the odor group is 22.8 years (SD = 2.97). Eight participants were excluded due to low (<80%) gaze sampling. Detailed demographic information of the participants is given in Table 1.
Among the visual disorders, farsightedness and nearsightedness were the most common, and three participants reported squinting. At the end of the questionnaire, six participants reported that they had experienced a partial loss of smell, and two participants had a partial loss of taste, due to post-COVID symptoms. These post-COVID symptoms have passed completely according to the participants, and therefore, they did not affect the measurement.

2.3. Eye-Tracker and Software

Information on the eye-tracking procedure has been added following the guidelines published in [51]. During the study, eye movements were tracked with a Tobii Pro X2-60 (Tobii Pro AB, Danderyd, Sweden) desktop eye-tracker and the timelines (image sequences) were presented using the Tobii Pro Lab v.1.171 (Tobii Pro AB, Danderyd, Sweden) software. The eye-tracker illuminates the eye with a near-infrared pattern, and then captures high-resolution images of it. The image processing algorithms search for characteristic details and reflection patterns in the user’s eyes and use these to calculate the gaze point and position of the eyes using a 3D eye model algorithm. An advantage of this type of eye-tracker is that it is small, unobtrusive, does not disturb the participant during the measurement and allows a certain freedom of movement of the head. The ideal viewing angle is ≤65° and the appropriate distance between the eye and the camera is 60–65 cm.

2.4. Process

The measurement was carried out with two groups. First, it was done with the control (odorless) group of 31 participants, whose eye movements and food choices were tested in an odor-free environment. This was followed by the odor group of 31 participants, where strawberry scent was sprayed into the air. The measurement procedure was the same for both groups, except that the odorless control group did not smell strawberries in the air.
First, participants were asked to sit in a comfortable position in front of the computer, then they were asked not to change their position during the measurement and were informed about the measurement procedure and the eye-tracker. The next step was to calibrate the eye-tracker, and after successful calibration, we started the timelines installed in the software. The measurement started with an initial slide containing the instructions. After reading the instructions, participants proceeded by clicking with the left mouse button. At the beginning of the timelines, trial slides (four lemonades made from different fruits: forest fruit, lemon, orange and peach) were used for practice and to familiarize the participants with the process. Participants were asked to select the product they liked the most from the four alternatives and then to press any key on the keyboard. After that, the mouse (which was hidden up until now) cursor appeared on the screen. They were then asked to click on the chosen product and press another key to move on to the next choice set. The two conditions (odorless and odor) saw identical timelines, and the decision making was not time-bound.

2.5. Visual Stimuli

Participants were shown five different sets of images, out of which the first were the trial slides. The trial slides were not included in the data analysis. The remaining four choice sets were identical in structure, but the order of the product groups and the order of the products was randomized. The image sequences covered four product groups—muesli bar, chocolate bar, tea and yoghurt—and there were always four product alternatives to choose from (Figure 1). The visual stimuli consisted of product alternatives from the same brand, with only one strawberry-flavored product. Each variation was preceded by a slide with a fixation cross presented in the middle of the screen for 2 s, so that the gaze always started from the center. The visual stimuli were presented on an LG W2452V-PF 24” Full HD LCD monitor at 1366 × 768 resolution. Areas of interests were defined as the presented products. The distance between AOIs were maximized to avoid any overlap between them. Identification by velocity threshold (I-VT) filter method was used that incorporated interpolation across gaps (75 ms), reduced noise (median), used velocity threshold at 30°/s and merged adjacent fixations (<0.5°) between fixations (<75 ms) and discarded short fixations (<60 ms).

2.6. Headspace Analysis of Essential Oil

2.6.1. Essential Oil

In total, 200 μL essential oil (MAYAM, Elemental, Oradea, Romania) was pipetted into a 20 mL headspace (HS) vial. After 30 min of equilibrium time, solid-phase microextraction (SPME) was inserted into the HS vials. The sampling time was 1 min at room temperature.

2.6.2. Sensory Testing Room

An essential oil solution was prepared as follows: 1.5 mL essential oil was added to 3.5 mL distilled water to achieve 20-fold dilution. An ultrasonic humidification device (Sencor SHF 920BL, Ricany, Czech Republic) was used to release the aroma into the air. SPME sampling was done 30 min, 60 min and 90 min after the device was switched on. Sampling time was 30 min at room temperature.

2.6.3. Rapid GC-MS Analysis

Determination of aroma compounds was performed on an Agilent 6890N gas chromatograph (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) coupled to an Agilent 5973 Mass Spectrometer (Agilent Technologies, USA).
After extraction, aroma compounds were thermally desorbed into the injector heated at 250 °C and equipped with an SPME liner (0.75 i.d., Supelco Inc., Bellefonte, PA, USA).
Separation was done on an HP-5MS capillary column (10 m × 0.1 mm i.d. × 0.4 μm df). Hydrogen was used as carrier gas under constant flow at 1 mL min−1.
An earlier published rapid GC temperature program [52] was used with slight modifications: 45 °C (2 min), 80 °C (30 °C min−1), 100 °C (60 °C min−1), 150 °C (30 °C min−1), 250 °C (80 °C min−1) and 250 °C (1 min). The temperature of the GC oven was kept constant at 250 °C. The total run time was 7.94 min.
Mass spectra in the 35 to 350 m/z range were recorded. The chromatography and spectral data were evaluated using Agilent Mass Hunter Software (Agilent Technologies, USA). Volatile compounds were identified with the NIST17 database (NIST, Gaithersburg, MD, USA) and by retention indices. Hydrocarbon standards (C8 to C20 in hexane, Sigma-Aldrich, Darmstadt, Germany) were injected to determine the retention indices using a modified Kováts method [52]. Every sample was analyzed in triplicate.

2.7. Data Analysis

Choice frequencies were tested using Chi-squared statistics. The effects of choice and scent on the gazing behavior were tested using repeated measures analysis of variance (RMANOVA), where the eye-tracking variables served as within-subject factors, while choice and scent served as between-subject factors. The following eye-tracking parameters were used during the data analysis:
-
Time To First Fixation (TTFF, time elapsed between the appearance of a stimuli and the user fixating their gaze first on an alternative in seconds);
-
First Fixation Duration (FFD, length of the first fixation on an alternative, in seconds);
-
Fixation Duration (FD, total length of fixations on an alternative, in seconds);
-
Fixation Count (FC, number of fixations on an alternative, count);
-
Dwell Duration (DD, time elapsed between the user’s first fixation on a product and the next fixation outside the product, in seconds);
-
Dwell Count (DC, number of “visits” on an alternative, count).
Data were recorded using Tobii Pro Lab v.1.171 (Tobii Pro AB, Danderyd, Sweden). Statistical data analysis was done using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 16.0. (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Choice Frequency

Each product was chosen at least eight times, and statistically significant differences in the choice frequency were found for two out of four product sets (Figure 2). In the case of chocolate, strawberry and raspberry alternatives received ~14% of choices, while peanut was chosen over 40% of the time. For teas, there was a significant difference between the choice frequency of the strawberry- and cherry-flavored alternatives, 36.7% and 15.9%, respectively.

3.2. Eye-Tracking Measures

The results of the Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (RMANOVA) showed that for the tea product group, the product had a significant effect on gazing behavior (Table 2). Product choice showed no significant effect in any of the four categories, while odor also showed no significant effect for any of the product categories. A significant interaction between product and choice was found for all four product categories, indicating that the product chosen was gazed at differently than those not chosen.

3.3. Product Effects on Gazing Behavior

Analyzing the significant effects in more detail, univariate tests show that the product had a significant effect on FC in the muesli bar category and DC in the chocolate category (Table 3). Between product and choice, TTFF and FFD showed no significant effect, supporting appropriate randomization. Significant interactions were found for FD, FC and DD for all four product categories, and for DC for chocolate, yoghurt and muesli bars. There was also no significant interaction between product and odor for TTFF, but there was a significant interaction for FFD for the yoghurt, muesli bar and tea product categories. For the DC indicator, significant results were obtained only for the yoghurt product category.
RMANOVA gives similar results in all four product categories, so we will only present them through the example of the yoghurt product group. The RMANOVA results of the other three product groups can be found in the supplementary materials (Figures S1–S3). Figure 3 shows whether participants chose the product that received the most visual attention. The figure shows that for all four products, participants chose the product that they spent the longest time looking at.
Yoghurt, tea and muesli bars have a faint smell compared to chocolate. When we see a product photo of a strawberry yoghurt, the smell of strawberries comes to mind. Figure 4 clearly shows that the strawberry scent significantly increased (p < 0.05) the FFD for the strawberry- and mango-flavored yoghurts.
The length of the first fixation did not influence other eye-tracker parameters, so no higher values were recorded for FD, FC, DD and DC when the strawberry odor was present. However, the choice frequency of products was partially influenced by the odor in the case of chocolates (χ2 = (3,6) = 13.675, p < 0.05) and yoghurts (χ2 = (3,6) = 110,077,703, p < 0.001). For chocolate products, strawberry-, raspberry- and Oreo-flavored alternatives received higher choice frequencies when the odor was present, while the peanut-flavored alternative was chosen less frequently. For yoghurts, participants chose strawberry- and blueberry-flavored alternatives with a higher frequency under the odor condition. Tea and muesli products showed no significant differences between the odor and odorless conditions (Figure 5).

3.4. Monitoring the Volatiles in the Air

Different aroma products might cause different sensory perceptions depending on the origin, producer, etc. of the aroma product. Not only the used aromas, but also their concentrations can affect the perceived strawberry odor in the air. In order to characterize and map the essential oil compounds in the air, the aroma profile of the commercially available essential oil was analyzed with GC-MS (Figure 6).
Thirteen aroma compounds were identified using Agilent NIST 2017 Mass Spectral Library (W9N08 and W10N11). The identification match factor was above 90% in all cases, and the calculated Kováts’ retention indexes (RI’s) also matched the RI data from NIST WebBook (https://webbook.nist.gov/, accessed on 10 June 2022). Table 4 shows the aroma profile of strawberry aroma and their identification results.
Examining the air composition of the sensory testing room, three compounds were clearly coming from the vaporized essential oil (Table 4). Methyl 2-methylbutyrate, ethyl butyrate and ethyl 2-methylbutyrate also showed up in the case of the total ion chromatogram (TIC) of testing room measures.

4. Discussion

It was shown earlier that although odors play a role in human behavior, their effect might be suppressed by other factors, such as music [53]. Ba and colleagues investigated the combined effects of sound and odor in order to study the behavior of crowds with respect to such effects, thereby improving the use of urban spaces. The study was a covert behavioral observation experiment, using the smell of bakery products as the odor source, while urban noise, music and fan noise were applied. There was also a control experiment where no sounds or smells were used. Their results showed that playing music in the absence of odor attracted the crowd, making the crowd’s path more concentrated. Mass duration was significantly increased by the presence of odor, but the interaction between sound and odor was not significant [54]. Our results support these findings, as the effect of strawberry odor was not the only factor affecting consumer decisions.
The results showed that the tea product group had a significant effect on gazing behavior. For any of the product categories, product choice and odor showed no significant effects. A significant interaction between product and choice was found for all four product categories, indicating that the product chosen was gazed at longer than those not chosen. These results support the findings of Danner and colleagues [24]. However, a significant interaction between product and odor was only found for yoghurt products, but not in the chocolate, muesli bar or tea product categories.
The univariate tests showed that the product had a significant effect on FC in the muesli bar category and DC in the chocolate category. TTFF and FFD showed no significant effects between product and choice. For TTFF, there was no significant interaction between product and odor, while for FFD, there was a significant interaction for the yoghurt, muesli bar and tea product categories. These results are in accordance with earlier findings, where the authors demonstrate that scents can increase visual attention towards congruent visual stimuli [6]. Our results show that for all four product categories, participants chose the product on which they gazed for the longest time. These results support the notion that the product chosen receives more visual attention, as expressed by the FD, FC, DD and DC indicators, but not the TTFF and FFD indicators. These results support H1, as participants chose the product that they had looked at the most often.
According to previous studies, the length of the first fixation may be influenced by the presence of odor [6,35]. In the present study, this claim holds for the yoghurt, muesli bar and tea product categories, but not for the chocolates (Figures S4–S6). A possible reason is that chocolate has a distinctive, strong smell, which overpowers the smell of strawberries. Therefore, when we see a picture of a strawberry chocolate, it is the smell of chocolate that first comes to mind. Our results also show that strawberry odor significantly (p < 0.05) increased the FFD for strawberry- and mango-flavored yoghurts. The mango-flavored yoghurt has received a relatively high attention due to its specificity. The obtained results, therefore, partially support H2, as strawberry scent influenced consumer decision making only in the case of strawberry- and mango-flavored yoghurts.
In the present study, a commercial strawberry aroma product was used and methyl 2-methylbutyrate, ethyl butyrate and ethyl 2-methylbutyrate were identified as its main components. Methyl 2-methylbutyrate has a sweet, fruity and green apple-like odor, while ethyl butyrate is described as fruity, sweet, pineapple, ester-like, strawberry, cheese, fruity and sweet and ethyl 2-methylbutyrate is described as sour, cheesy, sweaty and fruity [55,56]. However, other studies identified different compounds responsible for strawberry odors. For example, uraneol [2,5-dimethyl-4-hydroxy3(2H)-furanone] (DHF) and mesifurane [2,5-dimethyl-4-methoxy-3(2H)-furanone] (DMF) have been identified as the two major contributors to strawberry aroma due to large amounts of them being present in several cultivars [57]. In addition, 2,5-dimethyl-4-hydroxy3(2H)-furanone (Furaneol, DHF) is also considered as one of the essential components of strawberry aroma. It is widely used by food scientists as a synthetic aroma [58].
According to Kim and co-workers [55], four esters can be considered as the key constituents of fresh strawberry, which are ethyl butyrate, ethyl hexanoate, ethyl isovalerate and ethyl 2-methylbutytare. All of these compounds were found in our strawberry aroma (which contains esters dissolved in alcohol). Moreover, ethyl butyrate and ethyl 2-methylbutytare were also identified in the air of the testing room. Our results support H3, as a rapid GC-MS analysis of essential oils in the air supports eye-tracking studies.
The results suggest that the presence of strawberry odor has no significant effect on choice. However, participants did not fixate faster on the strawberry-flavored product, but their first fixation time was found to be longer, similarly to previous results [37].
We should note that the study has some limitations regarding the participants. It is well known that olfactory functions decline with ageing [6]. Over time, people become less sensitive to odors such as personal fragrances [59]. This encourages older people to use more fragrance than before. In contrast, people who are sensitive to fragrances or young people may be uncomfortable with large amounts of fragrance [60]. Due to these documented differences in olfactory sensitivity, the presented results should not be generalized. Additionally, reaction to odors might change over the time of the year. For example, cinnamon odor was shown to be more familiar and pleasant during the Christmas season compared to summertime [61].

5. Conclusions

The results of the study highlight that the smell of strawberries had no significant effect on choice, but it did affect the location of first fixations. However, the strawberry scent can also be triggered by several other volatile components, so taking the SPME measurement into account, we can only conclude that the presence of methyl 2-methylbutyrate, ethyl butyrate and ethyl 2-methylbutyrate did not influence choice. The obtained results, therefore, should not be generalized. Comprehensive analysis of other strawberry aroma compounds is needed to determine if there is any effect on the eye-movements that is specific to the type of the aroma. Our results demonstrate that participants tend to choose the product that receives the most visual attention. However, future studies should aim to involve other age groups and different scents to obtain a more comprehensive knowledge on the effect of scents on the gazing behavior and choice.
From an analytical point of view, further research should focus on the role of other types of aromas (natural, artificial, etc.), the method of evaporation (heat-based diffusion vs. cool diffusion) and the aerial concentration of the aroma compounds as environmental factors with an impact on human perception.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/app12146899/s1, Figure S1: Dwell counts of the four muesli bar product alternatives. Figure S2: Dwell counts of the four tea product alternatives. Figure S3: Dwell counts of the four chocolate product alternatives. Figure S4: First fixation durations between the odor and odorless conditions for the muesli bar product group. Figure S5: First fixation durations between the odor and odorless conditions for the tea product group. Figure S6: First fixation durations between the odor and odorless conditions for the chocolate product group.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.S. and A.G.; methodology, D.S., D.R., O.F. and A.G.; software, D.S.; validation, O.F.; formal analysis, D.S. and D.R.; investigation, D.S.; resources, D.R. and A.G.; data curation, A.G.; writing—original draft preparation, D.S. and D.R.; writing—review and editing, D.R., A.G. and O.F.; visualization, D.S.; supervision, A.G. and O.F.; project administration, D.R. and A.G.; funding acquisition, A.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Research, Development, and Innovation Office of Hungary (OTKA, contracts No FK 137577 and K 134260) and ÚNKP-21-5 New National Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology from the source of the National Research, Development and Innovation Fund.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study as research data has been robustly anonymized, such that the original providers of the data cannot be identified, directly or indirectly, by anyone.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

D.S. thanks the support of the Doctoral School of Economic and Regional Sciences, Hungarian University of Agriculture and Life Sciences. A.G. thanks the support of the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences. D.R. and A.G. thank the support of the National Research, Development, and Innovation Office of Hungary (OTKA, contracts No FK 137577 and K 134260). This study was supported by the ÚNKP-21-5 New National Excellence Program of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology from the source of the National Research, Development and Innovation Fund. The authors would like to thank AC Nielsen Ltd.—Nielsen Brandbank (Váci u. 8., H-1056 Budapest, Hungary) for kindly providing the product photos used in the research. The authors thank the help of Dávid Bajusz for his helpful advice and support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Huddleston, P.T.; Behe, B.K.; Driesener, C.; Minahan, S. Inside-outside: Using eye-tracking to investigate search-choice processes in the retail environment. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2018, 43, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Saleh, M.; Alhosseini, S.E.; Slambolchi, A. A Review of Consumer Decision-Making Styles: Existing Styles and Proposed Additional Styles. Int. J. Res. IT Manag. Eng. 2017, 7, 33–44. [Google Scholar]
  3. Gunaratne, N.M.; Fuentes, S.; Gunaratne, T.M.; Torrico, D.D.; Ashman, H.; Francis, C.; Viejo, C.G.; Dunshea, F.R. Consumer acceptability, eye fixation, and physiological responses: A study of novel and familiar chocolate packaging designs using eye-tracking devices. Foods 2019, 8, 253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Chandwaskar, P. A Review on: Neuromarketing as an emerging field in consumer research. Int. J. Manag. Technol. Eng. 2018, 8, 7. [Google Scholar]
  5. Boesveldt, S.; Parma, V. The importance of the olfactory system in human well-being, through nutrition and social behavior. Cell Tiss. Res. 2021, 383, 559–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Seo, H.S.; Hummel, T. Effects of olfactory dysfunction on sensory evaluation and preparation of foods. Appetite 2009, 53, 314–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Manesse, C.; Ferdenzi, C.; Sabri, M.; Bessy, M.; Rouby, C.; Faure, F.; Bellil, D.; Jomain, S.; Landis, B.N.; Hugentobler, M.; et al. Dysosmia-Associated Changes in Eating Behavior. Chemosens. Percept. 2017, 10, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Albrecht, J.; Schreder, T.; Kleemann, A.M.; Schöpf, V.; Kopietz, R.; Anzinger, A.; Demmel, M.; Linn, J.; Kettenmann, B.; Wiesmann, M. Olfactory detection thresholds and pleasantness of a food-related and a non-food odour in hunger and satiety. Rhinology 2009, 47, 160–165. [Google Scholar]
  9. Croy, I.; Springborn, M.; Lötsch, J.; Johnston, A.N.B.; Hummel, T. Agreeable smellers and sensitive Neurotics-Correlations among personality traits and sensory thresholds. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, 18701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Seo, H.S.; Hähner, A.; Gudziol, V.; Scheibe, M.; Hummel, T. Influence of background noise on the performance in the odor sensitivity task: Effects of noise type and extraversion. Exp. Brain Res. 2012, 222, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Doucé, L.; Poels, K.; Janssens, W.; De Backer, C. Smelling the books: The effect of chocolate scent on purchase-related behavior in a bookstore. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 65–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Spangenberg, E.R.; Sprott, D.E.; Grohmann, B.; Tracy, D.L. Gender-congruent ambient scent influences on approach and avoidance behaviors in a retail store. J. Bus. Res. 2006, 59, 1281–1287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gagarina, A.; Pikturnienė, I. The Effect of Ambient Scent Type and Intensiveness on Decision Making Heuristics. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015, 213, 605–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Biswas, D.; Szocs, C. The Smell of Healthy Choices: Cross-Modal Sensory Compensation Effects of Ambient Scent on Food Purchases. J. Mark. Res. 2019, 56, 123–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Krishna, A. An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. J. Consum. Psychol. 2012, 22, 332–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Biswas, D.; Lund, K.; Szocs, C. Sounds like a healthy retail atmospheric strategy: Effects of ambient music and background noise on food sales. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2019, 47, 37–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pagani, M.; Racat, M.; Hofacker, C.F. Adding Voice to the Omnichannel and How that Affects Brand Trust. J. Interact. Mark. 2019, 48, 89–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Peng-Li, D.; Mathiesen, S.L.; Chan, R.C.K.; Byrne, D.V.; Wang, Q.J. Sounds Healthy: Modelling sound-evoked consumer food choice through visual attention. Appetite 2021, 164, 105264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Errajaa, K.; Daucé, B.; Legohérel, P. Consumer reactions to olfactory congruence with brand image. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2020, 52, 101898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lwin, M.O.; Morrin, M.; Chong, C.S.T.; Goh, S.X. Odor Semantics and Visual Cues: What We Smell Impacts Where We Look, What We Remember, and What We Want to Buy. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 29, 336–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Herz, R.S. The role of odor-evoked memory in psychological and physiological health. Brain. Sci. 2016, 6, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Bytyçi, S. Influence of Colors as a Key Element in Consumer Marketing. Expert J. Mark. 2020, 8, 41–47. [Google Scholar]
  23. Golnar-Nik, P.; Farashi, S.; Safari, M.S. The application of EEG power for the prediction and interpretation of consumer decision-making: A neuromarketing study. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 207, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Danner, L.; De Antoni, N.; Gere, A.; Sipos, L.; Kovács, S.; Dürrschmid, K. Make a choice! Visual attention and choice behaviour in multialternative food choice situations. Acta Aliment. 2016, 45, 515–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. dos Santos, R.D.O.J.; de Oliveira, J.H.C.; Rocha, J.B.; Giraldi, J.D.M.E. Eye Tracking in Neuromarketing: A Research Agenda for Marketing Studies. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 2015, 7, 32–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Orquin, J.L.; Mueller Loose, S. Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychol. 2013, 144, 190–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Rojas, J.C.; Marín-Morales, J.; Ausín Azofra, J.M.; Contero, M. Recognizing Decision-Making Using Eye Movement: A Case Study With Children. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 570470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Distel, H.; Hudson, R. Judgement of odor intensity is influenced by subjects’ knowledge of the odor source. Chem. Sens. 2001, 26, 247–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. De Araujo, I.E.; Rolls, E.T.; Velazco, M.I.; Margot, C.; Cayeux, I. Cognitive modulation of olfactory processing. Neuron 2005, 46, 671–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Bensafi, M.; Rinck, F.; Schaal, B.; Rouby, C. Verbal cues modulate hedonic perception of odors in 5-Year-Old children as well as in adults. Chem. Sens. 2007, 32, 855–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Zellner, D.A.; Bartoli, A.M.; Eckard, R. Influence of color on odor identification and liking ratings. Am. J. Psychol. 1991, 104, 547–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Sakai, N.; Imada, S.; Saito, S.; Kobayakawa, T.; Deguchi, Y. The effect of visual images on perception of odors. Chem. Sens. 2005, 30 (Suppl. 1), 244–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Castiello, U.; Zucco, G.M.; Parma, V.; Ansuini, C.; Tirindelli, R. Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and vision when grasping. Chem. Sens. 2006, 31, 665–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Michael, G. Ambient odors modulate visual attentional capture. Neurosci. Lett. 2003, 352, 221–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tubaldi, F.; Ansuini, C.; Tirindelli, R.; Castiello, U. The grasping side of odours. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, 1795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Tubaldi, F.; Ansuini, C.; Demattè, M.L.; Tirindelli, R.; Castiello, U. Effects of olfactory stimuli on arm-reaching duration. Chem. Sens. 2008, 33, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Morquecho-Campos, P.; Hellmich, I.M.; Zwart, E.; de Graaf, K.; Boesveldt, S. Does odour priming influence snack choice?—An eye-tracking study to understand food choice processes. Appetite 2022, 168, 105772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fonyó, A. Az Orvosi Élettan Tankönyve; Medicina könyvkiadó Rt: Budapest, Hungary, 2011; ISBN 9789632263441. [Google Scholar]
  39. Heuberger, E.; Ilmberger, J. The influence of essential oils on human vigilance. Nat. Prod. Commun. 2010, 5, 1441–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Michael, G.A.; Jacquot, L.; Millot, J.L.; Brand, G. Ambient odors influence the amplitude and time course of visual distraction. Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 119, 708–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Walla, P. Olfaction and its dynamic influence on word and face processing: Cross-modal integration. Prog. Neurobiol. 2008, 84, 192–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Morrot, G.; Brochet, F.; Dubourdieu, D. The color of odors. Brain Lang. 2001, 79, 309–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Gottfried, J.A.; Dolan, R.J. The nose smells what the eye sees: Crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 2003, 39, 375–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Negri, A.S.; Allegra, D.; Simoni, L.; Rusconi, F.; Tonelli, C.; Espen, L.; Galbiati, M. Comparative analysis of fruit aroma patterns in the domesticated wild strawberries “Profumata di Tortona” (F. moschata) and “Regina delle Valli” (F. vesca). Front. Plant Sci. 2015, 6, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Ulrich, D.; Komes, D.; Olbricht, K.; Hoberg, E. Diversity of aroma patterns in wild and cultivated Fragaria accessions. Genet. Resour. Crop Evol. 2007, 54, 1185–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kafkas, E.; Kafkas, S.; Koch-Dean, M.; Schwab, W.; Larkov, O.; Lavid, N.; Bar, E.; Ravid, U.; Lewinsohn, E. Comparison of methodologies for the identification of aroma compounds in strawberry. Turk. J. Agric. For. 2005, 29, 383–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Fan, Z.; Hasing, T.; Johnson, T.S.; Garner, D.M.; Barbey, C.R.; Colquhoun, T.A.; Sims, C.A.; Resende, M.F.R.; Whitaker, V.M. Strawberry sweetness and consumer preference are enhanced by specific volatile compounds. Hortic. Res. 2021, 8, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Pelayo, C.; Ebeler, S.E.; Kader, A.A. Postharvest life and flavor quality of three strawberry cultivars kept at 5°C in air or air+20 kPa CO2. Postharvest. Biol. Technol. 2003, 27, 171–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Starowicz, M. Analysis of volatiles in food products. Separations 2021, 8, 157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Zhang, R.; Tang, C.; Jiang, B.; Mo, X.; Wang, Z. Optimization of hs-spme for gc-ms analysis and its application in characterization of volatile compounds in sweet potato. Molecules 2021, 26, 5808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fiedler, S.; Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M.; Renkewitz, F.; Orquin, J.L. Guideline for reporting standards of eye-tracking research in decision sciences. PsyArXiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Vandendriessche, T.; Nicolai, B.M.; Hertog, M.L.A.T.M. Optimization of HS SPME Fast GC-MS for High-Throughput Analysis of Strawberry Aroma. Food Anal. Methods 2013, 6, 512–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Velasco, C.; Balboa, D.; Marmolejo-Ramos, F.; Spence, C. Crossmodal effect of music and odor pleasantness on olfactory quality perception. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Ba, M.; Kang, J.; Li, Z. The effects of sounds and food odour on crowd behaviours in urban public open spaces. Build. Environ. 2020, 182, 107104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kim, Y.H.; Kim, K.H.; Szulejko, J.E.; Parker, D. Quantitative analysis of fragrance and odorants released from fresh and decaying strawberries. Sensors 2013, 13, 7939–7978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Burdock, G. Fenaroli’s Handbook of Flavor Ingredients, 4th ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2010; ISBN 978-1-4200-9077-2. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ménager, I.; Jost, M.; Aubert, C. Changes in Physicochemical Characteristics and Volatile Constituents of Strawberry (Cv. Cigaline) during Maturation. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2004, 52, 1248–1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ulrich, D.; Hoberg, E.; Rapp, A.; Kecke, S. Analysis of strawberry flavour-discrimination of aroma types by quantification of volatile compounds. Eur. Food Res. Technol. 1997, 205, 218–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sorokowska, A.; Schriever, V.A.; Gudziol, V.; Hummel, C.; Hähner, A.; Iannilli, E.; Sinding, C.; Aziz, M.; Seo, H.S.; Negoias, S.; et al. Changes of olfactory abilities in relation to age: Odor identification in more than 1400 people aged 4 to 80 years. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2015, 272, 1937–1944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Anderson, R.C.; Anderson, J.H. Acute toxic effects of fragrance products. Arch. Environ. Health 1998, 53, 138–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Seo, H.S.; Buschhüter, D.; Hummel, T. Odor attributes change in relation to the time of the year. Cinnamon odor is more familiar and pleasant during Christmas season than summertime. Appetite 2009, 53, 222–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The four presented choice sets from top left to bottom right: chocolate, tea, muesli bar, yoghurt.
Figure 1. The four presented choice sets from top left to bottom right: chocolate, tea, muesli bar, yoghurt.
Applsci 12 06899 g001
Figure 2. Frequency of choice for all four choice sets with each four alternatives. * indicates a significant effect at a level of p < 0.05. Product categories had different flavors; only the strawberry flavor was common, which is colored in red.
Figure 2. Frequency of choice for all four choice sets with each four alternatives. * indicates a significant effect at a level of p < 0.05. Product categories had different flavors; only the strawberry flavor was common, which is colored in red.
Applsci 12 06899 g002
Figure 3. Dwell counts of the four yoghurt product alternatives.
Figure 3. Dwell counts of the four yoghurt product alternatives.
Applsci 12 06899 g003
Figure 4. First fixation durations between the odor and odorless conditions for the yoghurt product group.
Figure 4. First fixation durations between the odor and odorless conditions for the yoghurt product group.
Applsci 12 06899 g004
Figure 5. Frequency of choice for all four choice sets with each four alternatives by the two conditions.
Figure 5. Frequency of choice for all four choice sets with each four alternatives by the two conditions.
Applsci 12 06899 g005
Figure 6. Aroma profile of strawberry essential oil. Total ion chromatograms of three parallel measurements (colored by black, green and red).
Figure 6. Aroma profile of strawberry essential oil. Total ion chromatograms of three parallel measurements (colored by black, green and red).
Applsci 12 06899 g006
Table 1. Demographic profile of the participants in the odor and odorless conditions (%).
Table 1. Demographic profile of the participants in the odor and odorless conditions (%).
OdorlessOdor
Gendermale 19.422.6
female30.627.4
place of livingmalecapital city11.314.5
large city3.21.6
small town3.21.6
rural1.64.8
femalecapital city8.13.2
large city4.83.2
small town9.712.9
rural8.18.1
educationmalegraduate3.21.6
undergraduate16.121.0
femalegraduate8.16.5
undergraduate22.621.0
visual aidmalecontact lenses1.63.2
glasses4.86.5
femalecontact lenses1.64.8
glasses8.13.2
Table 2. Repeated measures ANOVA presenting the F-values of the three main effects and two of their interactions for the four product groups.
Table 2. Repeated measures ANOVA presenting the F-values of the three main effects and two of their interactions for the four product groups.
EffectChocolateYoghurtMuesli BarTea
Product3.0681.0573.9995.181 *
Choice0.8540.9740.9231.919
Odor61.7621.9080.533
Product × Choice1.673 *1.527 *1.723 *1.134 *
Product × Odor0.7022.667 *0.781.329
Bold and * indicates significant effect at a significant level of p < 0.05.
Table 3. Results of the repeated measures ANOVA (RMANOVA). Factor choice is not stated due to no significant effects observed in RMANOVA.
Table 3. Results of the repeated measures ANOVA (RMANOVA). Factor choice is not stated due to no significant effects observed in RMANOVA.
Eye-Tracking ParameterChocolateYoghurtMuesli BarTea
ProductTTFF2.0151.4421.6582.206
FFD1.1441.3381.0891.326
FD2.5771.9561.5891.445
FC2.0831.0693.057 *2.698
DD2.2571.6311.8521.313
DC4.410 *0.9951.0391.873
Product × ChoiceTTFF1.2130.3690.3920.636
FFD0.3950.7370.861.173
FD4.472 *2.881 *3.219 *2.005 *
FC3.989 *3.282 *3.124 *2.261 *
DD4.607 *3.094 *3.449 *2.163 *
DC6.459 *3.720 *2.540 *1.211
Product × OdorTTFF0.130.6510.1071.642
FFD0.4332.326 *2.466 *3.284 *
FD0.3210.4720.411.75
FC0.1440.7780.2521.361
DD0.2640.3770.2721.643
DC0.2692.270 *0.440.343
Bold and * indicates significant effect at a significant level of p < 0.05. TTFF: Time To First Fixation; FFD: First Fixation Duration; FD: Fixation Duration; FC: Fixation Count; DD: Dwell Duration; DC: Dwell Count.
Table 4. List of strawberry aroma’s compounds.
Table 4. List of strawberry aroma’s compounds.
rT (min)NameCASFormulaMWRI (Calculated)RI (Literature)RI Diff
0.96Ethanol64-17-5C2H6O46.1-448-
1.30ethyl acetate141-78-6C4H8O288.16126131.0
2.58methyl 2-methylbutyrate868-57-5C6H12O2116.27807800.1
2.83ethyl butyrate105-54-4C6H12O2116.28058060.9
3.28ethyl 2-methylbutyrate7452-79-1C7H14O2130.28578516.4
3.32ethyl isovalerate (ethyl 3-methylbutyrate)108-64-5C7H14O2130.28628592.7
3.33(cis)-3-hexenol928-96-1C6H12O100.28638651.6
3.441-hexanol111-27-3C6H14O102.28758740.7
3.50isoamyl acetate (banana oil)123-92-2C7H14O2130.28828783.9
4.33ethyl hexanoate123-66-0C8H16O2144.29999980.7
4.38(E)-3-hexenol acetate3681-82-1C8H14O2142.2100610051.4
4.41hexyl acetate142-92-7C8H16O2144.2101310112.2
4.94iso-amyl isovalerate (apple oil)659-70-1C10H20O2172.3110611051.2
MW: Molecular weight, RI: Kováts’ retention index, RI diff: the difference between the calculated and determined RI’s. Bold letter indicates those compounds which were present in the testing room.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Szakál, D.; Fehér, O.; Radványi, D.; Gere, A. Effect of Scents on Gazing Behavior and Choice. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 6899. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12146899

AMA Style

Szakál D, Fehér O, Radványi D, Gere A. Effect of Scents on Gazing Behavior and Choice. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(14):6899. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12146899

Chicago/Turabian Style

Szakál, Dorina, Orsolya Fehér, Dalma Radványi, and Attila Gere. 2022. "Effect of Scents on Gazing Behavior and Choice" Applied Sciences 12, no. 14: 6899. https://doi.org/10.3390/app12146899

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop