On the Importance of Both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Defining Our Constructs
3. Considering Affective Valence
4. Considering Anger
4.1. An Introduction to the Structure and Functions of Anger
4.2. Exceptions to the Classifications of Anger as Negative and Approach Oriented
4.2.1. Anger Associated with Withdrawal
4.2.2. Anger Associated with Positive Valence?
5. Dissonance Affect and Approach Motivation
6. Similarities between Approach-Related Positive and Negative Emotions: Anger and Determination
7. Functions Associated with Discrete Emotions vs. Dimensions of Emotions
8. Discrete Emotions within Emotion Families
9. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Barrett, L.F.; Lindquist, K.A.; Bliss-Moreau, E.; Duncan, S.; Gendron, M.; Mize, J.; Brennan, L. Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 2, 297–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Russell, J.A. A circumplex model of affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 1161–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, D. Mood and Temperament; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation; Cole, J., Ed.; University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, UK, 1972; Volume 19, pp. 207–283. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. All emotions are basic. In The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions; Ekman, P., Davidson, R., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994; pp. 15–19. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. Emotions Revealved; Holt Paperbacks: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Izard, C.E. Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 2, 260–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lench, H.C.; Flores, S.A.; Bench, S.W. Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 137, 834–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Neurologizing the psychology of affects: How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 2, 281–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bradley, M.M. Emotion and motivation. In Handbook of Psychophysiology; Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., Berntson, G., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; Volume 2, pp. 602–642. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J.; Watt, D. What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience. Emot. Rev. 2011, 3, 387–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Izard, C.E. The many meanings/aspects of emotion: Definitions, functions, activation, and regulation. Emot. Rev. 2010, 2, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Amodio, D.M.; Gable, P.A. Attitudes toward emotions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Price, T.F. What is approach motivation? Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 291–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Price, T.F.; Peterson, C.K.; Gable, P.A.; Harmon-Jones, C. The influence of behavioral approach and behavioral inhibition sensitivities on emotion cognitive processes. In Handbook of Cognition and Emotion; Robinson, M.D., Watkins, E.R., Harmon-Jones, E., Eds.; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 329–346. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Gardner, W.L.; Berntson, G.G. The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 839–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biol. Psychiatry 1998, 44, 1248–1263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gable, P.A.; Harmon-Jones, E. Does arousal per se account for the influence of appetitive stimuli on attentional scope and the late positive potential? Psychophysiology 2013, 50, 344–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shaver, P.; Schwartz, J.; Kirson, D.; O’Connor, C. Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 1061–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, C.; Bastian, B.; Harmon-Jones, E. Detecting transient emotional responses with improved self-report measures and instructions. Emotion 2016, 16, 1086–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, C.; Bastian, B.; Harmon-Jones, E. The discrete emotions questionnaire: A new tool for measuring state self-reported emotions. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0159915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, C.A.; Lazarus, R.S. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cogn. Emot. 1993, 7, 233–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkowitz, L.; Harmon-Jones, E. Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion 2004, 4, 107–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carver, C.S. Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system. Emotion 2004, 4, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mikulincer, M. Reactance and helplessness following exposure to unsolvable problems: The effects of attributional style. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 679–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dollard, J.; Miller, N.E.; Doob, L.W.; Mowrer, O.H.; Sears, R.R. Frustration and Aggression; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1939. [Google Scholar]
- Carver, C.S.; Harmon-Jones, E. Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 183–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lewis, M.; Alessandri, S.M.; Sullivan, M.W. Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Dev. Psychol. 1990, 26, 745–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, M.; Sullivan, M.W.; Ramsay, D.S.; Alessandri, S.M. Individual differences in anger and sad expressions during extinction: Antecedents and consequences. Infant Behav. Dev. 1992, 15, 443–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coan, J.A.; Allen, J.J.; Harmon-Jones, E. Voluntary facial expression and hemispheric asymmetry over the frontal cortex. Psychophysiology 2001, 38, 912–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keltner, D.; Ellsworth, P.C.; Edwards, K. Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 64, 740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adams, R.B.; Kleck, R.E. Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion 2005, 5, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Adams, R.B.; Ambady, N.; Macrae, C.N.; Kleck, R.E. Emotional expressions forecast approach-avoidance behavior. Motiv. Emot. 2006, 30, 179–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, C.; Schmeichel, B.J.; Mennitt, E.; Harmon-Jones, E. The expression of determination: Similarities between anger and approach-related positive affect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amodio, D.M.; Harmon-Jones, E. Trait emotions and affective modulation of the startle eyeblink: On the unique relationship of trait anger. Emotion 2011, 11, 47–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E. Anger and the behavioral approach system. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 35, 995–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carver, C.S.; White, T.L. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deater-Deckard, K.; Beekman, C.; Wang, Z.; Kim, J.; Petrill, S.; Thompson, L.; De Thomrone, L. Approach/positive anticipation, frustration/anger, and overt aggression in childhood. J. Personal. 2010, 78, 991–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Allen, J.J. Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E. Trait anger predicts relative left frontal cortical activation to anger-inducing stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007, 66, 154–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Sigelman, J. State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 80, 797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, F.C.; Nimmo-Smith, I.; Lawrence, A.D. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 3, 207–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A. On the role of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity in approach and withdrawal motivation: An updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 2017, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moyer, K.E. The Psychobiology of Aggression; Harper & Row: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Adams, D.B. Brain mechanisms of aggressive behavior: An updated review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006, 30, 304–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lagerspetz, K.M.J. Aggression and aggressiveness in laboratory mice. In Aggressive Behavior; Garattini, S., Sigg, E.B., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1969; pp. 77–85. [Google Scholar]
- Kalin, N.H. Primate models to understand human aggression. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1999, 60, 29–32. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Nelson, R.J.; Trainor, B.C. Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kazlauckas, V.; Schuh, J.; Dall’Igna, O.P.; Pereira, G.S.; Bonan, C.D.; Lara, D.R. Behavioral and cognitive profile of mice with high and low exploratory phenotypes. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 162, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Plutchik, R. Emotions: A general psychoevolutionary theory. In Approaches to Emotion; Scherer, K.R., Ekman, P., Eds.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 1984; pp. 197–218. [Google Scholar]
- Sutton, S.K.; Davidson, R.J. Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychol. Sci. 1997, 8, 204–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zinner, L.R.; Brodish, A.B.; Devine, P.G.; Harmon-Jones, E. Anger and asymmetrical frontal cortical activity: Evidence for an anger–withdrawal relationship. Cogn. Emot. 2008, 22, 1081–1093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelley, N.J.; Hortensius, R.; Harmon-Jones, E. When anger leads to rumination: Induction of relative right frontal cortical activity with transcranial direct current stimulation increases anger-related rumination. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 475–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denson, T.F.; Pedersen, W.C.; Miller, N. The displaced aggression questionnaire. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 90, 1032–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Sigelman, J.; Bohlig, A.; Harmon-Jones, C. Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effect of coping potential on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hewig, J.; Hagemann, D.; Seifert, J.; Naumann, E.; Bartussek, D. On the selective relation of frontal cortical asymmetry and anger-out versus anger-control. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 87, 926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spielberger, C.D. Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI); Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, D.; Wiese, D.; Vaidya, J.; Tellegen, A. The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Abramson, L.; Peterson, C.K. PANAS positive activation is associated with anger. Emotion 2009, 9, 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Vaughn-Scott, K.; Mohr, S.; Sigelman, J.; Harmon-Jones, C. The effect of manipulated sympathy and anger on left and right frontal cortical activity. Emotion 2004, 4, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C. On the relationship of trait PANAS positive activation and trait anger: Evidence of a suppressor relationship. J. Res. Personal. 2010, 44, 120–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pettersson, E.; Turkheimer, E. Approach temperament, anger, and evaluation: Resolving a paradox. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance; Row, Peterson: Evanston, IL, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Elkin, R.A.; Leippe, M.R. Physiological arousal, dissonance, and attitude change: Evidence for a dissonance-arousal link and a “don’t remind me” effect. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Brehm, J.W.; Greenberg, J.; Simon, L.; Nelson, D.E. Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliot, A.J.; Devine, P.G. On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 382–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E. Cognitive dissonance and experienced negative affect: Evidence that dissonance increases experienced negative affect even in the absence of aversive consequences. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 26, 1490–1501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Amodio, D.M.; Harmon-Jones, C. Action-based model of dissonance: A review, integration, and expansion of conceptions of cognitive conflict. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Zanna, M.P., Ed.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2009; Volume 41, pp. 119–166. [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Levy, N. An action-based model of cognitive dissonance processes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 34, 184–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C. Testing the action-based model of cognitive dissonance: The effect of action orientation on postdecisional attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 28, 711–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Fearn, M.; Sigelman, J.D.; Johnson, P. Left frontal cortical activation and spreading of alternatives: Tests of the action-based model of dissonance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 94, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, C.; Schmeichel, B.J.; Inzlicht, M.; Harmon-Jones, E. Trait approach motivation relates to dissonance reduction. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2011, 2, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Gerdjikov, T.; Harmon-Jones, C. The effect of induced compliance on relative left frontal cortical activity: A test of the action-based model of dissonance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 38, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Serra, R.; Gable, P.A. The effect of commitment on relative left frontal cortical activity: Tests of the action-based model of dissonance. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011, 37, 395–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Price, T.F.; Harmon-Jones, C. Supine body posture decreases rationalizations: Testing the action-based model of dissonance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 56, 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amodio, D.M.; Devine, P.G.; Harmon-Jones, E. A dynamic model of guilt: Implications for motivation and self-regulation in the context of prejudice. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 524–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scholsberg, H. A scale for the judgement of facial expressions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1941, 29, 497–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1872. [Google Scholar]
- Plutchik, R. A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience; Plutchik, R., Kellerman, H., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1980; Volume 1, pp. 3–33. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, M.M. Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology 2009, 46, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A.; Price, T.F. Does negative affect always narrow and positive affect always broaden the mind? Considering the influence of motivational intensity on cognitive scope. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 22, 301–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carver, C. Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cogn. Emot. 2003, 17, 241–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gut, E. Productive and Unproductive Depression: Its Functions and Failures; Routledge: London, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Klinger, E. Meaning and Void: Inner Experience and the Incentives in Peoples Lives; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Navon, D. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 1977, 9, 353–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gable, P.A.; Harmon-Jones, E. Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 476–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gable, P.A.; Harmon-Jones, E. Attentional consequences of pregoal and postgoal positive affects. Emotion 2011, 11, 1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Price, T.F.; Harmon-Jones, E. The effect of embodied emotive states on cognitive categorization. Emotion 2010, 10, 934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gable, P.; Harmon-Jones, E. The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 211–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gable, P.A.; Poole, B.D.; Harmon-Jones, E. Anger perceptually and conceptually narrows cognitive scope. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 109, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Easterbrook, J.A. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychol. Rev. 1959, 66, 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bradley, M.M.; Lang, P.J. Emotion and motivation. In Handbook of Psychophysiology; Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., Berntson, G., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 581–607. [Google Scholar]
- Rubio-Godoy, M.R.A.; Curtis, V. Serotonin—A link between disgust and immunity? Med. Hypotheses 2007, 68, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blake, K.R.; Yih, J.; Zhao, K.; Sung, B.; Harmon-Jones, C. Skin-transmitted pathogens and the heebie jeebies: Evidence for a subclass of disgust stimuli that evoke a qualitatively unique emotional response. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 1153–1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davey, G.C.L. The ‘disgusting’ spider: The role of disease and illness in the perpetuation of fear of spiders. Soc. Anim. 1994, 2, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Summerell, E. On the Importance of Both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion. Behav. Sci. 2017, 7, 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066
Harmon-Jones E, Harmon-Jones C, Summerell E. On the Importance of Both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion. Behavioral Sciences. 2017; 7(4):66. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066
Chicago/Turabian StyleHarmon-Jones, Eddie, Cindy Harmon-Jones, and Elizabeth Summerell. 2017. "On the Importance of Both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion" Behavioral Sciences 7, no. 4: 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066
APA StyleHarmon-Jones, E., Harmon-Jones, C., & Summerell, E. (2017). On the Importance of Both Dimensional and Discrete Models of Emotion. Behavioral Sciences, 7(4), 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040066