Functional Freedom: A Psychological Model of Freedom in Decision-Making
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Defining Functional Freedom and Its Scope
3. The Model of Functional Freedom in Decision-Making
3.1. Rationale of the Model Construction
3.2. Self-Reflection—Freedom of the Decision Process
3.2.1. Idea and Background
3.2.2. Dimensional Indicators
- (1)
- A direct and intense focus of attention on the decision problem and its (mental) context.
- (2)
- A complex integration of anticipative, knowledge-based, and evaluative information and processes.
- (3)
- A high number of generated mental propositions (e.g., thoughts about other mental propositions; new insights and connections between representations; changes in preferences, beliefs or expectancies).
- (4)
- A greater activation, modification or generation of decision-related rules.
- (5)
- The decision is based strongly on logical and justifiable arguments.
3.3. Underdetermination—Freedom of the Decision Structure
3.3.1. Idea and Background
3.3.2. Dimensional Indicators
- (1)
- There are many relevant options.
- (2)
- There is a conflict between options (i.e., wants, motives) because they are mutually exclusive or very similar in value, so that the difference in preferences is very small.
- (3)
- The decision situation is unknown and new, so that there is no routine or default option available.
- (4)
- The decision outcome is of relevance for the individual and the self, as trivial choices (e.g., “which of two full bottles of water should I take?”) are likely to be skipped, for example, by picking randomly.
- (5)
- A larger degree of ambivalence within each option (i.e., option features with positive vs. negative valence).
3.4. Rationality—Freedom of the Decider
3.4.1. Idea and Background
3.4.2. Dimensional Indicators
- (1)
- (2)
- Self-awareness and self-knowledge, as well as knowledge about the world—this might be subsumed under the trait of wisdom (i.e., a generalized experience with and understanding of world affairs) [51].
- (3)
- Functional, intact memory processes (e.g., declarative knowledge, retrieval) in order to enable anticipation and mental simulation.
3.5. Assessing the Functional Freedom in a Decision
3.5.1. What Constrains Functional Freedom?
3.5.2. Characteristics of the Dimensions
4. Hypotheses, Benefits and Implications
4.1. Hypotheses and Preliminary Support
4.1.1. Subjective Freedom
4.1.2. Underdetermination
4.1.3. Self-Reflection
4.1.4. Rationality
4.1.5. Neuropsychology
4.2. Benefits and Implications
4.2.1. What Is It Good for?—Adaptive Benefits of Functional Freedom
4.2.2. Functional Freedom Can Be Lost and Attained
4.2.3. Implications for Research on Human Freedom
5. Concluding Remarks on Freedom as a Variable
6. Open Challenges
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Van Inwagen, P. An Essay on Free Will; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Lycan, W.G. Consciousness; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Gadenne, V. Philosophie der Psychologie; Bern: Huber, Singapore, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- McKenna, M.; Coates, D.J. Compatibilism. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Winter 2016 ed.; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Stanford University: Stanford, CA, USA, 2004; Available online: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/compatibilism/ (accessed on 20 April 2017).
- Westcott, M.R. Free will: An exercise in metaphysical truth or psychological consequences. Can. Psychol. Rev./Psychologie Canadienne 1977, 18, 249–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Westcott, M.R. The Psychology of Human Freedom: A Human Science Perspective and Critique; Springer: New York, UK, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Brehm, S.S.; Brehm, J.W. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R.M.; Deci, E.L. Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? J. Personal. 2006, 74, 1557–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baumeister, R.F. Free will in scientific psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 3, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vohs, K.D.; Schooler, J.W. The value of believing in free will: Encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 19, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haggard, P. Human volition: Towards a neuroscience of will. Nat. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 934–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schüür, F.; Haggard, P. What are self-generated actions? Conscious. Cognit. 2011, 20, 1697–1704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monroe, A.E.; Malle, B.F. From Uncaused Will to Conscious Choice: The Need to Study, Not Speculate About People’s Folk Concept of Free Will. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 2010, 1, 211–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monroe, A.E.; Dillon, K.D.; Malle, B.F. Bringing free will down to Earth: People’s psychological concept of free will and its role in moral judgment. Conscious. Cognit. 2014, 27, 100–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beckermann, A. Free will in a natural order of the world. In Philosophie und/als Wissenschaft; Nimtz, C., Beckermann, A., Eds.; Paderborn: Mentis, Germany, 2005; pp. 11–126. [Google Scholar]
- Monroe, A.E.; Malle, B.F. Free will without metaphysics. In Surrounding Free Will; Mele, A.R., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, in press.
- Nahmias, E. Free Will as a Psychological Accomplishment. In The Oxford Handbook of Freedom; Schmitdz, D., Pavel, C., Eds.; University Press: Oxford, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister, R.F. The self. In Handbook of Social Psychology; Gilbert, D.T., Fiske, S.T., Lindzey, G., Eds.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 680–740. [Google Scholar]
- Gigerenzer, G. Gut Feelings: The Intelligence of the Unconscious; Viking: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Spinoza, B. Die Ethik; Reclam: Leipzig, Germany, 1677/1988. [Google Scholar]
- Bieri, P. Das Handwerk der Freiheit. In Über Die Entdeckung des Eigenen Willens; München: Hanser, Germany, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Masicampo, E.J. Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: How mental simulations serve the animal-culture interface. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 945–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gadenne, V. Bewusstsein, Kognition und Gehirn: Einführung in Die Psychologie des Bewusstseins; Bern: Huber, Singapore, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Kluwe, R.H. Steuerung von Denkvorgängen in Modellen menschlicher Informationsverarbeitung. [Control of Thought Processes in Models of Human Information Processing.]. In Freiheit des Entscheidens und Handelns; Cranach, M.V., Foppa, K., Eds.; Asanger: Heidelberg, Germany, 1996; pp. 151–170. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, K.E.; West, R.F. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 645–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Farar, Straus and Giroux: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg, M. Is free will an illusion? Nature 2009, 459, 164–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kisner, M.J. Spinoza on Human Freedom: Reason, Autonomy and the Good Life; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Frankl, V.E. From Death-Camp to Existentialism: A Psychiatrist’s Path to a New Therapy; Beacon Press: Boston, MA, USA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
- Fromm, E. The Heart of Man: Its Genius for Good and Evil; Harper & Row: New York, NY, USA, 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. The Computer and the Mind; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Tuomela, R. Human Action and Its Explanation: A Study on the Philosophical Foundations of Psychology; D. Reidel Publishing: Dordrecht, The Netherlands; Boston, MA, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Stillman, T.F.; Baumeister, R.F.; Mele, A.R. Free will in everyday life: Autobiographical accounts of free and unfree actions. Philos. Psychol. 2011, 24, 381–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luchins, A.S. Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychol. Monogr. 1942, 54, 1–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beck, A.T. Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1963, 9, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dörner, D. Der freie Wille und Selbstreflexion. In Freiheit des Entscheidens und Handelns; Cranach, M.V., Foppa, K., Eds.; Asanger: Heidelberg, Germany, 1996; pp. 125–150. [Google Scholar]
- DeWall, C.N.; Baumeister, R.F.; Masicampo, E.J. Evidence that logical reasoning depends on conscious processing. Conscious. Cognit. 2008, 17, 628–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tversky, A.; Shafir, E. Choice under conflict: The dynamics of deferred decision. Psychol. Sci. 1992, 3, 358–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herrmann, T. Willensfreiheit-eine nützliche Fiktion? [Free Will—A Useful Fiction?]. In Freiheit des Entscheidens und Handelns; Cranach, M.V., Foppa, K., Eds.; Asanger: Heidelberg, Germany, 1996; pp. 56–69. [Google Scholar]
- Cranach, M.V. Action freedom and determination as process and experience. Z. Sozialpsychologie 1991, 3, 4–24. [Google Scholar]
- Cranach, M.V. Handlungs-Entscheidungsfreiheit: Ein sozialpsychologisches Modell. [Action-Decision Freedom: A Social-Psychological Model.]. In Freiheit des Entscheidens und Handelns; Cranach, M.V., Foppa, K., Eds.; Asanger: Heidelberg, Germany, 1996; pp. 253–283. [Google Scholar]
- Hájiček, P. Free will as relative freedom with conscious component. Conscious. Cognit. 2009, 18, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lipshitz, R.; Strauss, O. Coping with uncertainty: A naturalistic decision making analysis. Org. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1997, 69, 149–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lipshitz, R. Decision making as argument-driven action. In Decision Making in Action: Models and Methods; Klein, G.A., Orasanu, J., Calderwood, R., Zsambok, C.E., Eds.; Ablex Publishing: Westport, CT, USA, 1993; pp. 172–181. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, J. Thinking and Deciding, 4th ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Greene, J.; Cohen, J.D. For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. 2004, 259, 1775–1785. [Google Scholar]
- Walter, H. Willensfreiheit, Verantwortlichkeit und Neurowissenschaft.[Free Will, Responsibility and Neuroscience.]. Psychol. Rundsch. 2004, 55, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Bauer, I.M.; Lloyd, S.A. Choice, free will, and religion. Psychol. Relig. Spirit 2010, 2, 67–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofmann, W.; Schmeichel, B.J.; Baddeley, A.D. Executive functions and self-regulation. TRENDS Cognit. Sci. 2012, 16, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sternberg, R.J. A balance theory of wisdom. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 347–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luce, M.F. Choosing to avoid: Coping with negatively emotion laden consumer decisions. J. Consum. Res. 1998, 24, 409–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inbar, Y.; Cone, J.; Gilovich, T. People’s intuitions about intuitive insight and intuitive choice. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 232–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pacherie, E. The Sense of Control and the Sense of Agency. Psyche 2007, 13, 1–30. [Google Scholar]
- Mele, A.R. Effective Intentions: The Power of Conscious Will; Oxford University Press on Demand: Oxford, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Isham, E.A.; Wulf, K.A.; Mejia, C.; Krisst, L.C. Deliberation period during easy and difficult decisions: Re-examining Libet’s “veto” window in a more ecologically valid framework. Neurosci. Conscious. 2017, 3, nix002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nahmias, E.; Morris, S.; Nadelhoffer, T.; Turner, J. The phenomenology of free will. J. Conscious. Stud. 2004, 11, 162–179. [Google Scholar]
- Lau, S.; Hiemisch, A.; Baumeister, R.F. The experience of freedom in decisions—Questioning philosophical beliefs in favor of psychological determinants. Conscious. Cognit. 2015, 33, 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Filevich, E.; Vanneste, P.; Brass, M.; Fias, W.; Haggard, P.; Kühn, S. Brain correlates of subjective freedom of choice. Conscious. Cognit. 2013, 22, 1271–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wenke, D.; Fleming, S.M.; Haggard, P. Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action. Cognition 2010, 115, 26–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Damen, T.G.; van Baaren, R.B.; Dijksterhuis, A. You should read this! Perceiving and acting upon action primes influences one’s sense of agency. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 50, 21–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, H.; Drolet, A. Choice and Self-Expression: A Cultural Analysis of Variety-Seeking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 373–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Webster, D.M.; Kruglanski, A.W. Cognitive and social consequences of the need for cognitive closure. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 8, 133–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lau, S.; Walter, S. Is feeling free all about good outcomes?—A validation study. Manuscript in preparation. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon, K.M.; Kasser, T. Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress but not all progress is beneficial. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 24, 1319–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ciarocco, N.J.; Vohs, K.D.; Baumeister, R.F. Some good news about rumination: Task-focused thinking after failure facilitates performance improvement. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2010, 29, 1057–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Sparks, E.A.; Stillman, T.F.; Vohs, K.D. Free will in consumer behavior: Self-control, ego depletion, and choice. J. Consum. Psychol. 2008, 18, 4–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reis, J. Ambiguitätstoleranz: Beiträge zur Entwicklung eines Persönlichkeitskonstruktes; Asanger: Heidelberg, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Buhr, K.; Dugas, M.J. The intolerance of uncertainty scale: Psychometric properties of the English version. Behav. Res. Ther. 2002, 40, 931–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pacini, R.; Epstein, S. The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 76, 972–987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sebanz, N.; Prinz, W. Disorders of Volition; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Soon, C.S.; Brass, M.; Heinze, H.J.; Haynes, J.D. Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2008, 11, 543–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soon, C.S.; He, A.H.; Bode, S.; Haynes, J.-D. Predicting free choices for abstract intentions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 6217–6222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miles, J.B. ‘Irresponsible and a disservice’: The integrity of social psychology turns on the free will dilemma. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 52, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prinz, W. Free will as a social institution. In Does Consciousness Cause Behavior? Pockett, S., Banks, W.P., Gallagher, S., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 257–276. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister, R.F. The Cultural Animal: Human Nature, Meaning, and Social Life; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Vollmer, G. Evolutionäre Erkenntnistheorie; Stuttgart: Hirzel, Switzerland, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Barlas, Z.; Obhi, S. Freedom, choice, and the sense of agency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heckhausen, J.; Wrosch, C.; Schulz, R. A Motivational Theory of Life-Span Development. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Skinner, B.F. Beyond Freedom and Dignity; Knopf: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Bargh, J.A. Free will is un-natural. In Are We Free? Psychology and Free Will; Baer, J., Kaufman, J.C., Baumeister, R.F., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 128–154. [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig, A.M. The Price of Greatness: Resolving the Creativity and Madness Controversy; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Levy, D.A. Tools of Critical Thinking: Metathoughts for Psychology, 2nd ed.; Waveland Press: Long Grove, IL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Libet, B. Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behav. Brain Sci. 1985, 8, 529–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wegner, D.M. The Illusion of Conscious Will; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Wegner, D.M. The mind’s best trick: How we experience conscious will. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2003, 7, 65–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Libet, B.; Gleason, C.A.; Wright, E.W.; Pearl, D.K. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 1983, 106, 623–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wegner, D.M.; Wheatley, T. Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. Am. Psychol. 1999, 54, 480–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gazzaniga, M.S. Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 2007, 123, 1293–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bargh, J.A. Bypassing the will: Toward demystifying the nonconscious control of social behavior. In The New Unconscious; Hassin, R.R., Bargh, J.A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 37–58. [Google Scholar]
- Dijksterhuis, A.; Nordgren, L.F. A theory of unconscious thought. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 1, 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pizarro, D.A.; Bloom, P. The intelligence of the moral intuitions: A comment on Haidt (2001). Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 193–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frederick, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 2005, 19, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galdi, S.; Arcuri, L.; Gawronski, B. Automatic mental associations predict future choices of undecided decision-makers. Science 2008, 321, 1100–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weiner, B. Judgments of Responsibility: A Foundation for A Theory of Social Conduct; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lau, S.; Hiemisch, A. Functional Freedom: A Psychological Model of Freedom in Decision-Making. Behav. Sci. 2017, 7, 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030041
Lau S, Hiemisch A. Functional Freedom: A Psychological Model of Freedom in Decision-Making. Behavioral Sciences. 2017; 7(3):41. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030041
Chicago/Turabian StyleLau, Stephan, and Anette Hiemisch. 2017. "Functional Freedom: A Psychological Model of Freedom in Decision-Making" Behavioral Sciences 7, no. 3: 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030041
APA StyleLau, S., & Hiemisch, A. (2017). Functional Freedom: A Psychological Model of Freedom in Decision-Making. Behavioral Sciences, 7(3), 41. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030041