Working Memory, Language Skills, and Autism Symptomatology
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Results and Discussion
2.1. Group Comparisons
HFA; n = 18 | TD Controls; n = 18 | F | p | h2p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Score | Z-Score | Score | Z-Score | ||||
Spatial WM: | |||||||
Fing Wind SS *** | 9 (2); 5–14 | −0.50 (0.93); −2.08–1.47 | 11 (2); 6–14 | 0.35 (0.73); −1.69–1.47 | 14.18 | <0.001 | 0.30 |
Complex Verbal WM: | |||||||
Let-Numb Seq SS † (n = 16 for HFA) | 10 (3); 5–17 | −0.27 (0.85); −1.76 – 1.87 | 11 (2); 9–16 | 0.16 (0.53); −0.55–1.57 | 3.09 | 0.09 | 0.09 |
Comp. Lang. Proc. RS * | 31 (16); 10–63 | −0.41 (0.98); −1.90- 1.41 | 44 (14); 16–63 | 0.34 (0.90); −1.62–1.82 | 4.88 | 0.03 | 0.13 |
Phonological WM: | |||||||
Nonword Rep. RS ** | 16 (2); 12–20 | −0.48 (1.08); −2.54–1.35 | 18 (1); 15–20 | 0.34 (0.54); −0.60–1.02 | 7.98 | 0.01 | 0.20 |
WM Composite a,*** | −0.44 (0.71); −1.38–1.48 | 0.29 (0.38); −0.33–0.93 | 15.44 | <0.001 | 0.32 |
Impaired | Low Average | Average | High Average | Superior | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
TD: HFA | TD: HFA | TD: HFA | TD: HFA | TD: HFA | |
Spatial WM: Finger Windows | 1:2 | 0:7 | 9:6 | 7:2 | 1:1 |
Complex Verbal WM: Letter-Number Sequencing | 0:2 | 0:2 | 12:10 | 5:1 | 1:3 |
Total | 1:4 | 0:9 | 21:16 | 12:3 | 2:4 |
2.2. Regression Analyses
Language a | SRS b | ADOS b | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 (NVIQ) β | Step 2 (WM) β | Step 1 (NVIQ) β | Step 2 (WM) β | Step 1 (NVIQ) β | Step 2 (WM) β | ||
HFA Group Only; n = 18 | |||||||
Constant | 100.990 *** | 101.221 *** | 100.596 *** | 93.752 *** | 4.520 | 4.973 | |
Stanford Binet NVIQ | 0.070 | 0.118 | −0.564 | −0.522 * | 0.504 | 0.213 | |
WM Composite | - | 0.601 * | - | −0.634 * | - | −0.616 ** | |
R2 | 0.005 | 0.364 | 0.318 | 0.719 | 0.095 | 0.465 | |
F | 0.060 | 3.145 † | 3.271 | 7.682* | 1.477 | 5.658 * |
2.3. Discussion
3. Experimental Section
3.1. Participants
HFA n = 18 (16 boys) | TD Controls n = 18 (14 boys) | χ2 or F | p | η2p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 12 (3); 8–17 | 13 (2); 8–17 | 0.602 | 0.44 | 0.02 | |
Full Scale IQ a | 105 (10); 94–127 | 104 (10); 88–127 | 0.11 | 0.75 | 0.01 | |
Verbal IQ | 10 (3); 6–17 | 10 (2); 6–14 | <0.001 | 1.00 | <0.001 | |
Nonverbal IQ | 12 (3); 7–17 | 11 (2); 8–16 | 0.65 | 0.43 | 0.02 | |
CELF-4 b | 105 (13); 82–126 | 110 (7); 97–123 | 1.75 | 0.20 | 0.05 | |
PPVT-3 c | 112 (13); 83–131 | 116 (12); 100–147 | 0.55 | 0.46 | 0.02 | |
EVT-2 d | 107 (15); 81–136 | 109 (16); 84–139 | 0.10 | 0.75 | 0.01 | |
SRS e | 81 (7); 71–90 | 40 (11); 0–57 | 104.29 | <0.001 | 0.80 |
3.2. Measures
4. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
References and Notes
- Hill, E.L. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Dev. Rev. 2004, 24, 189–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rumsey, J.M. Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1985, 15, 23–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanders, J.; Johnson, K.; Garavan, H.; Gill, M.; Gallagher, L. A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autism spectrum disorders: Attention, inhibition and cogntive flexibility. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2008, 2, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eigsti, I.M. Executive functions. In Neuropsychology of Autism; Fein, D., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 185–204. [Google Scholar]
- Landa, R.J.; Goldberg, M.C. Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: A continuum of performance. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 557–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennetto, L.; Pennington, B.F.; Rogers, S.J. Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 1816–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawson, G.; Meltzoff, A.N.; Osterling, J.; Rinaldi, J. Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism. Child Dev. 1998, 69, 1276–1285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luna, B.; Minshew, N.J.; Garver, K.E.; Lazar, N.A.; Thulborn, K.R.; Eddy, W.F.; Sweeney, J.A. Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An FMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology 2002, 59, 834–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, D.L.; Goldstein, G.; Minshew, N.J. Impaired memory for faces and social scenes in autism: Clinical implications of memory dysfunction. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2005, 20, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corbett, B.A.; Constantine, L.J.; Hendren, R.; Rocke, D.; Ozonoff, S. Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 166, 210–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, J.; Jarrold, C.; Henry, L. Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1996, 37, 673–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffith, E.M.; Pennington, B.F.; Wehner, E.A.; Rogers, S.J. Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Dev. 1999, 70, 817–832. [Google Scholar]
- Dawson, G.; Munson, J.; Estes, A.; Osterling, J.; McPartland, J.; Toth, K.; Carver, L.; Abbott, R. Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 345–358. [Google Scholar]
- Whitehouse, A.J.O.; Barry, J.G.; Bishop, D.V.M. Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? J. Commun. Disord. 2008, 41, 319–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hooper, S.R.; Poon, K.K.; Marcus, L.; Fine, C. Neuropsychological characteristics of school-age children with high-functioning autism: Performance on the nepsy. Child Neuropsychol. 2006, 12, 299–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, D.L.; Goldstein, G.; Carpenter, P.A.; Minshew, N.J. Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 747–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, D.L.; Goldstein, G.; Minshew, N.J. The profile of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology 2006, 20, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. Working Memory; Claredon Press: Oxford, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Bayliss, D.M.; Jarrold, C.; Gunn, D.M.; Baddeley, A.D. The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 71–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daneman, M.; Carpenter, P.A. Individual differences in working memory and reading. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 1980, 19, 450–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boucher, J.; Mayes, A.; Bigham, S. Memory in autistic spectrum disorder. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 458–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ozonoff, S.; Strayer, D.L. Further evidence of intact working memory in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2001, 31, 257–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paul, R. Communication. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders; Cohen, D.J., Donnellan, A.M., Paul, R., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1987; pp. 61–84. [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald, M.C.; Christiansen, M.H. Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychol. Rev. 2002, 109, 35–54; discussion 55–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Associations among WM tasks were also explored. Given the lack of a task by group interaction, we conducted correlational analyses within groups for the WM measures. No correlations were significant, with all p’s > 0.09. This likely reflects the sample size as well as differences in task modality and complexity. The apparent similarity of performance across WM tasks led to the determination that using a WM composite variable would be the most conservative approach for the regression analysis in the following section.
- Semel, E.; Wiig, E.H.; Secord, W.A. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th ed.; Harcourt Assessment, Inc.: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Constantino, J. The Social Responsiveness Scale; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; DiLavore, P.; Risi, S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Poirier, M.; Martin, J.S.; Gaigg, S.B.; Bowler, D.M. Short-term memory in autism spectrum disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2011, 120, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, D.; Happe, F.; Jarrold, C. Intact inner speech use in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a short-term memory task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Minshew, N.J.; Goldstein, G.; Siegel, D.J. Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1997, 3, 303–316. [Google Scholar]
- Cui, J.; Gao, D.; Chen, Y.; Zou, X.; Wang, Y. Working memory in early-school-age children with asperger's syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2010, 40, 958–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegel, D.J.; Minshew, N.J.; Goldstein, G. Wechsler iq profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1996, 26, 389–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Minshew, N.J.; Turner, C.A.; Goldstein, G. The application of short forms of the wechsler intelligence scales in adults and children with high functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 45–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joseph, R.M.; Steele, S.D.; Meyer, E.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: Failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory? Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1400–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.W.; Tuholski, S.W.; Laughlin, J.E.; Conway, A.R. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1999, 128, 309–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, D.M.; Bowler, D.M.; Jarrold, C. Inner speech is used to mediate short-term memory, but not planning, among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 2012, 24, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schuh, J.M.; Mirman, D.; Eigsti, I.M. Finding common ground: The impact of individual differences in working memory on referential communication for children and adults. Cogn. Sci. under review.
- Gioia, G.; Isquith, P.; Kenworthy, L.; Barton, R. Brief: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.: Lutz, FL, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Turner, M.A. Generating novel ideas: Fluency performance in high-functioning and learning disabled individuals with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1999, 40, 189–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shafritz, K.M.; Dichter, G.S.; Baranek, G.T.; Belger, A. The neural circuitry mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 974–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maes, J.H.; Eling, P.A.; Wezenberg, E.; Vissers, C.T.; Kan, C.C. Attentional set shifting in autism spectrum disorder: Differentiating between the role of perseveration, learned irrelevance, and novelty processing. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2011, 33, 210–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ozonoff, S. Reliability and validity of the wisconsin card sorting test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology 1995, 9, 491–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascualvaca, D.M.; Fantie, B.D.; Papageorgiou, M.; Mirsky, A.F. Attentional capacities in children with autism: Is there a general deficit in shifting focus? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1998, 28, 467–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; Le Couteur, A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1994, 24, 659–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roid, G. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, 5th ed.; Riverside Publishing: Itasca, IL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.; Dunn, L. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed.; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, K.T. Expressive Vocabulary Test; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Gaulin, C.A.; Campbell, T.F. Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Percept. Mot. Skills 1994, 79, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S.E.; Hitch, G.J.; Service, E.; Martin, A.J. Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Dev. Psychol. 1997, 33, 966–979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baltruschat, L.; Hasselhorn, M.; Tarbox, J.; Dixon, D.; Najdowski, A.; Mullins, R.; Gould, E. Further analysis of the effects of positive reinforcement on working memory in children with autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 855–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baltruschat, L.; Hasselhorn, M.; Tarbox, J.; Dixon, D.; Najdowski, A.; Mullins, R.; Gould, E. Addressing working memory in children with autism through behavioral intervention. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 267–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2012 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Share and Cite
Schuh, J.M.; Eigsti, I.-M. Working Memory, Language Skills, and Autism Symptomatology. Behav. Sci. 2012, 2, 207-218. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs2040207
Schuh JM, Eigsti I-M. Working Memory, Language Skills, and Autism Symptomatology. Behavioral Sciences. 2012; 2(4):207-218. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs2040207
Chicago/Turabian StyleSchuh, Jillian M., and Inge-Marie Eigsti. 2012. "Working Memory, Language Skills, and Autism Symptomatology" Behavioral Sciences 2, no. 4: 207-218. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs2040207
APA StyleSchuh, J. M., & Eigsti, I. -M. (2012). Working Memory, Language Skills, and Autism Symptomatology. Behavioral Sciences, 2(4), 207-218. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs2040207