Effects of Moral Elevation on Children’s Implicit and Explicit Prosociality: Evidence from Behavioral and Physiological Responses
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Moral Elevation
1.2. Autonomic Foundations of Moral Elevation
1.3. Developmental Perspectives on Moral Elevation and Prosociality
1.4. The Present Study
- (1)
- Moral elevation stimuli would elicit a pattern of autonomic coactivation, reflected in increased sympathetic nervous system activity (as measured by SCL) and increased parasympathetic influence (as indicated by HF-HRV), significantly differing from responses in both Joy and Neutral conditions.
- (2)
- Moral elevation would lead to higher levels of explicitly self-reported prosocial outcomes compared to Joy and Neutral conditions.
- (3)
- Children in the Moral Elevation condition would display a stronger implicit prosocial bias on the Implicit Association Test than those in the Joy or Neutral conditions.
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Materials
- (1)
- Emotional self-assessment
- (2)
- Children’s prosocial behavior
- (3)
- Emotionally induced videos
2.1.3. Design and Procedure
2.1.4. Physiological Measures
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Differences in Subjective Emotional Experiences
2.2.2. Differences in Physiological Arousal Indicators
2.2.3. Differences in Prosocial Behavior Scores
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Materials
3.1.3. Design and Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Subjective Emotional Experiences
3.2.2. Prosocial Behavior
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Step | Task Description | (Left Upper Side) Label (Right Upper Side) | Presentation Materials |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Attribute Word Discrimination | Self-Other | Self or Other Vocabulary |
2 | Initial Target Word Discrimination | Altruism-Nonaltruism | Altruism or Nonaltruism Vocabulary |
3 | Compatible Task (Practice) | Altruism+Self-Nonaltruism + Other | All Vocabulary |
4 | Compatible Task (Test) | Altruism+Self-Nonaltruism + Other | All Vocabulary |
5 | Reverse Target Word Discrimination | Nonaltruism-Altruism | Nonaltruism or Altruism Vocabulary |
6 | Incompatible Task (Practice) | Altruism+Other-Nonaltruism + Self | All Vocabulary |
7 | Incompatible Task (Test) | Altruism+Other-Nonaltruism + Self | All Vocabulary |
References
- Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aydinli, A., Bender, M., Chasiotis, A., Cemalcilar, Z., & Van de Vijver, F. J. (2014). When does self-reported prosocial motivation predict helping? The moderating role of implicit prosocial motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 645–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bajović, M., & Rizzo, K. (2020). Meta-moral cognition: Bridging the gap among adolescents’ moral thinking, moral emotions and moral actions. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 26(1), 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Banerjee, R. (2002). Children’s understanding of self-presentational behavior: Links with mental-state reasoning and the attribution of embarrassment. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 378–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: A transdiagnostic biomarker of emotion dysregulation and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 3, 43–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cai, H. J. (2003). A review on implicit association test. Advances in Psychological Science, 11(3), 339–344. [Google Scholar]
- Cameron, C. D., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Mindfulness facets predict helping behavior and distinct helping-related emotions. Mindfulness, 6(5), 1211–1218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlo, G., Hausmann, A., Christiansen, S., & Randall, B. A. (2003). Sociocognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of prosocial tendencies for adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 23(1), 107–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chai, Q., Yin, J., Shen, M., & He, J. (2024). Act generously when others do so: Majority influence on young children’s sharing behavior. Developmental Science, 27(3), e13472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Christner, N., Pletti, C., & Paulus, M. (2022). How does the moral self-concept relate to prosocial behaviour? Investigating the role of emotions and consistency preference. Cognition and Emotion, 36(5), 894–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cox, K. S. (2010). Elevation predicts domain-specific volunteerism 3 months later. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 5(5), 333–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cvencek, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Measuring implicit attitudes of 4-year-olds: The preschool implicit association test. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 187–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denham, S. A. (2006). Social-emotional competence as support for school readiness: What is it and how do we assess it? Early Education and Development, 17(1), 57–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Witte, N. A. J., Sutterlin, S., Braet, C., & Mueller, S. C. (2016). Getting to the heart of emotion regulation in youth: The role of interoceptive sensitivity, heart rate variability, and parental psychopathology. PLoS ONE, 11(10), e0164615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diessner, R., Pohling, R., Stacy, S., & Güsewell, A. (2018). Trait appreciation of beauty: A story of love, transcendence, and inquiry. Review of General Psychology, 22(4), 377–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 248–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- El-Sheikh, M., Kouros, C. D., Erath, S., Cummings, E. M., Keller, P., & Staton, L. (2009). Marital conflict and children’s externalizing behavior: Interactions between parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system activity. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 74(1), vii. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eslinger, P. J., Anders, S., Ballarini, T., Boutros, S., Krach, S., Mayer, A. V., Moll, J., Newton, T. L., Schroeter, M. L., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Raber, J., Sullivan, G. B., Swain, J. E., Lowe, L., & Zahn, R. (2021). The neuroscience of social feelings: Mechanisms of adaptive social functioning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 128, 592–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeman, D., Aquino, K., & McFerran, B. (2009). Overcoming beneficiary race as an impediment to charitable donations: Social dominance orientation, the experience of moral elevation, and donation behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(1), 72–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2008). Implicit and explicit processes in social cognition. Neuron, 60(3), 503–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibhardt, S., Hepach, R., & Henderson, A. M. E. (2024). Observing prosociality and talent: The emotional characteristics and behavioral outcomes of elevation and admiration in 6.5- to 8.5-year-old children. Frontiers in Psychology, 15, 1392331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidt, J. (2000). The Positive emotion of elevation. Prevention & Treatment, 3(1), 3c. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haidt, J. (2003). Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In C. L. M. Keyes, & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 275–289). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- He, N., & Zhu, Y. L. (2016). Self-love and other-love: Research on the relationships among narcissism, empathy and implicit altruism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(2), 199–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hepach, R., & Tomasello, M. (2020). Young children show positive emotions when seeing someone get the help they deserve. Cognitive Development, 56, 100935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children are intrinsically motivated to see others helped. Psychological Science, 23(9), 967–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jiang, D., Wang, X. R., Fu, L., & Zhou, R. L. (2008). A study on implicit altruistic behavior. Psychological Science, 31(1), 79–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krettenauer, T., Campbell, S., & Hertz, S. (2013). Moral emotions and the development of the moral self in childhood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(2), 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lai, C. K., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). Moral elevation reduces prejudice against gay men. Cognition and Emotion, 28(5), 781–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, H. Y. (2012). The relationship between the status of being bullied, prosocial value orientation and interpersonal satisfaction of pupils in primary school [Master’s thesis, Sichuan Normal University]. [Google Scholar]
- Mastromatteo, L. Y., Girardi, P., Miller, J. G., & Scrimin, S. (2024). Moderate cardiac vagal tone is associated with more cooperation in children. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 201, 112371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGuire, A. P., Hayden, C., Frankfurt, S. B., Kurz, A. S., Anderson, A. R., Howard, B. A. N., & Szabo, Y. Z. (2020). Social engagement early in the US COVID-19 crisis: Exploring social support and prosocial behavior between those with and without depression or anxiety in an online sample. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 39(10), 923–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, J. G., Kahle, S., & Hastings, P. D. (2017). Moderate baseline vagal tone predicts greater prosociality in children. Developmental Psychology, 53(2), 274–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monroe, A. (2020). Moral elevation: Indications of functional integration with welfare trade-off calibration and estimation mechanisms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(4), 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pergantis, P., Bamicha, V., Doulou, A., Christou, A. I., Bardis, N., Skianis, C., & Drigas, A. (2025). Assistive and emerging technologies to detect and reduce neurophysiological stress and anxiety in children and adolescents with autism and sensory processing disorders: A systematic review. Technologies, 13(4), 144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child (1st ed.). Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. (1964). Part I: Cognitive development in children: Piaget development and learning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2(3), 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piper, W. T., Saslow, L. R., & Saturn, S. R. (2015). Autonomic and prefrontal events during moral elevation. Biological Psychology, 108, 51–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology, 32(4), 301–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porges, S. W., & Furman, S. A. (2011). The early development of the autonomic nervous system provides a neural platform for social behaviour: A polyvagal perspective. Infant and Child Development, 20(1), 106–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schnall, S., Roper, J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2010). Elevation leads to altruistic behavior. Psychological Science, 21(3), 315–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S. H., & Howard, J. A. (1984). Internalized values as motivators of altruism. In Development and maintenance of prosocial behavior (pp. 229–255). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smetana, J. G. (2006). Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies and variations in children’s moral and social judgments. In Handbook of moral development (pp. 119–153). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(38), 8223–8231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vecchio, G. M., Zava, F., Cattelino, E., Zuffianò, A., & Pallini, S. (2023). Children’s prosocial and aggressive behaviors: The role of emotion regulation and sympathy. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 89, 101598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, P. F., Huang, Y. X., & Luo, Y. J. (2010). Establishment and assessment of a native Chinese affective video system. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 24(7), 551–554. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, L. (2012). The development of emotional competence in lower primary school students and its relationship with prosocial behavior [Master’s thesis, Shaanxi Normal University]. [Google Scholar]
- Yarritu, I., Méndez, I., & Idoiaga-Mondragón, N. (2025). Analysis of the moral elevation experience in adolescents. Current Psychology, 44(10), 8736–8750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Moral Elevation | Joy | Neutral | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Reactivity | Baseline | Reactivity | Baseline | Reactivity | |
Basic emotion | ||||||
Peace | 4.48 ± 1.36 | 3.00 ± 1.59 | 4.44 ± 1.62 | 3.09 ± 1.36 | 4.47 ± 1.31 | 4.62 ± 1.60 |
Joy | 3.77 ± 2.05 | 1.71 ± 1.37 | 3.71 ± 1.78 | 5.00 ± 1.74 | 3.58 ± 1.60 | 4.00 ± 1.39 |
Fear | 1.23 ± 0.50 | 1.13 ± 0.50 | 1.09 ± 0.29 | 1.03 ± 0.17 | 1.56 ± 1.24 | 1.32 ± 0.88 |
Anger | 1.19 ± 0.75 | 1.10 ± 0.30 | 1.15 ± 0.61 | 1.10 ± 0.29 | 1.12 ± 0.69 | 1.12 ± 0.54 |
Sadness | 1.32 ± 0.95 | 2.32 ± 1.62 | 1.03 ± 0.17 | 1.03 ± 0.17 | 1.09 ± 0.38 | 1.24 ± 0.74 |
Disgust | 1.07 ± 0.36 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.15 ± 0.61 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.18 ± 0.72 | 1.06 ± 0.24 |
Moral elevation | 1.72 ± 1.07 | 3.85 ± 1.52 | ||||
Inspired | 1.97 ± 1.38 | 2.84 ± 1.90 | ||||
Moved | 1.74 ± 1.39 | 4.61 ± 2.03 | ||||
Touched | 1.94 ± 1.59 | 4.16 ± 2.07 | ||||
Admiration | 1.52 ± 1.21 | 4.45 ± 2.28 | ||||
Uplifted | 1.45 ± 1.23 | 3.19 ± 1.94 | ||||
Physiological arousal | ||||||
SCL (µs) | 8.73 ± 6.28 | 10.28 ± 7.13 | 9.27 ± 4.55 | 10.56 ± 4.49 | 8.32 ± 4.89 | 8.94 ± 5.37 |
HR (BPM) | 89.90 ± 9.68 | 92.37 ± 9.62 | 88.88 ± 8.50 | 90.11 ± 8.48 | 86.79 ± 10.24 | 86.77 ± 10.44 |
HF-HRV | 3.82 ± 1.06 | 4.36 ± 1.17 | 4.36 ± 1.30 | 4.08 ± 1.81 | 4.43 ± 1.21 | 4.27 ± 1.12 |
Moral Elevation | Joy | Neutral | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Reactivity | Baseline | Reactivity | Baseline | Reactivity | |
Basic emotion | ||||||
Peace | 4.65 ± 1.43 | 3.23 ± 1.54 | 5.03 ± 1.56 | 3.34 ± 1.58 | 4.45 ± 1.38 | 4.48 ± 1.57 |
Joy | 4.13 ± 1.89 | 2.32 ± 1.58 | 4.25 ± 1.76 | 5.34 ± 1.60 | 3.52 ± 1.81 | 3.97 ± 1.80 |
Fear | 1.39 ± 0.92 | 1.36 ± 1.17 | 1.09 ± 0.30 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.28 ± 0.65 | 1.28 ± 0.80 |
Anger | 1.36 ± 0.88 | 1.23 ± 0.34 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.34 ± 0.74 | 1.10 ± 0.56 |
Sadness | 1.42 ± 0.99 | 1.87 ± 1.41 | 1.09 ± 0.53 | 1.13 ± 0.55 | 1.10 ± 0.41 | 1.31 ± 0.81 |
Disgust | 1.42 ± 1.21 | 1.26 ± 1.23 | 1.03 ± 0.18 | 1.00 ± 0.00 | 1.72 ± 0.76 | 1.07 ± 0.26 |
Moral elevation | 2.21 ± 1.30 | 4.35 ± 1.41 | ||||
Inspired | 2.68 ± 1.56 | 3.65 ± 1.45 | ||||
Moved | 1.90 ± 1.56 | 4.65 ± 1.98 | ||||
Touched | 2.23 ± 1.61 | 4.68 ± 1.90 | ||||
Admiration | 2.26 ± 1.69 | 4.84 ± 1.99 | ||||
Uplifted | 1.97 ± 1.49 | 3.94 ± 1.79 |
Moral Elevation (n = 31) | Joy (n = 32) | Neutral (n = 29) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RT (ms) | log | RT (ms) | log | RT (ms) | log | |
Compatible | 939 ± 274 | 6.81 ± 0.27 | 1007 ± 217 | 6.90 ± 0.20 | 996 ± 223 | 6.88 ± 0.20 |
Incompatible | 1387 ± 272 | 7.22 ± 0.19 | 1287 ± 276 | 7.14 ± 0.21 | 1286 ± 291 | 7.14 ± 0.22 |
IAT | 447 ± 286 | 0.41 ± 0.25 | 247 ± 326 | 0.25 ± 0.25 | 291 ± 302 | 0.25 ± 0.26 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wang, Q.; Zhou, X.; Xun, L. Effects of Moral Elevation on Children’s Implicit and Explicit Prosociality: Evidence from Behavioral and Physiological Responses. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1246. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091246
Wang Q, Zhou X, Xun L. Effects of Moral Elevation on Children’s Implicit and Explicit Prosociality: Evidence from Behavioral and Physiological Responses. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1246. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091246
Chicago/Turabian StyleWang, Qin, Xia Zhou, and Lei Xun. 2025. "Effects of Moral Elevation on Children’s Implicit and Explicit Prosociality: Evidence from Behavioral and Physiological Responses" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1246. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091246
APA StyleWang, Q., Zhou, X., & Xun, L. (2025). Effects of Moral Elevation on Children’s Implicit and Explicit Prosociality: Evidence from Behavioral and Physiological Responses. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1246. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091246