Next Article in Journal
Integrated Cognitive Processing Therapy and Relapse Prevention for Co-Occurring PTSD and Alcohol Use Disorder: A Case Series Examining Acceptability and Initial Efficacy
Previous Article in Journal
A Longitudinal Examination of Stress, Affect Dynamics, and Alcohol-Related Outcomes Across Emerging Adulthood
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotion Socialization Under One Roof: How Parental Response Patterns Shape Adolescent Emotional Well-Being

1
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
2
School of Nursing, Hubei University of Chinese Medicine, Wuhan 430065, China
3
Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10012, USA
4
Department of Psychology, Shaoguan University, Shaoguan 512158, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(8), 999; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15080999
Submission received: 16 April 2025 / Revised: 1 July 2025 / Accepted: 15 July 2025 / Published: 22 July 2025

Abstract

(1) Background: This study used latent profile analysis (LPA) to investigate family patterns of paternal and maternal responses to adolescents’ discrete emotions (happiness, sadness, and anger) and examined the relationship between these profiles and demographic factors, as well as adolescents’ emotion adjustment (emotion regulation and depressive symptoms). (2) Methods: A sample of 666 adolescents reported parental responses and their emotional adjustment; their mothers provided family information. (3) Results: (a) The LPA identified four profiles for adolescent happiness, including high enhancing but low dampening and neglect from both parents (Consistent Supportive); low enhancing but high dampening and neglect from both parents (Consistent Unsupportive); low to moderate scores on each response from both parents (Consistent Disengaging); and high maternal dampening and neglect but relatively low scores on the paternal response (Inconsistent). There were two profiles for sadness (Consistent Supportive, Consistent Unsupportive) and three for anger (Consistent Supportive, Consistent Unsupportive, Consistent Disengaging). (b) Parents with boys, higher incomes, better education, and greater marital satisfaction were likely to be classified into the Consistent Supportive profile across emotions. (c) When adolescents perceived their parents with the Consistent Supportive profile, they would show the best emotional adjustment; while for parents with the Inconsistent profile (for happiness) and the Consistent Unsupportive profile, the adolescents had the poorest outcome. Interestingly, adolescents who perceived their parents as fitting the Consistent Disengaging profile (especially for anger) exhibited comparatively less adverse adjustment. (4) Implications: A person-centered approach highlights different patterns of emotion socialization, underscores the importance of fostering parental cooperation and supportive responses to adolescents’ happiness, and suggests that joint disengagement from anger may promote healthier emotional development.

1. Introduction

Parental emotion socialization (ES) influences how children experience, express, and regulate emotions, thus contributing to child socio-emotional development (Eisenberg et al., 1998a; Eisenberg, 2020; Morris et al., 2007). Adolescence is a particularly critical period for emotional development, marked by heightened vulnerability to emotional problems such as depression and emotion dysregulation. Consequently, understanding how parents’ responses to adolescents’ emotions shape their psychological adjustment has become a significant area of inquiry. Existing research on ES has predominantly focused on parental reactions to negative emotions (e.g., sadness, anger) while overlooking their reactions to positive emotions (e.g., happiness; Fredrickson, 1998, 2013). Furthermore, most studies have centered on maternal socialization practices, neglecting the unique and complementary roles of fathers (Cassano et al., 2007; McElwain et al., 2007). This limited scope leaves a significant gap in understanding how parents jointly influence adolescents’ emotional outcomes. To address these gaps, this study conducted latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify patterns of parental reactions to discrete emotions in two-parent households and explored the key correlations informed by previous research, including demographics (i.e., adolescents’ gender, mothers’ education, marital satisfaction, and family income) and outcomes (i.e., emotion regulation and depressive symptoms).

1.1. Parental Emotion Socialization

Parental emotion socialization refers to the parenting practices that reflect parental beliefs and goals concerning their children’s emotional experience, expression, and regulation. Such practices encompass three key components: parents’ reactions to children’s emotions, discussions of emotions, and parental expression of emotions (Eisenberg et al., 1998b). Parents may react to children’s negative emotions in various ways, which can be deemed as supportive or unsupportive. Supportive reactions, such as emotion coaching, convey parents’ accepting and encouraging attitudes toward children’s emotional displays. In addition to emotion coaching, supportive parental reactions may be presented through more specific and diverse pathways, such as praising, offering comfort (i.e., reward), and engaging in joint problem-solving. It has been documented that those supportive reactions are associated with fewer children internalizing problems (Klimes-Dougan et al., 2007; O’Neal & Magai, 2005), greater positive emotion experience, and better emotion expression ability (Garside, 2004). In contrast, parents’ unsupportive practices, such as dismissing or neglecting reactions, can exacerbate emotional dysregulation and psychological distress (Garside & Klimes-Dougan, 2002; Lunkenheimer et al., 2007). Nevertheless, previous studies on parental reactions to children’s emotions were predominantly conducted with samples from Western countries, with a limited number of studies evaluating it in other cultural groups (i.e., China).
Individuals from collectivistic cultures, driven by values emphasizing social harmony, often employ emotion restraint strategies more frequently than those from individualistic cultures (Matsumoto et al., 2008). In China, the Analects proverb “To restrain oneself and return to propriety” (ke ji fu li; Analects 12-1) encapsulates Confucian ideals of self-control and harmony, which underpin ES practices aimed at relational cohesion (Ding et al., 2024; Luo et al., 2013). Among these culturally influenced practices, two parental reactions have particularly attracted attention and debate: temporarily withholding communication (especially regarding children’s anger; Raval & Martini, 2011) and minimizing the seriousness of emotional situations (Shi et al., 2024a; Tao et al., 2010). Although Chinese parents often display these unsupportive reactions more frequently than Western parents (Yang et al., 2020), emerging evidence suggests they are not necessarily detrimental to children in Eastern contexts. For instance, parental minimization showed no negative link to Chinese children’s adjustment (Tao et al., 2010), even correlating positively with children’s functioning (Shi et al., 2024a). Similarly, temporary withholding of communication (i.e., not talking to the child for a brief period) occurs more frequently in well-adjusted rather than maladjusted families (Raval & Martini, 2011). Scholars suggest that Chinese children, influenced by Confucian filial piety, may interpret these parental strategies positively—as expressions of parental care that provide opportunities for self-reflection, emotional recovery. These strategies might be particularly valuable for early adolescents due to their rapidly developing cognitive and reasoning abilities. Thus, recognizing these cultural nuances is essential for understanding how parental reactions influence adolescents’ adjustment. We therefore surveyed Chinese seventh graders to examine associations between culturally embedded parental responses to adolescents’ emotions and to explore whether such reactions would show weaker negative associations with adjustment within this cultural context.

1.2. Socialization of the Different Emotions

From a functionalist perspective, each emotion serves a distinct social function (Barett & Campos, 1987), and parents tend to tailor their responses accordingly (O’Neal & Magai, 2005). However, most studies have conceptualized negative emotions broadly, overlooking distinctions among discrete emotions. Anger typically arises when obstacles impede goal attainment and receives fewer supportive responses; whereas sadness, as a nonhostile emotion, follows perceived loss and evokes empathy and nurturing from parents (Tomkins, 2008). Empirical studies demonstrate that parents provide more empathy, comfort, and practical support for sadness than for anger (O’Neal & Magai, 2005; Shipman & Zeman, 2001; Zeman & Shipman, 1997). This pattern is especially pronounced in collectivist cultures that prioritize harmony; for example, Indian mothers regard expressions of anger as less acceptable than sadness (Raval et al., 2016; Raval & Martini, 2009). Thus, this study examines Chinese adolescents’ negative discrete emotions, hypothesizing that parental responses will be less supportive toward anger compared to sadness.
Increasingly, scholars recognize how crucial the socialization of positive emotion is for development. Parents’ enhancing/supportive behaviors generally relate to better emotion regulation and fewer depressive symptoms (Katz et al., 2014; Shi et al., 2024a; Moran et al., 2018; Yap et al., 2008; Yi et al., 2016), whereas dampening/unsupportive responses do not, particularly among Chinese adolescents (Shi et al., 2024a; Song et al., 2019). Although most work focused on the general positive emotions, Russell’s (1980) circumplex model delineates emotions by valence (pleasantness) and arousal (activation). Happiness (high pleasure, high arousal) and anger (low pleasure, high arousal) both share heightened arousal and potential for interpersonal contagion, whereas sadness (low pleasure, low arousal) differs substantially.
Building on this framework, we speculate that patterns of parental response to happiness and anger may be more similar due to their shared high arousal, whereas responses to happiness may differ more markedly from those toward sadness. Nonetheless, happiness remains distinct because it is often viewed as a desirable internal state and elicits emotion upregulation strategies that broaden children’s thought–action repertoires as described by broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998). Laboratory studies further illustrate that parents respond to children’s happiness with greater tonal warmth, emotional coaching, and flexibility (i.e., employing a broader range of strategies) than they do to anger or distress (Suveg et al., 2008; Lunkenheimer et al., 2007, 2012). Thus, the current study examines ES by including happiness alongside sadness and anger, and we expect that parental reactions to happiness will be notably diverse and spontaneous, as they may not require the heightened vigilance typically associated with negative emotions.

1.3. Person-Centered Approach to Parental Emotion Socialization in Families

Traditionally, ES research has focused on mothers as primary caregivers, but recent studies highlight the important yet distinct role of fathers. Fathers tend to respond to children’s negative emotions with more minimizing, neglecting, or punishing behaviors, while mothers are generally more constructive (Cassano et al., 2007; Eisenberg et al., 1996). In Chinese cultural contexts, fathers are often seen as strict and emotionally reserved (Li, 2021); Wang et al. (2019) likewise found that paternal disengagement predominates in China, whereas a cross-cultural study reported particularly high levels of unsupportive responses among Chinese mothers (Song et al., 2019). Despite lower paternal involvement in children’s emotional lives, fathers’ reactions significantly influence children’s emotional arousal (Van Lissa et al., 2019), especially among depressed adolescents (Allen et al., 2012). Parental responses vary by emotion type: Hooven et al. (1995) observed that fathers attend more to children’s anger, while mothers were more concerned with sadness. Unsupportive reactions in these respective domains may thus exert stronger effects. Consistent with this, Sanders et al. (2015) found that paternal unsupportive responses to anger, rather than sadness, predicted higher levels of children’s depressive symptoms, whereas the reverse was true for mothers. These findings underscore the importance of considering both parent gender and emotion type in the process of ES.
However, from a family systems perspective, mothers and fathers jointly form a subsystem where their combined emotional responses influence child outcomes. Yu et al. (2015) explored this dynamic by creating an interaction term and found that Chinese children benefited most when both parents responded supportively; conversely, divergent parental reactions (a highly supportive father paired with a less supportive mother) were linked to more internalizing problems. Although this study employed a variable-centered approach, it highlighted the critical importance of parental consistency in emotion socialization, expanding prior research by examining how joint parental patterns influence adolescent development.
Recently, researchers have increasingly adopted person-centered approaches (i.e., cluster analysis) to better capture family heterogeneity by identifying distinct patterns of parental responses. Miller-Slough et al. (2018) identified three ES classes among U.S. two-parent families: two classes reflecting parental consistency—“supportive” (high support from both parents) and “unsupportive” (high nonsupport from both parents)—and one labeled “father dominant,” reflecting divergences between parents, with high paternal but low maternal involvement. Similarly, Zhu and Dunsmore (2023) classified Chinese families into five patterns combining parental ES and family functioning, highlighting consistent (both supportive or unsupportive) and discrepant classes (e.g., engaged father/mother). Both studies found that children with consistently supportive parents showed better adjustment, those with unsupportive parents fared worst, and the discrepancy pattern had intermediate outcomes. Childrearing agreement and parental encouragement foster a stable emotional environment for secure attachment (Feinberg, 2003). Therefore, we anticipated that Chinese parents would also exhibit these consistent and discrepant ES patterns, with consistently supportive responses being particularly beneficial for child adjustment outcomes.
Moreover, we observed a “balance/diffuse” pattern first identified by Zhu and Dunsmore (2023) in Chinese two-parent households and also detected by Wang et al. (2019) in a Chinese father sample (where it was labeled “disengaged” via LPA). This profile is characterized by uniformly low levels of emotional responsiveness—reflecting relative passivity on both parents’ part, or especially the father’s—and appears common in Chinese families. Interestingly, both studies found no differences in children’s emotional adjustment between the disengaging and supportive profiles. Some researchers have attributed this to cultural norms of emotional restraint, whereby less-harsh unsupportive reactions (e.g., withholding communication or minimization) are not linked to poorer child outcomes, as mentioned before. Accordingly, we expected that this disengaged emotion socialization pattern would emerge in our sample and that adolescents exposed to it would demonstrate levels of emotion regulation and depressive symptoms comparable to those of peers with supportive parents.
Although person-centered studies (i.e., cluster analysis, LPA) have advanced our understanding of ES, they focus on broad negative emotions and rarely address positive ones like happiness. Moreover, predictors of parental ES patterns, including parents’ (i.e., marital quality and socioeconomic status) and children’s characteristics (i.e., gender), remain inadequately explored. Specifically, King et al. (2023) linked interparental conflict with a dismissive parenting profile, but marital satisfaction has been overlooked. Socioeconomic status shows mixed results: while higher family income is linked to parents’ active engagement style (“hyper-engaged”; Sosa-Hernandez et al., 2020), other studies did not find any significant relationships (King et al., 2023). Few studies have included child gender as a factor, though some empirical studies using traditional analyses suggest that parents encourage sadness in girls and tolerate more anger in boys (Chaplin et al., 2005; Garside & Klimes-Dougan, 2002). This study thus used adolescent-reported ES and explored the role of parental characteristics and child gender in ES patterns.

1.4. The Current Study

The present study drew on adolescents’ perceptions and used LPA to explore (1) family patterns of Chinese fathers’ and mothers’ socialization practices regarding adolescents’ happiness, sadness, and anger; (2) associations between these profiles and demographic variables (i.e., child’s gender, mother’s education, marital satisfaction, and family income); and (3) links between profile membership and adolescent adjustment (emotion regulation and depressive symptoms). Given the paucity of research on discrete emotion socialization and parents’ joint contributions, we took an exploratory stance and formulated the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1:
Profile enumeration. At least four parental emotion-socialization profiles would emerge: three consistent (supportive, unsupportive, disengaged) and one discrepant pattern in which mothers and fathers adopt divergent strategies (i.e., a supportive father with an unsupportive mother, or a disengaged father with an engaged mother). Further, the consistent profiles would emerge across all emotional conditions, whereas the discrepant profile would be most pronounced during happiness socialization.
Hypothesis 2:
Demographic correlates. Parents of daughters (especially in sadness contexts), as well as those with higher income and education, would be more likely to belong to the Consistent Supportive profile.
Hypothesis 3:
Adjustment differences. Compared with other profiles, adolescents from families with consistently supportive parents would demonstrate superior emotion regulation and the lowest levels of depressive symptoms.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study included 666 adolescents from a middle school in Guangdong Province, China, and their mothers (n = 513). The discrepancy between the number of adolescents and mothers in our study is primarily attributed to some mothers not returning their questionnaires promptly due to various reasons, such as time constraints or lower educational levels. Adolescents consisted of 7th-grade students, including 362 boys and 304 girls, with an average age of 13.16 years. The monthly income of most families (54.69%) was CNY 6000 or less, and nearly half (49.80%) of mothers had an education level of junior high school or below. Details for demographic information can be seen in Table 1.

2.2. Procedures

This study collaborated with a middle school where teachers introduced the research and invited students and their fathers and mothers to participate. Parents signed the Parent/Guardian and Adolescent Informed Consent Form if they agreed to both their own and their adolescent’s participation in the survey. After obtaining approval from the parents and schools, trained research assistants distributed the informed assent and monitored the completion of adolescent questionnaires (i.e., paternal and maternal response to discrete emotion, emotion regulation, and depressive symptoms) in classrooms, which took approximately 30 min. Adolescents then took home questionnaires for their parents to complete; to maximize sample size and data completeness, we used demographic data reported by mothers. The protocol was approved by the Shaoguan University Ethics Committee, and participants received a small gift upon returning fully completed surveys.

2.3. Measurements

2.3.1. Parental Reactions to Adolescents’ Happiness, Sadness, and Anger

We used the 90-item Emotions as a Child Negative and Positive Scales, Version 2 (EAC2; Garside, 2004), and the happiness, sadness, and anger subscales were included (30 items each). Adolescents rated maternal and paternal responses to their emotions on a 1 (never) to 5 (always) scale. Except for interchanging “mother” with “father,” the wording of the items did not vary. After obtaining email authorization from the original developers, the English version of the EAC2 was translated using the back-translation technique.
The original version of the scale comprised five dimensions: reward (comfort or encouragement), magnify (intensifying expressions), punish (derogation), override (providing distraction to redirect attention), and neglect (ignoring). Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) showed a three-factor model for each subscale, and items and the structure were shown in Table 2. One item (“not like my being sad”) was dropped due to a low loading (<0.30). Cronbach’s alpha showed good reliability for all factors except sadness magnify (0.54), likely due to its two-item composition. This factor was retained for analysis. Guided by Gottman’s conceptualization (Gottman et al., 1996), the factors of the sadness subscale were labeled “coaching (comfort and soothe),” “dismissing (ignoring and derogating),” and “magnifying (being distressed).” Similarly, the anger subscale factors were labeled “coaching,” “dismissing,” and “neglect.” For the happiness subscale, the factors were labeled “enhancing (acceptance),” “dampening (suppressing),” and “neglect (ignoring)” following prior studies (Shi et al., 2024b).

2.3.2. Marital Satisfaction

Mothers rated their marital satisfaction on a 0–70 visual analog scale (0 = extremely unsatisfied; 70 = extremely satisfied; Locke & Wallace, 1959).

2.3.3. Emotion Regulation

Adolescents reported their emotion regulation ability using the Children’s Emotion Management Scale: Sadness and Anger Scales (Zeman et al., 2001). Each subscale comprises three dimensions (i.e., coping, inhibition, and dysregulation), and items are rated on a scale from 1 (hardly ever) to 3 (often). In the current study, the emotion coping dimension was included, with five items for sadness (i.e., “I stay calm and don’t let sad things get to me”) and four items for anger (i.e., “When I’m angry, I stay calm and keep my cool”). Internal consistency was satisfactory (Cronbach’s α = 0.83 for sadness; α = 0.86 for anger), and the two coping scores (r = 0.67) were averaged into a composite regulation score, with higher values indicating better regulation.

2.3.4. Depressive Symptoms

Adolescents’ depressive symptoms were measured by the 13-item DSM-Oriented Affective Problems Scale from the Youth Self-Report (Achenbach et al., 2001). Adolescents rated their symptoms on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 (“not true”) to 2 (“very true”). Higher scores indicated greater depressive symptoms (Cronbach’s α = 0.89).

2.4. Data Analysis

The data was analyzed via SPSS 27.0, Rstudio 4.5, and Mplus 8.11. In SPSS 27.0, we computed descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, Pearson’s correlations, and paired-sample t-tests) for the focal variables and conducted an EFA of the EAC2. Visualizations such as a heatmap and forest plot were produced with RStudio 4.5.
All subsequent mixture models were carried out in Mplus 8.11. Step 1. LPA was conducted to identify profiles of parental reactions to adolescents’ happiness, sadness, and anger. The fit indices included the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), sample size-adjusted BIC (aBIC), Bootstrapping Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT), Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test (VLMR), and entropy. The following criteria determined the superior model: (a) lower AIC, BIC, and aBIC, (b) significant BLRT and VLMR, and (c) higher entropy (0.80). Step 2. Logistic regression was performed using the AUXILIARY command (R3STEP) to model demographics (gender, education, income, and marital satisfaction) as predictors of profile membership (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014). Step 3. Differences in the outcome across profiles were examined with the manual Bolck–Croon–Hagenaars approach, adjusting for covariates by (a) saving the profile membership probabilities, and (b) testing the regression models with the TRAINING and MODEL TEST commands (McLarnon & O’Neill, 2018).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Pearson’s correlations showed that enhancing (happiness) and coaching (anger, sadness) relate to better emotion regulation, fewer depressive symptoms, and higher SES, whereas dampening, neglect, and dismissing show the opposite (see Figure 1). Besides, the gender difference showed mothers displayed more enhancing (happiness), coaching (sadness and anger), magnifying (sadness), and dismissing (anger) responses, but less neglect (happiness) compared with fathers.

3.2. Latent Profile Analysis of Parental Reactions to Emotions

3.2.1. Profiles of Parental Reactions to Happiness

The model fit results for parental reactions to happiness are shown in Table 3. The AIC, BIC, and aBIC values all decreased as the number of profiles increased, whereas the VLMR p-value indicated that the three- and five-profile models failed to improve the model fit. Both the two- and four-profile models produced entropy values above 0.80. However, the AIC, BIC, and aBIC values were lower in the four-profile model than in the two-profile model. Therefore, a four-profile model was selected. As shown in Figure 2A, three profiles displayed consistent reactions between fathers and mothers, indicating that adolescents generally perceive both parents as using similar ES strategies. Specifically, the first profile featured high enhancing but low dampening and neglect, which was labeled as Consistent Supportive ES (38.2%). Characterized by low enhancing, as well as above-average dampening and neglect, the second profile was labeled Consistent Unsupportive ES (12.4%). The third profile was labeled Consistent Disengaging ES (40.9%), with the means of all indicators below zero. In the fourth profile, maternal and paternal ES behaviors differed remarkably and were labeled as Inconsistent ES (8.5%). Mothers were perceived as having high levels of dampening and neglect, whereas fathers were perceived as having low levels on average for all indicators for adolescents.

3.2.2. Profiles of Parental Reactions to Sadness and Anger

The two-profile model was selected as the best-fit model for parental reactions to sadness (Table 3). As shown in Figure 2B, most families were classified in the Consistent Supportive ES profile (61.2%); both parents in the profile exhibited high coaching and magnifying but low dismissing. The rest were classified as the Consistent Unsupportive ES profile (38.8%); both parents showed high coaching and magnifying but low dismissing. Similarly, regarding the parental reactions to anger, the three-profile model was selected based on the aforementioned criteria (Table 3). As expected, the first two profiles were labeled Consistent Supportive ES (53.8%) and Consistent Unsupportive ES (22.5%). The third profile, characterized by low levels of all indicators, was labeled as Consistent Disengaging ES (53.8%; Figure 2C).

3.3. Child Gender, Mothers’ Education, Marital Satisfaction, and Income Correlates

A series of logistic regressions were conducted using the R3STEP, with the Consistent Supportive ES profile set as the reference group, as it was the most common pattern identified across LPA models (Table 4). For the happiness condition, the results showed that low levels of family income were associated with greater odds of parents being classified in the Consistent Disengagement profile than in the Consistent Supportive profile (OR = 0.79, p < 0.05). Similarly, low levels of marital satisfaction were associated with greater odds of parents being classified in the Consistent Unsupportive ES profile (OR = 0.98, p < 0.05). For the sadness condition, the results showed that girls were more likely than boys to perceive their parents as fitting the Consistent Unsupportive ES profile rather than the Consistent Supportive ES profile (OR = 1.88, p < 0.05). For the anger condition, the results showed that girls (OR = 2.22, p < 0.05) and low levels of mothers’ education (OR = 0.60, p < 0.05) were associated with greater odds of parents being classified in the Consistent Disengaging ES profile than in the Consistent Supportive ES profile. Comparisons between the Consistent Supportive ES profile and the other profiles did not significantly differ in terms of adolescents’ gender, mothers’ education, marital satisfaction, and family income.

3.4. Latent Profiles and Depressive Symptoms and Emotion Regulation

As shown in Table 5, we compared the differences in adolescents’ emotional adjustment after adjusting for demographics. When adolescents perceived their parents to be in the Consistent Supportive profile, they showed optimal emotional adjustment (i.e., lowest levels of depressive symptoms and highest levels of emotion regulation) for all emotional conditions. In contrast, adolescents who perceived their parents to be in the Consistent Unsupportive profile demonstrated the poorest emotional functioning, especially in response to sadness. Findings for the Consistent Disengaging profile varied by emotion. In the happiness condition, these adolescents reported lower depressive symptoms and better emotion regulation than those perceiving a Consistent Supportive profile; under the anger context, however, depressive symptom levels did not differ between the two groups, suggesting that parental disengagement in response to anger may be less detrimental. Finally, adolescents perceiving an Inconsistent profile experienced higher depressive symptoms than those perceiving a Consistent Disengaging profile, while their emotion regulation and overall symptom levels were similar to those of the Consistent Unsupportive profile.

4. Discussion

The current study used LPA to identify profiles of how Chinese parents jointly react to adolescents’ discrete emotions (happiness, sadness, and anger), and examined familial, parent, and child correlates of these profiles. Results generally supported our initial hypothesis: most families exhibited similar maternal and paternal reactions within each emotional context (i.e., both supportive, both unsupportive, or both disengaging), while a subset exhibited divergent parental responses. Interestingly, reaction patterns varied by emotion: both parents tended to disengage in response to happiness and anger, but not sadness. In addition, the divergent reaction pattern, characterized by unsupportive mothers and disengaging fathers, emerged exclusively for happiness. These patterns were associated with demographic factors and linked to adolescents’ emotional adjustment at varying levels, underscoring the need for tailored, emotion-specific interventions that account for family context to optimally support adolescent well-being.

4.1. Profile Enumeration of Socialization of Discrete Emotions

Consistent with Hypothesis 1, we captured parental consistency of ES across all emotional conditions, including the Consistent Supportive and Consistent Unsupportive profiles, which reflects the similar reactions between fathers and mothers regardless of the emotion types. These findings align with previous studies on parental reactions to negative emotions (Miller-Slough et al., 2018; Zhu & Dunsmore, 2023), and expand our understanding of how adolescents’ happiness is socialized. The general similarity between fathers and mothers may be explained by assortative mating theory, which suggests that partners with similar attitudes and beliefs tend to select each other (Lansford et al., 2014). Once in a relationship, partners also tend to influence each other and become more similar over time. Therefore, as parents operate as a coparenting unit, they may adopt each other’s parenting practices and react consistently to their children’s emotional expressions, for better or worse. Additionally, mothers and fathers may show similarities in parenting because they are responding to the stimulus of the same child. Finally, shared method variance due to adolescents reporting on both mothers’ and fathers’ behaviors might have contributed to cross-parent similarities on our measures (Jager et al., 2016).
However, this consistency did not extend to parents’ disengaging responses, which emerged only for happiness and anger but not for sadness, labeled as the Consistent Disengaging profile (relatively low across measures). This pattern aligns with Russell’s circumplex model, as both happiness (high pleasure, high arousal) and anger (low pleasure, high arousal) share heightened arousal and potential for interpersonal contagion. In collectivist cultures such as China, where interpersonal harmony is strongly emphasized, high-arousal emotions are often viewed as disruptive or inconsiderate of others’ feelings, potentially provoking jealousy or gossip (Aggarwal et al., 2022; Malatesta & Wilson, 1988; O’Neal & Magai, 2005). As a result, emotionally reserved Chinese parents may adopt a passive approach to managing their children’s happiness and anger, not merely relying on direct suppression, reflecting a broader range of subtle emotional management strategies aimed at maintaining cohesion. In contrast, sadness is a nonhostile emotion with low arousal that tends to elicit engagement (i.e., either supportive or non-supportive) rather than withdrawal (Barett & Campos, 1987), which may explain the absence of a disengaging profile in the sadness condition. Future studies should examine whether cultural factors, such as collectivist orientation or sensitivity to emotional contagion, contribute to parents’ passive disengagement from high-arousal emotions. The implications of such disengagement may differ between happiness and anger, which will be explored in the following subsection.
Beyond these general similarities, we also expected and identified an Inconsistent ES profile marked by divergent maternal and paternal reactions exclusively in the happiness condition (not in the sadness or anger). This finding aligns with Lunkenheimer et al. (2012), whose laboratory observations revealed that parents exhibit greater flexibility and employ a wider range of strategies when responding to children’s expressions of happiness compared to negative feelings. From a functionalist perspective, happiness represents a desirable state that triggers “free activation,” encouraging spontaneous and unplanned interactions (Aggarwal et al., 2022; Fredrickson, 2013). In contrast, sadness and anger are often treated as “problem emotions” that require parents’ special attention or additional intervention and call for more uniform and goal-directed responses to their children’s distress (Forgas, 2017; Majdandžić et al., 2012). Consistent with this interpretation, Crockenberg and Leerke’s (2003) suggest that distressed children may elicit intensified parental cooperation, prompting parents to align their approaches to downregulate negative emotions. Therefore, the unique divergence observed in responses to happiness likely reflects the lower perceived necessity for coordinated interventions and highlights the spontaneous and individualized nature of parental reactions in positive emotional contexts.
Furthermore, this divergence profile (characterized by low scores on all paternal reactions and high maternal dampening and neglect reactions) reflects paternal disengagement and maternal nonsupport, which was unexpected and has not been fully captured by those studies on parents’ collaborative efforts. Nevertheless, researchers focusing on either fathers or mothers individually suggest that paternal disengagement (from negative emotions) and maternal nonsupport (toward positive emotions) are relatively common in Chinese families (Shi et al., 2024b; Song et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2019). These findings may be better understood from a cultural lens. In collectivist contexts, emotional restraint is valued to maintain interpersonal harmony, a tendency especially notable among Chinese fathers, who typically show lower emotional and verbal expressiveness than mothers (Li, 2021). Given that positive emotions like happiness generally do not prompt parental intervention in the same manner as negative emotions, Chinese fathers might be more inclined toward passive disengagement when responding to happiness. This paternal disengagement can unintentionally increase maternal parenting burdens, often described as “widow-parenting,” a phenomenon reflecting the limitations of traditional gendered divisions of labor (“men in charge of the outside, women in charge of the inside”). Indeed, from the broader parenting literature, prior research has shown that paternal disengagement exacerbates maternal parenting stress (Nomaguchi et al., 2017). Under the heightened parenting responsibilities and difficulties, mothers were less likely to employ supportive strategies to address household chaos and assist children in managing emotions (Cui et al., 2021). This culturally shaped, gender-based dynamic has recently sparked significant debate on Chinese social media (Shen & Jiao, 2024), underscoring the necessity of further empirical investigations to clarify its broader implications for the nuanced interplay among familial roles, cultural values, and adolescent emotion socialization.

4.2. Relationships Between ES Patterns and Their Predictors and Outcomes

Logistic regression results indicated that parents with higher income, better education, and greater marital satisfaction were more likely to be in the Consistent Supportive profile and provide more positive reactions to their children, which is consistent with Hypothesis 2 and previous person-centered research (King et al., 2023; Sosa-Hernandez et al., 2020). These results provide further support for spillover theory, which posits that positive marital interactions and advantageous socioeconomic circumstances extend into the parent–child relationship, enhancing parents’ emotional resources and facilitating more adaptive parental responses toward their children. Although parents’ greater support for boys’ anger replicates Radke-Yarrow and Kochanska’s (1990) observation that boys’ expressions of anger are met with relative tolerance, our finding that boys also received more support than girls when displaying sadness requires a broader interpretation of gender norms, developmental vulnerability, and cultural change. In many families, a boy’s sadness may violate prevailing expectations of male stoicism, rendering his distress unusually salient and prompting parents to respond with heightened empathy and practical assistance (Fabes & Martin, 1991). During adolescence in particular, parents may perceive boys as less practiced at articulating or regulating sadness and therefore intervene more readily to alleviate perceived vulnerability (Raval & Walker, 2019). At the same time, contemporary shifts in Chinese parenting ideals—spurred by urbanization and increased emphasis on emotional competence—have encouraged caregivers to support boys’ emotional expression, including sadness, as part of healthy socioemotional development (Li, 2021). Therefore, children’s gender may prove an intriguing focus for future longitudinal research to explore these dynamics further.
Generally speaking, adolescents with consistently supportive ES showed the best emotional adjustment, whereas those with parents with consistently unsupportive ES showed the poorest, supporting our Hypothesis 3 and prior work (Miller-Slough et al., 2018; Yu et al., 2015; Zhu & Dunsmore, 2023). Furthermore, we found parents in the Inconsistent profile were associated with greater depressive symptoms and emotion dysregulation, aligning with the results of Yu et al. (2015), who used the variable-centered approach. This might be because inconsistent reactions of parents convey mixed messages that undermine emotional security, particularly during adolescence, a critical period of emotional instability and self-identity development. Unexpectedly, adolescents whose parents employed disengaging strategies (Consistent Disengaging profile) exhibited more depressive symptoms in the happiness condition, whereas the opposite was true for anger. Functionalists argue that happiness is typically shared to gain acceptance (Barett & Campos, 1987). Thus, disengaging or unsupportive reactions might greatly suppress adolescents’ desire to communicate these feelings, increasing the risk for dysregulation and depression (Katz et al., 2014; Yap et al., 2008; Yi et al., 2016). In addition, adolescents’ egocentrism heightens their sensitivity to others’ appraisals, increasing the likelihood of perceiving disengagement as rejection (Rohner & Lansford, 2017). Such cognitive tendencies potentially shape interactions. In contrast, parental disengagement from adolescents’ anger may have a constructive role (Vanwoerden et al., 2022), especially in collectivist contexts that value interpersonal harmony. Specifically, anger, characterized by high arousal and unpleasantness, can trigger interpersonal contagion and escalate conflicts if not properly managed (Russell, 1980). An interview study of Asian mothers found that those with healthy children (rather than those with maladjusted children) were more likely to use a “not talking to the child for a brief period” strategy when their children expressed anger. They perceived it as beneficial because this approach provides adolescents with some time to reflect and allows parents to calm down and think about what to do (Raval & Martini, 2011). Our findings echo this culturally framed approach, indicating that parental disengagement during anger may promote adolescent self-regulation and relational repair. Future studies should examine how such culturally informed disengagement tactics influence long-term emotion regulation and family dynamics.

4.3. Limitations and Implications

This study has several limitations. First, the sole reliance on adolescent self-reports of ES may inflate shared method variance and obscure genuine differences between maternal and paternal behaviors; future studies should triangulate adolescents’ perceptions with father- and mother-reports or employ observational and ecological-momentary assessment methods to reduce informant bias. Second, we assessed marital satisfaction only from mothers’ perspectives, thereby offering an incomplete portrait of the couple’s relational climate; incorporating paternal self-reports and behavioral coding of partner interactions would enhance the validity of family-level analyses. Third, the cross-sectional design precludes any inference regarding temporal precedence or causality between emotion-socialization strategies and adolescent adjustment; longitudinal or intervention-based research is necessary to establish directional effects and developmental trajectories. Finally, our sample was drawn from a single middle school in Guangdong Province, which may constrain the generalizability of results; replication with more diverse samples across different cultural, socioeconomic, and geographic settings is therefore recommended.
Our identification of distinct parental emotion profiles yields two primary implications for practice. The first concerns joint, emotion-specific parent training: programs should involve both mothers and fathers and tailor content to discrete emotions. For sadness and anger, modules can teach soothing techniques and empathic listening to alleviate distress; for happiness, activities should guide parents toward a shared response strategy, fostering consensus and reinforcing positive interactions. The second involves strategic disengagement for anger management: when addressing anger, parents and adolescents benefit from techniques such as “not for talking for a brief period”—brief pauses in communication that give adolescents space to self-regulate before conflicts escalate. Counseling centers can incorporate these methods into parent-education curricula, training caregivers to recognize and implement temporary disengagement as a constructive emotion regulation tool.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.G.; Methodology, C.Z.; Software, W.P.; Formal analysis, H.G.; Investigation, W.P.; Writing—original draft, H.G., W.P. and Y.G. (Yuhan Gao); Writing—review & editing, Y.G. (Yue Guan), Y.G. (Yuhan Gao), J.M., S.L. and C.Z.; Funding acquisition, C.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 82301740) and the Innovation Projects of Universities in Guangdong Province (grant number 2023WTSCX085).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by Shaoguan University Ethics Committee (protocol code yxy1lscb202203, approved on 22 October 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6-18, TRF, and YSR. University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aggarwal, P., Raval, V. V., Sathiyaseelan, A., Trevethan, M., & Luebbe, A. M. (2022). Scripts about happiness among urban families in South India. Journal of Happiness Studies, 23(5), 2059–2082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Allen, N. B., Kuppens, P., & Sheeber, L. B. (2012). Heart rate responses to parental behavior in depressed adolescents. Biological Psychology, 90(1), 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three–step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 21(3), 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barett, K. C., & Campos, J. J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 555–578). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on parental socialization of children’s sadness regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 210–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion, 5(1), 80–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Crockenberg, S., & Leerkes, E. (2003). Infant negative emotionality, caregiving, and family relationships. In A. C. Crouter, & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics (pp. 57–78). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  9. Cui, L., Zhang, X., & Han, Z. R. (2021). Perceived child difficultness, emotion dysregulation, and emotion-related parenting among Chinese parents. Family Process, 60(4), 1403–1417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Ding, R., Yang, Y., & Wang, Q. (2024). Do parents show interpersonally oriented socialization practices for adolescents’ negative emotions? Through the lens of Chinese families. Emotion, 25(3), 556–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Eisenberg, N. (2020). Findings, issues, and new directions for research on emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998a). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 2227–2247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Cumberland, A. (1998b). The socialization of emotion: Reply to commentaries. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 317–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fabes, R. A., & Martin, C. L. (1991). Gender and age stereotypes of emotionality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 532–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Feinberg, M. E. (2003). The internal structure and ecological context of coparenting: A framework for research and intervention. Parenting, 3(2), 95–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Forgas, J. P. (2017). The Role of Dysphoria in Social Thinking and Behaviour. Australian Psychologist, 52(1), 18–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). Cultivated emotions: Parental socialization of positive emotions and self-conscious emotions. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 279–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. In P. Devine, & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 1–53). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  20. Garside, R. B. (2004). Parental socialization of discrete positive and negative emotions: Implications for emotional functioning (Publication No. 3148117) [Doctoral dissertation, The Catholic University of America]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. [Google Scholar]
  21. Garside, R. B., & Klimes-Dougan, B. (2002). Socialization of discrete negative emotions: Gender differences and links with psychological distress. Sex Roles, 47(3), 115–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10(3), 243–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hooven, C., Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1995). Parental meta-emotion structure predicts family and child outcomes. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2–3), 229–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jager, J., Mahler, A., An, D., Putnick, D. L., Bornstein, M. H., Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Skinner, A. T., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2016). Early adolescents’ unique perspectives of maternal and paternal rejection: Examining their across-dyad generalizability and relations with adjustment 1 year later. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(10), 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Katz, L. F., Shortt, J. W., Allen, N. B., Davis, B., Hunter, E., Leve, C., & Sheeber, L. (2014). Parental emotion socialization in clinically depressed adolescents: Enhancing and dampening positive affect. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(2), 205–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. King, G. L., Macdonald, J. A., Greenwood, C. J., Kehoe, C., Dunsmore, J. C., Havighurst, S. S., Youssef, G. J., Berkowitz, T. S., & Westrupp, E. M. (2023). Profiles of parents’ emotion socialization within a multinational sample of parents. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1161418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Klimes-Dougan, B., Brand, A. E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Usher, B., Hastings, P. D., Kendziora, K., & Garside, R. B. (2007). Parental emotion socialization in adolescence: Differences in sex, age and problem status. Social Development, 16(2), 326–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lansford, J. E., Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2014). Mothers’ and fathers’ autonomy-relevant parenting: Longitudinal links with adolescents’ externalizing and internalizing behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(11), 1877–1889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Li, X. (2021). How do Chinese fathers express love? Viewing paternal warmth through the eyes of Chinese fathers, mothers, and their children. Psychology of Men & Masculinities, 22(3), 500–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21(3), 251–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lunkenheimer, E. S., Hollenstein, T., Wang, J., & Shields, A. M. (2012). Flexibility and attractors in context: Family emotion socialization patterns and children’s emotion regulation in late childhood. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 16(3), 269–291. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  32. Lunkenheimer, E. S., Shields, A. M., & Cortina, K. S. (2007). Parental emotion coaching and dismissing in family interaction. Social Development, 16(2), 232–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Song, L. (2013). Chinese parents’ goals and practices in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 843–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Majdandžić, M., de Vente, W., Feinberg, M. E., Aktar, E., & Bögels, S. M. (2012). Bidirectional associations between coparenting relations and family member anxiety: A review and conceptual model. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(1), 28–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Malatesta, C. Z., & Wilson, A. (1988). Emotion cognition interaction in personality development: A discrete emotions, functionalist analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27(1), 91–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Nakagawa, S. (2008). Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 925–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. McElwain, N. L., Halberstadt, A. G., & Volling, B. L. (2007). Mother- and father-reported reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to young children’s emotional understanding and friendship quality. Child Development, 78(5), 1407–1425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. McLarnon, M. J. W., & O’Neill, T. A. (2018). Extensions of auxiliary variable approaches for the investigation of mediation, moderation, and conditional effects in mixture models. Organizational Research Methods, 21(4), 955–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Miller-Slough, R. L., Dunsmore, J. C., Zeman, J. L., Sanders, W. M., & Poon, J. A. (2018). Maternal and paternal reactions to child sadness predict children’s psychosocial outcomes: A family-centered approach. Social Development, 27(3), 495–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Moran, K. M., Root, A. E., Vizy, B. K., Wilson, T. K., & Gentzler, A. L. (2018). Maternal socialization of children’s positive affect regulation: Associations with children’s savoring, dampening, and depressive symptoms. Social Development, 28(2), 306–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16(2), 361–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Nomaguchi, K., Brown, S., & Leyman, T. M. (2017). Fathers’ participation in parenting and maternal parenting stress: Variation by relationship status. Journal of Family Issues, 38(8), 1132–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. O’Neal, C. R., & Magai, C. (2005). Do parents respond in different ways when children feel different emotions? The emotional context of parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 467–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Radke-Yarrow, M., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Anger in young children. In N. L. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 297–310). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  45. Raval, V., Daga, S. S., Raval, P. H., & Panchal, I. N. (2016). Asian Indian mothers’ emotion socialization and child emotion expression as a function of situational context. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(9), 2853–2861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Raval, V., & Martini, T. S. (2009). Maternal socialization of children’s anger, sadness, and physical pain in two communities in Gujarat, India. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(3), 215–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Raval, V., & Martini, T. S. (2011). ‘Making the child understand:’ Socialization of emotion in urban India. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(6), 847–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Raval, V., & Walker, B. L. (2019). Unpacking ‘culture’: Caregiver socialization of emotion and child functioning in diverse families. Developmental Review, 51, 146–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Rohner, R. P., & Lansford, J. E. (2017). Deep structure of the human affectional system: Introduction to interpersonal acceptance–rejection theory. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 9(4), 426–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sanders, W., Zeman, J., Poon, J., & Miller, R. (2015). Child regulation of negative emotions and depressive symptoms: The moderating role of parental emotion socialization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(2), 402–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Shen, Y., & Jiao, L. (2024). ‘Widow-style parenting’: The discursive formation of gendered parenting roles and popular feminism on social media in China. Chinese Journal of Communication, 17(4), 399–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Shi, Z., Fan, L., & Wang, Q. (2024a). Scrutinizing parental minimization reactions to adolescents’ negative emotions through the lens of Chinese culture. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 34(2), 380–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Shi, Z., Qu, Y., Yan, Y., & Wang, Q. (2024b). Chinese mothers’ reactions to adolescents’ positive emotions: Relations to adolescents’ emotional adjustment and mothers’ socialization goals. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 53(5), 1214–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Shipman, K. L., & Zeman, J. (2001). Socialization of children’s emotion regulation in mother–child dyads: A developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 13(2), 317–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Song, Q., Yang, Y., Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2019). Savoring or dampening? Maternal reactions to children’s positive emotions in cultural contexts. Culture and Brain, 7(2), 172–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Sosa-Hernandez, L., Sack, L., Seddon, J. A., Bailey, K., & Thomassin, K. (2020). Mother and father repertoires of emotion socialization practices in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 69, 101159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Suveg, C., Sood, E., Barmish, A., Tiwari, S., Hudson, J. L., & Kendall, P. C. (2008). ‘I’d rather not talk about it’: Emotion parenting in families of children with an anxiety disorder. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(6), 875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Tao, A., Zhou, Q., & Wang, Y. (2010). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Prospective relations to Chinese children’s psychological adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(2), 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Tomkins, S. (2008). Affect, imagery, consciousness: The complete edtion. Springer. [Google Scholar]
  61. Van Lissa, C. J., Keizer, R., Van Lier, P. A., Meeus, W. H., & Branje, S. (2019). The role of fathers’ versus mothers’ parenting in emotion-regulation development from mid–late adolescence: Disentangling between-family differences from within-family effects. Developmental Psychology, 55(2), 377–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Vanwoerden, S., Byrd, A. L., Vine, V., Beeney, J. E., Scott, L. N., & Stepp, S. D. (2022). Momentary borderline personality disorder symptoms in youth as a function of parental invalidation and youth-perceived support. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(2), 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Wang, M., Liang, Y., Zhou, N., & Zou, H. (2019). Chinese fathers’ emotion socialization profiles and adolescents’ emotion regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 137(15), 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Yang, Y., Song, Q., Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2020). Maternal reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s socio-emotional development among European American and Chinese immigrant children. Transcultural Psychiatry, 57(3), 408–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Yap, M. B. H., Allen, N. B., & Ladouceur, C. D. (2008). Maternal socialization of positive affect: The impact of invalidation on adolescent emotion regulation and depressive symptomatology. Child Development, 79(5), 1415–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Yi, C. Y., Gentzler, A. L., Ramsey, M. A., & Root, A. E. (2016). Linking maternal socialization of positive emotions to children’s behavioral problems: The moderating role of self-control. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(5), 1550–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Yu, T., Volling, B. L., & Niu, W. (2015). Emotion socialization and children’s behavioral problems in China and the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 46(3), 419–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Zeman, J., & Shipman, K. (1997). Social-Contextual influences on expectancies for managing anger and sadness: The transition from middle childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Development and initial validation of the Children’s Sadness Management Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(3), 187–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zhu, D., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2023). Family functioning and emotion socialization in Chinese two-parent households: A person-centered approach. Social Development, 32(2), 463–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Correlation heatmap and mother–father emotion socialization differences. Note: connecting lines represent differences in ES between fathers and mothers as perceived by adolescents (gray lines denote marginal significance); H = happiness; S = sadness; A = anger; M- = maternal; P- = paternal. ** p < 0.01. * p < 0.05.
Figure 1. Correlation heatmap and mother–father emotion socialization differences. Note: connecting lines represent differences in ES between fathers and mothers as perceived by adolescents (gray lines denote marginal significance); H = happiness; S = sadness; A = anger; M- = maternal; P- = paternal. ** p < 0.01. * p < 0.05.
Behavsci 15 00999 g001
Figure 2. Standardized means of parental emotion socialization varied by profile. (A) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ happiness. (B) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ sadness. (C) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ anger.
Figure 2. Standardized means of parental emotion socialization varied by profile. (A) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ happiness. (B) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ sadness. (C) Profiles of paternal and maternal reactions to adolescents’ anger.
Behavsci 15 00999 g002
Table 1. Demographic Information in the Current Study.
Table 1. Demographic Information in the Current Study.
Frequency%
Gender
boys36254.35
girls30445.65
Education
≤middle school25538.29
senior secondary school or high school16224.32
community college7411.11
≥bachelor213.15
Monthly Income
≤29999814.71
3000–599917626.43
6000–899910115.17
9000–14,9998512.76
15,000–19,999233.45
≥20,000182.70
Table 2. The factorial structure and items of EAC2 used in the current study.
Table 2. The factorial structure and items of EAC2 used in the current study.
HappinessSadnessAnger
ORIG.
Structure
ItemsFactorsORIG. StructureItemsFactorsORIG. StructureItemsFactors
h1h2h3s1s2s3a1a2a3
REWARDinterested in why I was happy REWARDasked me about it REWARDfound out what made me angry
encouraged me to share deal with the problem deal with the problem
listened to me comforted me understood why I was angry
MAGNIFYsaid he/she was happy, too OVERRIDEtold me not to worry OVERRIDEtold me things were not so bad
became very happy/themselves told me to cheer up told me to change my attitude
joined me in my happiness bought something I like told me to keep quiet
PUNISHnot like my being happy PUNISHnot like my being sad a said I should be ashamed
told me to keep it to myself gave me a disgusted look PUNISHpunished me
told me to settle down called me a crybaby gave me something else to do
OVERRIDEother things were more important NEGLECTusually was not around NEGLECTignored
changed the subject most times did not notice most times did not notice
quickly moved on to something else ignored me usually was not around
NEGLECTusually did not notice MAGNIFYbecame sad themselves MAGNIFYyelled back at me
usually was not around became upset became angry at me
failed to ask about it became tearful or cried became upset at me
Note. Gray shading indicates factor loadings ≥0.40; ORIG. = the original version of the five-factor scale; h1 = enhancing (αm = 0.90, αf = 0.92); h2 = dampening (αm = 0.67, αf = 0.65); h3 = neglect (αm = 0.80, αf = 0.84); s1 = coaching (αm = 0.90, αf = 0.91); s2 = dismissing (αm = 0.69, αf = 0.76); s3 = magnifying (αm = 0.54, αf = 0.54); a1 = coaching (αm = 0.86, αf = 0.86); a2 = neglect (αm = 0.72, αf = 0.71); a3 = dismissing (αm = 0.75, αf = 0.78). a The item was eliminated and not included in the subsequent analysis.
Table 3. LPAs of parental socialization of different emotions.
Table 3. LPAs of parental socialization of different emotions.
No. ProfilesAICBICaBICEntropyVLMR p-ValueBLRT p-Value
ES in the Happiness
1 Profile11,071.85511,125.79911,087.698
2 Profiles10,410.66510,496.07510,435.7500.8660.012<0.001
3 Profiles10,170.74010,287.61710,205.0660.7560.347<0.001
4 Profiles9969.34910,117.69210,012.9160.8030.035<0.001
5 Profiles9840.62510,020.4369893.4340.8060.168<0.001
ES in the Sadness
1 Profile11,054.82811,108.77111,070.671
2 Profiles10,387.57010,472.98010,412.6550.818<0.001<0.001
3 Profiles10,143.23810,260.11510,177.5640.8350.132<0.001
4 Profiles9912.59110,060.9359956.1590.8470.469<0.001
5 Profiles9795.6869975.4979848.4950.8390.243<0.001
ES in the Anger
1 Profile11,066.18011,120.12311,082.022
2 Profiles10,434.81410,520.22410,459.8990.831<0.001<0.001
3 Profiles10,135.32910,252.20610,169.6550.8060.009<0.001
4 Profiles9938.49510,086.8399982.0630.8240.0940.939
5 Profiles9809.7679989.5779862.5760.8380.503<0.001
Note. Optimal models for each emotion type are highlighted in bold. AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; aBIC = adjusted BIC; LMR = Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test; BLRT = Bootstrapped likelihood ratio test.
Table 4. Logistic regressions across emotion types with the 3-step method.
Table 4. Logistic regressions across emotion types with the 3-step method.
Behavsci 15 00999 i001
Note. Statistically significant differences are denoted in bold. The consistent supportive ES was the reference group; Profile 1 = consistent supportive ES; Profile 2 = consistent unsupportive ES; Profile 3 = consistent disengagement ES; Profile 4 = inconsistent ES; satisfaction = marital satisfaction. a 0 = male, 1 = female. b 1 = middle school or below, 2 = senior secondary school or high school, 3 = community college, 4 = bachelor or above; c 1 = ≤2999, 2 = 3000–5999, 3 = 6000–8999, 4 = 9000–14,999, 5 = 15,000–19,999, 6 = ≥20,000.  p < 0.10. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Differences in the outcomes across the profiles of ES with the manual BCH method.
Table 5. Differences in the outcomes across the profiles of ES with the manual BCH method.
OutcomesHappinessSadnessAnger
ProfileMχ2(3)DiffProfileMχ2(1)DiffProfileMχ2(2)Diff
Emotion regulation 13.66 **P1 > P3 = P4 = P2 20.98 ***P1 > P2 21.09 ***P1 > P2 = P3
P12.09 P12.06 P12.09
P21.89 P21.79 P21.84
P31.85 P31.80
P41.91
Depressive symptoms 59.41 ***P1 < P3 < P4 = P2 27.80 ***P1 < P2 65.80 ***P1 = P3 < P2
P10.39 P10.45 P10.43
P20.84 P20.71 P20.91
P30.55 P30.48
P40.81
Note. Covariates include gender, education, income, and marital satisfaction; P1 = consistent supportive ES; P2 = consistent unsupportive ES; P3 = consistent disengagement ES; P4 = inconsistent ES. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gao, H.; Guan, Y.; Pei, W.; Gao, Y.; Mao, J.; Liao, S.; Zeng, C. Emotion Socialization Under One Roof: How Parental Response Patterns Shape Adolescent Emotional Well-Being. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 999. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15080999

AMA Style

Gao H, Guan Y, Pei W, Gao Y, Mao J, Liao S, Zeng C. Emotion Socialization Under One Roof: How Parental Response Patterns Shape Adolescent Emotional Well-Being. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(8):999. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15080999

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gao, Huiyuan, Yue Guan, Wenyue Pei, Yuhan Gao, Jiayue Mao, Suqun Liao, and Can Zeng. 2025. "Emotion Socialization Under One Roof: How Parental Response Patterns Shape Adolescent Emotional Well-Being" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 8: 999. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15080999

APA Style

Gao, H., Guan, Y., Pei, W., Gao, Y., Mao, J., Liao, S., & Zeng, C. (2025). Emotion Socialization Under One Roof: How Parental Response Patterns Shape Adolescent Emotional Well-Being. Behavioral Sciences, 15(8), 999. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15080999

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop