1. Introduction
Medical empathy is considered an attribute that enables the comprehension of patients’ thoughts and feelings and allows the treating physician to respond appropriately from an emotional standpoint (
Adam et al., 2022). As a result, empathy is a construct structured by two components: the cognitive one, which provides the treating medical professional with an understanding of the patient’s experiences and expectations regarding their health situation (
Ulloque et al., 2019), and the emotional component, which allows the medical professional to understand and feel the patient’s suffering (
Tehranineshat et al., 2019;
Thompson et al., 2019). Therefore, it is essential to consider empathy as a complex and dynamic system since its components are dialectically interconnected (
Altuna, 2018). As a consequence of those above, an imbalance in this equilibrium results in a decrease in empathetic capacity and, in the worst case, severe psychopathology when one of these components has a critical flaw (
Blair, 2018;
Kerr-Gaffney et al., 2019). The cognitive component has two dimensions, perspective adoption (PA) and “walking in the patient’s shoes” (WIPS), while the emotional component consists of one dimension, compassionate care (CC). The complexity of the empathic development process in a human being is not limited to the interaction between the components or their dimensions. Indeed, two factors are involved in developing empathy in humans: phylogeny and ontogeny (
Decety & Svetlova, 2012). The former is the product of biological evolution and the changes it entails, including those in the brain, which were (and still are) slow processes. This process serves to accumulate positive adaptive changes in the form of genetic information and, over time, allows the formation of the limbic system of the brain, which is responsible for a person’s emotional processes and is subsequently involved in the development of the frontal lobe, which can be associated with the development of cognitive capacity in the human being (
Barrera-Gil et al., 2018). The latter (ontogeny) is composed of all the factors that can influence a person throughout their existence and starts since birth or even before. Therefore, it is necessary to study factors that may be “suspected” of contributing to or hindering the normal development of the brain structures that support empathy components, the interconnections (neural networks) between these structures, and the functioning of said structures and interconnections (
Couette et al., 2022). For this reason, numerous factors in the ontogeny process can influence empathy in early childhood, adolescence, or young adulthood. These factors are also present in the formation process of students in various healthcare fields. Some of these factors may be: child abuse (
Berzenski & Yates, 2022), family violence (
Ramos et al., 2021), bullying (
Garandeau et al., 2022), burnout (
Lopes & Nihei, 2020), academic stress (
Banerjee et al., 2019), personality (
Dávila-Pontón et al., 2020a), family functioning (
Dávila-Pontón et al., 2020b), culture (
Ullrich, 2022), prosocial behaviors (
Suazo et al., 2020), and resilience (
Spilg et al., 2022), among many others. Some of these factors can critically influence empathic development (
Allen et al., 2021;
Hartman et al., 2019;
Metcalf et al., 2021), and their outcomes can determine the presence of a decrease in empathy or be associated with psychopathologies. Given these multiple implications, empathy studying empathy determinants is highly relevant.
There are several definitions of the concept of “resilience”. This variability in this construct’s definition resides in its theoretical foundations and associated characteristics (
Bonanno, 2021;
Tang et al., 2022;
Young et al., 2019). However, all these theories agree that resilience is complex and determined by several variables or traits attributed to it, which are not necessarily wholly independent and can interact with each other.
There are a series of risk factors generally associated with resilience, such as experiencing depression, anxiety, or abuse, as well as protective factors like positive affect, self-esteem, optimism, or self-efficacy, among many others (
Hunsu et al., 2023). Regarding risks, there seems to be agreement among authors on the association between exposure to significant risk and positive evolution in terms of psychosocial well-being despite the threat being faced (
Yule et al., 2019), considering as “significant risk” a situation that appears to have no solution and may lead to dysfunctional adaptation and psychological distress (
Yule et al., 2019). In this context, resilient individuals can adapt and face the negative experiences that may occur in their lives, thanks to the ability to generate a “positive evolution” of their life project despite adverse conditions that may arise in their environment. They acquire the ability to use personal, family, relational, and existential resources properly to cope with suffering and psychic vulnerability (
Dulin et al., 2018).
However, the concept of resilience is used in many ways depending on the field of application and the theoretical-practical basis. This situation prevents obtaining a common definition of the concept. It makes it difficult to compare the results of different research (
Moya & Goenechea, 2022), with the implication that the absence of a common definition complicates its objective measurement (
Sisto et al., 2019).
The absence of a single theory of individual resilience in psychology makes its operationalization challenging. So far, there are two approaches: the buffering hypothesis and the trait approach (
Johnson et al., 2011;
Khalil et al., 2022). The buffering approach measures resilience on a binary scale, having or not having a specific characteristic (
Heritage et al., 2019), whereas the trait approach examines how individuals cope with events they perceive as negative and takes into account their capacity for recovery (
Maltby et al., 2015). Based on Holling’s approach (
Holling, 2006), which combines the systems theory and ecology to describe resilience in various ecological systems, three subsystems are derived to explain resilience: engineering resilience, ecological resilience, and adaptive capacity. Engineering resilience is the ability to return to or recover equilibrium after disturbance (
Hoffman & Hancock, 2017). Ecological resilience is the capacity of a system to absorb or resist a disturbance before realigning the key fundamental critical mechanisms of the system and maintaining its stable state in terms of function, purpose, structure, or identity (
Maltby et al., 2015;
Timpane-Padgham et al., 2017). Adaptive capacity is the ability of an ecosystem to manage and accommodate change and to adapt (
Faye et al., 2018;
Maltby et al., 2015;
Palacio et al., 2020).
In the psychological literature, engineering resilience is an individual’s capacity to recover or “return” to their original state after challenging experiences (
Howell & Miller-Graff, 2014). Ecological resilience has been recognized in the psychological literature as the ability to be robust; demonstrate confidence in one’s strengths and abilities; and be stoic, resourceful, and determined as one navigates through life (
Golubovich et al., 2014;
Vaughn & DeJonckheere, 2021). Adaptive capacity has been recognized in the psychological literature as the ability to adapt to adverse circumstances, be flexible when facing them, and have the capacity to modify said circumstances and respond effectively to disruptions (
Connor & Davidson, 2003).
Empathy and resilience, along with other factors, have been studied to explain various behaviors in healthcare professionals and students of healthcare sciences (
Chaukos et al., 2017;
Elam & Taku, 2022;
Foster et al., 2018;
Rafaqat et al., 2022;
Sturzu et al., 2019;
Velayudhan, 2021). In this regard, a positive effect of resilience and self-efficacy was observed on compassion fatigue in nurses (
Zhang et al., 2022). Compassion fatigue is one of the elements involved in the process of empathic erosion. The cited study demonstrated resilience by raising the nurses’ compassion threshold. It has also been observed that resilience acts as a mediator between empathy and work commitment (
Cao & Chen, 2020).
In this sense, it has been demonstrated that dental students are subjected to the action of various factors that can cause a decrease in empathy toward the patient. These factors include stress (
Nitschke & Bartz, 2023), depression (
Rajput et al., 2020), burn-out (
MacAulay et al., 2023), personal well-being (
Paloniemi et al., 2021), and academic performance and anxiety (
Ahmad et al., 2022), among other factors. The aforementioned factors could be modulated by resilience by eliminating or attenuating its negative effect on the cognitive and emotional components of empathy and reducing the risk of the presence of empathic erosion in dental students in particular and of the health sciences student, in general (
Díaz-Narváez et al., 2020). In this sense, several authors have presented information that leads to considering that resilience could be an independent variable of empathy (
Findyartini et al., 2021;
Halimi et al., 2023;
Morice-Ramat et al., 2018;
Ord et al., 2020;
Rafaqat et al., 2022;
Stern et al., 2023;
Waddimba et al., 2021;
Wu et al., 2022). Additionally, it is necessary to consider the proven existence of variability in the distribution of empathy in dental students in Latin America (
Díaz-Narváez et al., 2021), which implies that the main question to be answered is not whether there is an association between the two variables under study but how these variables are associated explicitly in a population of students.
However, studies on resilience as a predictor of empathy and its dimensions have been relatively scarce. Specifically, studies on resilience as a predictor of empathy in dental students from Latin America and other continents are almost non-existent. Furthermore, studies of this type in Latin America must be carried out with particular care due to the significant variability in the distribution of empathy among students within the same specialty and across different specialties within and among countries of this continent. Empathy is a desirable competency for healthcare professionals, and it enhances the quality of patient care in a few ways (
Cruz, 2020). Determining the associated factors and predictors of empathy, such as resilience, could improve empathic capacity by strengthening resilience. All these variables, taken together, can improve the clinical practice of dentists and the stress experienced by dental students and general and pediatric dentists (
Tokgöz Kaplan, 2024).
Since empathy is an essential competency in the health care setting and resilience could influence its development, it is necessary to study the relationship between these variables. This study sought to investigate the psychometric properties of empathy and resilience measures in dental students and how resilience was associated with predicting empathy levels in this population. We set out with two consecutive objectives. The first was to establish the psychometric properties of empathy and resilience measures, including sex invariance. The second objective was to analyze the association between empathy and resilience to predict empathy values as a function of resilience in a population of dental students.
2. Methods
2.1. Design
A cross-sectional study was conducted.
2.2. Participants
The sample consisted of 397 dental surgery students from the first to the seventh year in 2022, distributed as 73.8% (n = 293) females and 26.2% (n = 104) males, with ages ranging from 18 to 33 years (M = 21.49, SD = 2.79). The participants were selected through convenience sampling from a population of 462 students of the Faculty of Dentistry “Evangélica University of El Salvador”, with 14.07% of the population of students who chose not to participate or were absent from classes during the data collection period.
2.3. Study Measures
The Jefferson scale of empathy (JSE-HPS) (
Hojat et al., 2002b) consisted of 20 items that measured the levels of empathy with patients in health science students of any specialty. The items were rated on a 7-point response scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scale measured three dimensions: compassionate care (CC, items 1, 7, 8, 11, 12, 14, 18, and 19), perspective adoption (PA, items 2, 4, 5, 9, 10, 13, 15, 16, 17, and 20), and “walking in the patient’s shoes” (WIPS, items 3 and 6). The scale demonstrated adequate internal consistency (α = 0.78–0.92) and appropriate correlations with other psychological variables.
The resilience trait scale (EEA,
Maltby et al., 2015) assessed three facets of resilience: engineering, ecological, and adaptive resilience. It had a 12-item Likert-type format, with five response levels per item, from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (5). The FSS demonstrated adequate internal and test–retest reliability, a cross-culturally stable factor structure, convergent and construct validity in terms of associations with personality, and a positive contribution to clinical and non-clinical psychological health states (
Maltby et al., 2015,
2016).
2.4. Procedures
All students who agreed to participate signed an informed consent form before completing the instruments, ensuring their autonomy, voluntariness, and the confidentiality of information. All students were subjected to the same pedagogical strategy, emphasizing the teaching of human values.
Data collection for both study periods took place in September 2022. The instruments were administered in groups, in pen and paper format, during the students’ regular class schedule. Data were collected by professors from the Faculty of Dentistry who were not involved in this research and had received the necessary training to administer the instruments correctly, address student questions, and ensure the proper reception of responses. The following were considered inclusion criteria: voluntarily agreeing to respond to the instrument, having regular student status (basic, pre-clinical, or clinical), and being present during the instrument’s administration. All the evaluated students belonged to the Faculty of Dentistry of the Evangelical University of El Salvador (El Salvador).
The study adhered to the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (2013). The Institutional Ethics Committee of Andrés Bello University, Record 020-2022, approved the research project and informed consent. All sociodemographic and personal data and the responses from the administered instruments were considered confidential.
2.5. Data Analysis
To portray empathy and resilience, a series of descriptive statistics were calculated for each of the items and dimensions of the instruments. Cronbach’s alpha (α) and McDonald’s omega (ω) were used to assess the measure’s reliability. Values of α > 0.70 were considered to indicate “good” reliability. To establish differences by sex, the Student’s t-test was used for independent samples.
A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted using the maximum likelihood estimation method to determine the validity of the resilience and empathy models. To assess the goodness of fit, the relative chi-square (CMIN/DF) was estimated along with the chi-square and degrees of freedom, the goodness of fit index (GFI), the comparative fit index (CFI), the non-normed fit index (NNFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), following the recommendations of
Hoyle (
1995),
Hu and Bentler (
1999), and
Kline (
2005). Acceptable fit was indicated by a CMIN/DF of less than 2 or 3, GFI and CFI of at least 0.90, RMSEA index between 0.05 and 0.08, and SRMR less than 0.08.
Factorial invariance was analyzed through a multigroup-analyzed model (
Jöreskog, 1971b), using the Chi-square test (χ
2) to assess the goodness of fit. However, since the Chi-square test was sensitive to the sample size, decreases in CFI of less than 0.01 (Δ ± 0.01) compared with the previous model were considered a more appropriate indicator of invariance (
Cheung & Rensvold, 2002).
Empathy (E) and its dimensions (CC, PT, and WIPS) were considered dependent variables, while engineering resilience, ecological resilience, adaptive resilience, and age were treated as independent variables. The analysis was weighted by sex due to the imbalance in this variable in the sample. The values of multiple R, multiple R2, adjusted R2, the F-value from the analysis of variance, the Durbin–Watson statistic (DW), tolerance statistic, and variance inflation factor (VIF) were estimated. Descriptive statistics were calculated using SPSS 27, and the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed using AMOS 25 on the collected data based on the three-factor resilience model.
3. Results
Table 1 shows that all items had skewness (Sk) and kurtosis (Kurt) values within the expected limits (Sk < ±2; Kurt < ±7).
Table 2 presents the results for the mean and standard deviation levels of E, CC, PT, PA, and WIPS in the factors Y, C, and S, as well as combinations of these factors’ levels.
Reliability: The dimensions of the empathy scale had adequate reliability indices: PT or PA (α = 0.89; ω = 0.89), CC (α = 0.79; ω = 0.79), and WIPS (α = 0.68; ω = 0.68). Similarly, in the sample of females, all three dimensions had acceptable reliability indices: PA (α = 0.89; ω = 0.89), CC (α = 0.77; ω = 0.77), and WIPS (α = 0.64; ω = 0.64). In the sample of males, the three dimensions also presented adequate adjustment indices: PA (α = 0.89; ω = 0.89), CC (α = 0.83; ω = 0.83), and WIPS (α = 0.74; ω = 0.75).
Validity based on the internal structure: It was evident that the original model of three related factors presented adequate adjustment indices (χ
2 = 329.87; df = 167;
p < 0.01; IFC = 0.92; TLI = 0.91; RMSEA = 0.056 [90% CI 0.047–0.064]; SRMR = 0.057). In addition, the factor weight of most of the items was adequate. It was also clear that the dimensions were related to each other.
Factorial Invariance: In
Table 3 it is observed that the factorial structure of the scale showed evidence of being strictly invariant for the groups of males and females in the sequence of proposed invariance models: metric invariance (ΔCFI = −0.001; ΔRMSEA = −0.002), scalar (ΔCFI = −0.010; ΔRMSEA = 0.002), and strict (ΔCFI = −0.003; ΔRMSEA = −0.001).
3.1. Resilience
Table 4 presents the descriptive statistics of the items and dimensions of resilience. The skewness (Sk) and kurtosis (Kurt) values of the items and the dimensions were within the range of +/−1, indicating a “very good” symmetry of a univariate normal distribution.
In addition, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the scales (engineering resilience, α = 0.86; ecological resilience, α = 0.80; and adaptive resilience, α = 0.75), with α = 0.84 for the total scale, surpass the aforementioned internal reliability criterion of α > 0.70, which is considered “good”. Finally, differences in resilience were found based on sex, with females (M = 40.86, SD = 7.15) exhibiting lower resilience than males (M = 44.69, SD = 7.55) (t = 4.628, df = 395, p = 0.0001).
When establishing the factorial validity of the resilience scale through CFA, significant factor loadings were observed, ranging between λ = 0.538 and λ = 0.839 (
Table 5), with goodness-of-fit statistics for the three-factor model that met the aforementioned criteria of adequate fit to the data, incorporating correlated errors for items 3 and 4 (χ
2 = 123.968, df = 50,
p < 0.001; χ
2/df = 2.479; GFI = 0.952, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.061, 90% CI for RMSEA = 0.048–0.075, SRMR = 0.0445). When examining the goodness of fit of the model for both sexes, an adequate fit was observed for females, χ
2 = 110.331, df = 50,
p < 0.001; χ
2/df = 2.207; GFI = 0.944, CFI = 0.954, RMSEA = 0.064, 90% CI for RMSEA = 0.048–0.081, SRMR = 0.0448, as well as for males, χ
2 = 89.845, df = 50,
p < 0.001; χ
2/df = 1.797; GFI = 0.877, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.088, 90% CI for RMSEA = 0.058–0.117, SRMR = 0.0862.
3.2. Sex Invariance Indices of the Resilience Scale
A factorial invariance analysis was conducted, comparing females and males through a multigroup analysis, revealing a reasonably adequate fit of the model to the data: χ
2 = 200.411,
p < 0.0001, χ
2/df = 2.004, SRMR = 0.047, GFI = 0.925, CFI = 0.945, RMSEA = 0.050 (90% CI = 0.040–0.061). After establishing the baseline models by sex and nested models based on the baseline model, no significant changes were observed in the chi-square value, with differences in CFI that were not relevant (ΔCFI < 0.01), which was less than 0.01, allowing the assumption of configural and metric invariance (
Cheung & Rensvold, 2002) (
Table 6).
Table 7 shows the results of the ANOVA F-value. All of them were significant or highly significant, except the dependent variable WIPS. This meant that the correlation coefficient values (R) of the corresponding regression equations were significant (different from zero). In all cases, the DW statistic was not significant and ranged between the values of 1.5 and 2.5, implying that there was no autocorrelation. The residuals were distributed independently of each other (
Savin & White, 1977). Finally, the regression coefficient values and determination coefficients found for the significant variables showed that the degree of association fluctuated between 18.2% and 22.5%, and the variance explained by these variables fluctuated between 3.3% and 5.0%.
Table 8 shows the regression analysis results (weighted by sex). Ecological resilience was positively associated with empathy, CC was negatively associated with adaptive resilience, and WIPS was not associated with any of the independent variables. The associations described above were highly significant in all cases, with tolerance values greater than 0.1 and VIF values less than 10, indicating that the independent variables were not collinear. Age was not significant; therefore, in the studied sample, it did not influence empathy levels and dimensions.
4. Discussion
Our first objective was to establish the psychometric properties of the measures used, including sex invariance. The psychometric analyses conducted on measurements of resilience and empathy allowed us to demonstrate that the model of three underlying dimensions in each of them was met in the sample of the population of dental students. This finding was important because an a priori assumption should not be made that the model of a construct fit the collected data, even in samples taken from the same population in which this compliance had been observed before, especially if the context changed (
Velickovic et al., 2020). Additionally, the invariance analysis allowed us to observe that the model of both constructs held for both sexes. This finding supported the reliability of sex comparisons (
de Roover et al., 2022). Furthermore, the regression model complied with the assumptions for its robustness, validating the model, and therefore, the associations found could be considered reliable in the studied sample.
Empathy is a complex construct and has been the subject of extensive theoretical discussions without reaching a complete agreement (
Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990;
Eisenberg et al., 1991;
Férriz et al., 2018;
Machado & Calvetti, 2019). Nevertheless, there is agreement that medical empathy has both cognitive and emotional components (
Hojat et al., 2002a). In the literature, it has been observed that there are some differences in the definition in the sense that some authors consider clinical empathy to be predominantly cognitive when it comes to patient care (
Hojat et al., 2002a,
2020,
2023). However, other authors (
Díaz-Narváez et al., 2022) suggest that empathy is a system composed of dimensions, and none of these dimensions is predominant over the others; instead, there is a dialectical relation between these components, and this relation is dynamic. As a system, empathy requires the active participation of its three dimensions. Therefore, one cannot be empathetic with the patient if the three dimensions do not operate in the process of intersubjectivity with the patient. A similar situation occurs with individual resilience, which is characterized by a lack of clarity regarding the exact meaning of this construct and its constituents (
Alzugaray et al., 2018). The trait approach is characterized by the ability to combine systems theory and ecology to describe resilience. Therefore, attempting to study how a complex system, such as resilience, can modulate another complex system, such as empathy, is a difficult task that allows only approximate knowledge of the interactions between both systems. Indeed, resilience could be generally defined by a high degree of expectation, self-determination, flexibility, optimism, cognitive reappraisal, and active coping (
Han & Nestler, 2017).
In this sense, it is known that dental students are exposed to exogenous disturbances that hinder their academic and clinical performance. Stress, anxiety, depression, academic pressure, workload, financial problems, year of study, exams, grades, and patient care are among these factors (
Fonseca-Molina et al., 2018a,
2018b). The students’ response to this situation should positively face these disturbances (
Atkinson et al., 2009). In this regard, the findings suggested that measures of resilience were related to adaptive traits (extroversion, agreeableness, openness, and conscientiousness) and adaptive coping traits (problem-focused coping, positive appraisals, and emotional regulation) (
Gariépy et al., 2016;
Alzugaray et al., 2020).
The results observed in this study showed that adaptive resilience was negatively associated with the empathy component CC. Therefore, we should expect that the levels of this dimension were somewhat deficient compared with the maximum score that could be achieved in this dimension (56 points), and this should be observed empirically. Therefore, if adaptive resilience is characterized by the ability to adapt well, adjust, be flexible, change, innovate, modify, and respond effectively to disturbances (
Maltby et al., 2015), it means that the presence of these traits will lead to a decrease in the levels of the CC component (a decrease in the empathetic threshold). Indeed, the values of this dimension, according to cutoff points recently calculated for dental students in Latin America, can be characterized as high but with values lower than the 25th percentile.
The possible consequences of this finding could be explained by the potential presence of some degree of empathic fatigue and, consequently, empathic erosion in students, with the negative impact this has on the student–patient relationship. However, it is also possible to speculate that this finding could be explained by a conscious process of adaptation to the conditions of observing pain and an excellent response to this disturbance, primarily using cognitive empathy to find the necessary “mechanism” to maintain an objective perspective and make informed decisions, based on the recognition that emotional empathy is also necessary to establish an effective connection with patients. Therefore, what was described above may imply the ability to “apply” both cognitive and emotional empathy in different situations and to be aware of when it is appropriate to “utilize” each of them with a greater “intensity” in relation to the other in order to create a suitable empathetic balance that allows the regulation of the effect of the patient’s pain on the student’s empathic effectiveness and ensures appropriate patient care. This alternative can explain the coping success based on the idea that adaptive resilience produces an increase in the compassionate threshold and that this process possibly allows the student to positively adapt to disturbances through emotional regulation mechanisms without this regulation diminishing their compassionate capacity. In this case, resilience would contribute to finding a balance without affecting their ability to feel compassion. Certainly, a response to this inference should be studied in future research.
On the other hand, the positive association found between ecological resilience and the PA component of empathy can be understood in the sense that the traits contained in this resilience (the ability to be robust, self-confidence, confidence in one’s abilities, stoic attitude, resourcefulness, and determination, which are achieved through critical domains in one’s life) can be associated with the development of executive functions in a complex process. In this sense, cognitive development is additive-systemic throughout the ontogeny process: selectivity and control of cognitive processes, increased ability to create mental schemas, greater mental flexibility, increased use and complexity of memory-learning strategies, and improved organization and planning of cognitive and behavioral activity. Based on constant development of the attitude toward analyzing things in increasingly abstract ways, as well as using more complex psycholinguistic elements (
Ganesan & Steinbeis, 2022;
Shibaoka et al., 2023), the development of these cognitive aspects, in turn, favors the specific cognitive aspects of the PA component of empathy. Indeed,
Decety and Jackson (
2004) stated that cognitive flexibility is required to adopt the perspective of others, and the traits of ecological resilience could, in some way, positively influence the ability to develop the necessary cognitive flexibility to develop the PA dimension in empathy positively. Indeed, this inference needs to be subjected to further research that can determine the “intersection zones” between the traits of ecological resilience, the ontogenetic cognitive development of executive functions, and the ability to develop patient perspective adoption, which is ultimately the intellectual understanding of their condition.
The lack of association between resilience in general and the WIPS dimension in the present study is difficult to explain. On the one hand, it is known that the WIPS dimension provides the ability to understand the meaning of the other person’s thoughts without losing the objectivity of that meaning. Thus, the presence of a negative association between adaptive resilience, which raises the threshold of compassion (possibly without losing this capacity); the positive association of ecological resilience with respect to the PA dimension; and possibly the absence of highly intense events in the process of providing patient care that would motivate an alteration of the empathic balance could be the necessary and sufficient conditions for each type of resilience to influence, in some way, the levels of WIPS. Regarding the resilience construct, it has been observed that the engineering dimension, when measured in adolescents, may be less significant in relation to the other dimensions (
Alzugaray et al., 2020), which could diminish the overall effect of resilience.
Finally, the role of certain sociodemographic variables, such as age, sex, education, and income, which are associated with resilience and could be confounding factors, must be further investigated (
Ueno et al., 2021). Recent studies suggest that females may be less resilient (
Afshari et al., 2021;
Singh et al., 2019) and that resilience increases with age (
Afshari et al., 2021;
Staneva et al., 2022).
Among the study’s limitations, we can note that the results of the associations found cannot be generalized a priori to all dental students in El Salvador and Latin America, due to the significant variability in empathy and its dimensions by sex, country, and different forms that empathy levels and its dimensions take throughout courses (empathic decline).