Next Article in Journal
Violence Under Control: Self-Control and Psychopathy in Women Convicted of Violent Crimes
Previous Article in Journal
How Does Nature Connectedness Improve Mental Health in College Students? A Study of Chain Mediating Effects
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Change in Self-Esteem Trajectories Among Adolescents and Adults with Intellectual Disabilities

Department of Secondary Special Education, Jeonju University, Jeonju 55069, Republic of Korea
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(5), 655; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050655
Submission received: 24 February 2025 / Revised: 29 April 2025 / Accepted: 9 May 2025 / Published: 12 May 2025

Abstract

:
Self-esteem significantly influences and shapes an individual’s social behavior and indicates his or her psychological and mental health. In this study, the following two premises are examined: (1) whether the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities undergoes changes over time and (2) the demographic variables that influence this change. For this study, a longitudinal analysis spanning 2 years was conducted using response data from 398 participants—as sourced from the Korea Employment Agency for the Disabled—which comprised gender, age, education, and severity of disability as input variables. A potential growth model analysis confirmed the appropriateness of the second measurement change model (significant increase in self-esteem between the first and second measurements, no changes between second and third measurements) that presented the main premises of his study. The findings indicated that the level of self-esteem and its rate of change varied significantly among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities, and the variations were primarily associated with gender and severity of disabilities.

1. Introduction

As an integral part of the self-concept, self-esteem plays a vital role in human adaptation and mental health, thus affecting an individual’s overall well-being significantly. Most definitions of self-esteem provide an individual’s subjective evaluation of self-worth. Coopersmith (1981) defined self-esteem as a positive self-concept that involves self-perceptions of capability and value. Donnellan et al. (2011) presented self-esteem as an individual’s subjective evaluation of their intrinsic self-worth, regardless of external validation. Rosenberg (1965) conceptualized self-esteem as an individual’s feeling of being good enough, which entails self-acceptance, self-respect, and self-valuing. Baumeister et al. (1993) viewed self-esteem as an evaluative element of self-knowledge and argued that it depends on an individual’s accurate recognition of his or her unique characteristics. However, some individuals cannot evaluate themselves positively with respect to specific value criteria, even if the accuracy of their self-judgment is assessed based on an objective external criterion. In contrast, Rosenberg (1989) argued that a self-report alone is sufficient, without external objective criteria.
Understanding the dynamics of self-esteem enables effective implementation of interventions aimed at enhancing self-esteem. Despite being stable, self-esteem is not static; it can change in response to various events, experiences, and social environment (Grumm et al., 2009; Lintunen et al., 1995; Trzesniewski et al., 2013). While some studies reported that one-third to one-half of adolescents experience low self-esteem during early adolescence (Harter, 2013; Hirsch & DuBois, 1991), it tends to stabilize or increase during middle or late adolescence (Demo & Savin-Williams, 1983). Self-esteem has been reported as very stable during the developmental ages (Cole et al., 2001).
Self-esteem is closely related to an individual’s psychological development. It can vary among individuals in terms of demographic factors such as gender, age, and education. Therefore, analyzing the relationship among the aforementioned factors and self-esteem is important to understand what influences the formation of and changes in self-esteem. Time-invariant variables, such as gender, age, and education, are representative demographic factors related to self-esteem, and gender is reported to show a relatively weak correlation (Bachman et al., 2011; Baumeister et al., 1993). A meta-analysis by Kling et al. (1999) indicated minimal gender-based effects on self-esteem, and men exhibited self-esteem slightly more positively. With respect to gender, girls tend to have lower self-esteem than boys, and this difference appears to widen in late adolescence (Emler, 2001). With respect to age, self-esteem is high in childhood, decreases in adolescence, and gradually increases throughout adulthood (Robins et al., 2002). With respect to strong correlation of education level with self-esteem than income (Twenge & Campbell, 2002) individuals with higher educational levels exhibit a higher self-esteem trajectory (Orth et al., 2010).
Despite its importance as a psychological construct and association with mental and physical health, little is known about self-esteem among adults with intellectual disabilities (Lee et al., 2023). American Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities characterizes intellectual disability by significant limitations in intellectual functioning and adaptive behavior, which is expressed in conceptual, social, and practical adaptive skills (Schalock et al., 2021). Most studies on self-esteem in people with intellectual disabilities have focused on analyzing the level of self-esteem at a specific point in time or the correlations among related factors. While these studies have provided basic data for understanding the characteristics of self-esteem in people with intellectual disabilities, they could not effectively capture changes with respect to time or individuals. Garaigordobil and Pérez (2007) analyzed individuals’ differences in terms of self-concept, self-esteem, and psychopathology symptoms in two groups, with and without intellectual disabilities. They reported that the group with intellectual disabilities had significantly lower self-concept and self-esteem and exhibited most psychopathology symptoms. Furthermore, gender-based differences were insignificant in both groups. This is a cross-sectional study with data obtained at the same point in time; thus, it cannot identify changes in self-concept or self-esteem over time. Only 42 out of the 170 participants were intellectually disabled, limiting the results’ generalizability. A systematic literature review revealed that school-aged youth with intellectual disabilities had lower cognitive, academic, and self-esteem scores than typically developing adolescents, and recognized few significant relationships of self-concept with academic achievement and gender (Maïano et al., 2019). This study explored self-concept and self-esteem in adolescence; however, it may have missed changes or developmental perspectives over time due to the lack of a longitudinal approach. Studies on self-esteem targeting general public are often cross-sectional and longitudinal, and finding research on the developmental trajectory of self-esteem among people with intellectual disabilities remains difficult. Previous cross-sectional studies on self-esteem in people with intellectual disabilities suggest limitations in interpreting causal relationships and highlight the need for longitudinal studies to identify changes over time, because self-concept and self-esteem are developmental in nature (Davies et al., 2021; Syropoulou et al., 2021).
Self-esteem is a key element in human psychological development that affects mental health, social relationships, and quality of life (Orth & Robins, 2014). In a study of 619 people with intellectual disabilities, happiness and a positive outlook on life were significantly associated with positive self-perception and evaluations from others, high self-esteem, and a sense of autonomy (Ashori et al., 2014). Anctil et al. (2008) reported that the higher the self-determination of students with disabilities, the higher their physical and psychological health, self-esteem, and overall satisfaction in life (Schalock et al., 2021). In general, self-esteem tends to be stable or increase during the transition from childhood to adolescence and adulthood (Erol & Orth, 2011). However, such tendency of general population with typical development may not appear in people with intellectual disabilities. In the middle adolescence period, from the age 15 onwards, self-concept becomes increasingly sophisticated and stable (Harter, 2015; Orth et al., 2018), and social factors, such as peer relationships and school experiences, significantly influence self-esteem (Robins & Trzesniewski, 2005). In the transitional period of adolescence, people with intellectual disabilities may experience difficulties in social integration, identity formation, and emotional regulation (Emerson & Hatton, 2007). Hence, this study examines change aspects of self-esteem in adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities and aims to understand their psychological characteristics in detail.
Despite realizing the importance of longitudinal studies, research on long-term changes in the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities remains lacking because of insufficient highly representative panel data, making systematic verification of these conflicting claims difficult. In a previous study targeting adults with intellectual disabilities, 25 papers were analyzed, and mixed evidence was obtained regarding their self-esteem levels. The study suggested that factors such as engagement in life were related to higher self-esteem, and low self-esteem were associated with depression (Lee et al., 2023). In particular, understanding the changeability in self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities remains difficult. To address this knowledge gap, the current study used panel data to determine the patterns of change in the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities and the demographic variables influencing such change.
Thus, this study aims to answer the following research questions: (1) Does the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities change over time? (2) Which demographic variables influence changes in self-esteem among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities?

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

In this study, a longitudinal design was employed.

2.2. Data

In this study, the first and second waves of the 8th Panel Survey on Employment for the Disabled data provided by the Korea Employment Agency for the Disabled were used to determine changes in self-esteem among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities. The second-wave data were obtained from a 2018 survey because of the accumulation of structural problems of the longitudinal design in the first-wave survey (the occurrence of withdrawal due to unavoidable reasons such as death, loss of the youth population due to a natural increase in age, aging of the disabled, etc.) hindered the representation of people with disabilities in the survey results. This survey included the first, second, and third measurements in 2018, 2019, and 2020, respectively. The inclusion criteria comprised individuals with disabilities who were aged 15–64 years and were registered under the Welfare of Persons with Disabilities Act of the Republic of Korea. Because the Republic of Korea has adopted a disability registration system in which a doctor’s diagnosis of a disability is required for registration, participation was restricted to individuals who had received a physician-diagnosed intellectual disability.
A two-stage sampling method was employed to control the number of extracted areas and samples according to disability type, severity, and age. Disability samples were selected using a one-stage colony sampling method, followed by stratification based on disability type, severity, and age. The stratification was performed to meet the margin level of the target error. Of 4577 panel participants, 398 had intellectual disabilities. The target group included adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities who lived in the community, while none lived in facilities. In the second measurement, attribution occurred and 374 people responded. In the third measurement, no further attribution occurred, and the same 374 people responded. The appropriate sample size for the latent growth model may vary depending on the complexity of the model and the number of free parameters; however samples of at least 100 are recommended (Hamilton et al., 2003).
The survey was conducted using a tablet PC-assisted personal interviewing system. After visiting the panel, the interviewer used a tablet PC to ask questions from the respondents according to a structured questionnaire and saved their responses on the computer to collect data. Before conducting the survey, a preliminary preference survey was conducted through telephone in which the respondents’ information, address, and contact information were confirmed. Then, the main survey was initiated, and the visit date was set in advance for each participant. The survey followed a data verification procedure in the following five stages: (1) by the tablet PC-assisted personal interviewing system, (2) self-verification (3) through supervisor review, (4) by the independent telephone verification team, and (5) through logical errors and comparative data. Discovery of an error during the verification process required reconfirmation with the panel, another round of survey was conducted via telephone.

2.3. Measurement

In this study, the Rosenberg (1965) Self-Esteem Scale was used to measure self-esteem. After initially developing the scale in 1965, Rosenberg revised and supplemented it in 1989, allowing researchers worldwide to use it freely, without copyright. This self-reported scale measures individuals’ attitudes toward themselves and comprises 5 positive and 5 negative self-esteem questions. Responses are rated on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from “not at all” (1) to “very much” (4). A higher overall score indicates higher self-esteem, and negative self-esteem questions, 3, 5, 8, 9, and 10, are scored using reverse calculations. By analyzing the second-wave measurement data of the panel, self-esteem levels of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities were confirmed and ascertained. Park and Park (2019) found an appropriate item fit, rating scale, and separate index (reliability) for measuring self-esteem through Rasch analysis and confirmed the scale as a useful tool for the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities. Rosenberg self-esteem scale structure composed of a four-point Likert scale for elementary school students with mild intellectual disabilities and confirmed the validity of the bifactor model (Syropoulou et al., 2021). The scale demonstrated excellent reliability with Cronbach’s α values of 0.976 (0.973–0.980), 0.983 (0.980–0.985), and 0.984 (0.981–0.986) for the first, second, and third measurements, respectively.
The obtained demographic information included gender (male was coded as 1 and female as 2), age (15–29 years, 30–39 years, 40–49 years, 50–59 years, and above 60 years), educational level (below middle school graduate, high school graduate, and above college graduate), severity of disability (Levels 1, 2, and 3), presence of comorbid disability (yes, no), residential area (big city, small city, medium-sized city, and rural), and marital status (unmarried, married, divorced, and bereaved). For analyzing the conditional latent growth model, age was the input as a continuous variable.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, and correlations) were used to analyze the survey data of individuals with intellectual disabilities using SPSS 26.0 and AMOS 26.0. The latent growth model examined changes in self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities over time and investigated the variables affecting this change. The basic linear growth model was first tested using panel survey data on self-esteem from the three measurements in which the slope of the basic linear model changed; therefore, the constant term was fixed to 1. The slope assumed a linear pattern of change and was designated as 1, 2, and 3 for first, second, and third measurements, respectively. The second measurement change model assumed a change in the slope between the first and second measurements; however, there were no changes between second and third measurements. Therefore, the slope of the first measurement was designated as 0, and those of second and third measurements were designated as 1. The third measurement change model assumed no change between first and second measurements and a change in slope in the third measurement. Therefore, the slopes of the first and second years were designated as 0, and that of the third measurement was designated as 1. The second and third measurement change model were considered as a nonlinear model.
An unconditional latent growth model was used to identify a suitable model to explain the change in the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities among first, second, and third measurement change models. Conditional latent growth model was used to identify the effects of gender, age, and education level on the initial (intercept) and subsequent change in self-esteem over time (slope) among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities.
In this study, the comparative fit index (CFI), normed fit index (NFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) were used as fit indices. The fit indices of values more than 0.90, were considered a good fit. For RMSEA, a value more than 0.10 was considered a poor fit and a value from 0.05 to 0.08 was considered acceptable (Hu & Bentler, 1999). After examining the fit of the model for changes in self-esteem among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities, the relationship between the change models and demographic variables was verified.
The full information maximum likelihood (FIML) method was employed to address the missing values, as it estimates the characteristics of the population by considering the pattern of missing values appropriately even under the assumptions of completely randomized longitudinal study (Enders, 2006). In this study, 24 data points from the second and third waves were missing.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The study included 253 men (63.6%) and 145 women (36.4%). The majority of participants were aged 15–29 years 203 (51.0%); 211 (53.0%) were high school graduates; and 34 (8.5%) had concomitant disabilities. Most participants (338; 84.9%) were unmarried and lived in large cities (168; 42.2%) (Table 1). The results showed significant differences based on the participants’ gender and residential area in the first measurement and based on the severity of disabilities and residential area in the second and third measurements.
Correlation analysis revealed significant relationships between gender and the first measurement of self-esteem, age and education level, first and second self-esteem measurements, and second and third self-esteem measurements (p < 0.05).

3.2. Analysis of Change in Self-Esteem and General Characteristics

The model’s appropriateness for analyzing changes in self-esteem was verified using confirmatory factor analysis and justified based on fit indices (Figure 1). Table 2 shows the results regarding the goodness-of-fit of the first, second, and third measurement change model. Upon comprehensive examination of the values of χ2, RMSEA, NFI, and CFI, the measurement change model provided the best fit for self-esteem.
Table 3 shows the changes in self-esteem based on the second measurement change model. This means that a significant increase in self-esteem occurred between the first and second measurements; however, there were no changes between the second and third measurements. The initial self-esteem value was 29.864 (p < 0.001), with a variance of 89.873 (p < 0.001), and the rate of change was 2.777 (p < 0.05), with a variance of 237.905 (p < 0.001). The significant initial values for mean and variance indicated individual differences in self-esteem. The mean of the significant rate of change indicated significant change in self-esteem over the 2 years, and the significant variance indicated that the trajectory of change in self-esteem over the 2 years has shown significant differences among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities. The significance of the covariate reflected the relationship between the initial value and the rate of change.

3.3. Relationship Between Self-Esteem and Demographic Characteristics

The results of the relationship between changes in self-esteem and demographic characteristics are provided in Figure 2. The appropriateness of the model was verified, as the value of χ2 was 6.859 (p = 0.334), and the values of NFI, CFI, and RMSEA were 0.960, 0.997, and 0.013, respectively. The results showed that gender was associated with the initial self-esteem level (B = −3.907, p < 0.05) and changes in self-esteem (B = 4.696, p < 0.05). Gender was negatively correlated with initial self-esteem, indicating that females had lower self-esteem than males at the first measurement, and was positively correlated with the rate of change in self-esteem, indicating that self-esteem of females increased more quickly than males. Severity of disability was associated with changes in self-esteem (B = −3.630, p < 0.05).

4. Discussion

Because a longitudinal analysis is crucial for developing interventions to promote healthy self-esteem by identifying the major factors that improve or worsen self-esteem, previous research included longitudinal studies investigating changes in self-esteem and describing its patterns in general population (Baldwin & Hoffmann, 2002; Erol & Orth, 2011; Greene & Way, 2005). However, such studies in people with intellectual disabilities are lacking. This study is the first to apply growth curve modeling to examine whether the self-esteem among adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities changes over time using a longitudinal analysis and whether demographic variables affect this change. The rationale of analysis starting from individuals of 15 years of age is that in this developmental period, self-concept is stabilized and social influence is strengthened (Orth et al., 2018).
The findings indicate that the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities change over time in a nonlinear fashion. In particular, self-esteem underwent significant change between first and second measurements but remained stable between second and third measurements. The change between first and second measurements suggest that individuals’ self-esteem increased significantly as they initially responded to the intervention or changes in daily life. Stabilization after the second measurement indicates stabilized self-esteem. In addition, initially low self-esteem may indicate that no further significant change occurred after it improved to a certain level. Gradual psychological changes in terms of self-esteem show a flattened growth curve at a certain level. A longitudinal study examining youth and adults reported that changes in self-esteem are nonlinear, and they slow down with increasing age (Erol & Orth, 2011). Another study revealed that self-esteem exhibits linear growth between 16 and 32 years, peaks thereafter, and shows no significant growth between 32 and 42 years (Kiviruusu et al., 2015). Baldwin and Hoffmann (2002) suggested a curvilinear relationship between age and self-esteem, indicating the dynamic nature of self-esteem during adolescence. Kiviruusu et al. (2015) reported a quadratic self-esteem model using a linear model suggesting nonlinear change in self-esteem. The average initial values of self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities was 29.864 and 87.873, respectively, and these significant values demonstrated significant individual differences. The average rate of change was 2.777, showing that the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities tended to increase over time. Furthermore, the significant variation in the rate of change (237.905) indicated significant individual differences in terms of the rate of change in the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities. The correlation between the initial value and the rate of change was negative and significant at −62.459, indicating that the higher the level of self-esteem in the first year, the lower the increase in self-esteem. The results of this study are consistent with those of previous studies targeting the general public and reporting that self-esteem decreases during childhood and adolescence owing to confusion in confidence and identity caused by changes in various physical and social cognitive characteristics Feldman and Elliott (1990). while it stabilizes or increases during adulthood (Greene & Way, 2005). Relatively short-term longitudinal analysis studies have reported that self-esteem gradually increases during middle age (Mullis et al., 1992; Wallace et al., 1984).
Demographic factors such as gender, age, and education, along with severity of the disability, affect individuals’ self-esteem (Alkhasawneh et al., 2022; Haq, 2016). These factors are considered appropriate for examining change patterns in the latent growth model due to their time-invariant characteristics. The negative relationship between gender and the initial self-esteem level showed that women have lower self-esteem than men. The positive relationship between gender and the rate of change in self-esteem showing that increase in self-esteem was faster among women with intellectual disabilities than their male counterparts. The results of previous longitudinal and cross-sectional studies on the effects of gender on self-esteem have yielded conflicting results. Some longitudinal studies have reported small gender differences in adolescence (Quatman & Watson, 2001) and young adulthood (Orth et al., 2010; Robins et al., 2002), while others have reported no significant gender effects (Erol & Orth, 2011). Some cross-sectional studies reported gender-based differences in self-esteem (Garaigordobil Landazabal, 2005), while others reported no such differences (Garaigordobil & Pérez, 2007) The nonsignificant age-related results of this study differed from those of previous studies, as Baldwin and Hoffmann (2002) demonstrated the significant effect of age on self-esteem in a growth curve analysis. The differences may be due to the differences in the participants’ age range, which was 15–65 years for this study and 11–16 years for the study by Baldwin and Hoffmann (2002). A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies reported that participants with higher educational levels had higher self-esteem at all ages (Orth et al., 2018). The trajectory of self-esteem among individuals with higher education levels consistently remained exhibited a curvilinear trend for the groups with higher and lower educational levels (Orth et al., 2012). In this study, no difference in self-esteem was observed in terms of severity of disability, and self-esteem was found to decrease with the increasing degree of disability. Such negative relationship between the degree of disability and self-esteem has also been reported in a cross-sectional study (Garaigordobil & Pérez, 2007). The changes in self-esteem based on gender and the severity of the disability may be due to the initial low self-esteem value in the first measurement. In cases of initially low self-esteem, individuals may react sensitively to psychological intervention, social support, and environmental changes, exhibiting significant changes.
Although self-esteem is recognized as a significant psychological construct with implications for mental and physical health in the general population, only a limited understanding has been acquired of its manifestations in adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities (Lee et al., 2023). The results of this study showed that the self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities increased over time, and gender was identified as a key factor affecting the initial level and changes in self-esteem.

4.1. Limitations and Directions for Future Research

This study has several limitations. First, since this study utilized panel data, which is secondary data, it does not sufficiently reflect the various demographic and environmental variables that may affect changes in self-esteem. For example, family support, peer relationships, quality of special education services, and level of community integration are major factors that may affect self-esteem; however, they were not included in these data. Thus, future studies need to collect primary data that includes a wider range of variables and analyze other factors that affect self-esteem. Second, the sample size in this study was insufficient to divide the intellectually disabled individuals according to their age groups for better analysis; hence, life cycle–related change trajectory in self-esteem could not be clearly identified. In future studies, a large-scale longitudinal sample that sufficiently includes the intellectually disabled by age group should be secured to enable a comparative analysis of the developmental trajectory of self-esteem throughout the life cycle. Third limitation is that individual differences in terms of rate or pattern of change in self-esteem among individual participants could not be analyzed precisely. Therefore, future studies should also utilize analytical techniques, such as latent class growth modeling, to capture not only intraindividual but also interindividual changes.

4.2. Implications for Practice

The results of this study provide the following implications for practical implementation supporting intellectually disabled adolescents and adults:
First, self-esteem should be recognized as a dynamic psychological aspect that requires continuous psychological and emotional support. Hence, systematic interventions to improve self-esteem are required in schools, welfare organizations, and adult transition support centers. In particular, the results confirming the existence of intraindividual differences suggest the need for individually tailored training programs to enhance self-esteem. Second, this study found that gender and the severity of disability affected changes in self-esteem, and the rate of change was particularly high in women with low initial self-esteem levels, suggesting the need for customized self-esteem improvement programs tailored to individual characteristics, especially gender and severity of disability. In particular, programs such as peer support groups, emotional expression training, and expanded opportunities for self-expression targeting female adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities can have a positive effect. Third, since self-esteem is closely related to an individual’s social integration, identity formation, and emotional stability, interventions to improve self-esteem can go beyond simple psychological support and enhance overall quality of life. Therefore, systematically designing opportunities for positive interaction, accumulation of small-scale success experiences, and experiences of social recognition remains essential. Fourth, in this study, self-esteem significantly increased between the first and second measurements, and there was no change between the second and third measurements, showing that self-esteem interventions can work more effectively in the early stages.

5. Conclusions

This longitudinal study differs from previous short-term or cross-sectional studies that focus on the change in self-esteem of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities. The results confirm that the self-esteem development trajectories of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities differ among individuals. The results further showed a significant change between first and second measurements, and gender and severity of disability in adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities might have had an effect on changes in their self-esteem. These results contribute to a deeper understanding of the psychological development of individuals with intellectual disabilities by presenting meaningful empirical evidence that self-esteem remains dynamic throughout life. In particular, in the case of adolescents and adults with intellectual disabilities with low self-esteem, a high rate of change in women’s self-esteem indicates that providing programs to improve self-esteem can be helpful.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All procedures performed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments, or comparable ethical standards. Ethical review and approval were not required for the study of human participants in accordance with local legislation and institutional requirements.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants provided written informed consent prior to enrollment.

Data Availability Statement

https://edi.kead.or.kr/BoardType17.do?bid=18&mid=37 (accessed on 10 November 2024). To download a data file, connect to the site and request data.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Korea’s Disabled People’s Development Institute for providing the data.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Alkhasawneh, T., Al-Shaar, A. S., Khasawneh, M., Darawsheh, S., & Aburaya, N. (2022). Self-Esteem and its relationship to some demographic variables among students with learning disabilities. Information Sciences Letters, 11(6), 1929–1936. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anctil, T. M., Ishikawa, M. E., & Tao Scott, A. (2008). Academic identity development through self-determination: Successful college students with learning disabilities. Career Development for Exceptional Individuals, 31(3), 164–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ashori, M., Jalilabkenar, S. S., Ghobari Bonab, B., & Habibpoor, S. (2014). The effect of social skills training on self-esteem of adolescents with intellectual disability. Journal of Research in Rehabilitation Sciences, 9(6), 1565–1570. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bachman, J. G., O’Malley, P. M., Freedman-Doan, P., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2011). Adolescent self-esteem: Differences by race/ethnicity, gender, and age. Self and Identity, 10(4), 445–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Baldwin, S. A., & Hoffmann, J. P. (2002). The dynamics of self-esteem: A growth-curve analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 101–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1993). When ego threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences of high self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(1), 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cole, D. A., Maxwell, S. E., Martin, J. M., Peeke, L. G., Seroczynski, A. D., Tram, J. M., & Maschman, T. (2001). The development of multiple domains of child and adolescent self-concept: A cohort sequential longitudinal design. Child Development, 72(6), 1723–1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Coopersmith, S. (1981). Coopersmith self-esteem inventories. Consulting Psychologist Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Davies, L., Randle-Phillips, C., Russell, A., & Delaney, C. (2021). The relationship between adverse interpersonal experiences and self-esteem in people with intellectual disabilities: The role of shame, self-compassion and social support. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 34(4), 1037–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Demo, D. H., & Savin-Williams, R. C. (1983). Early adolescent self-esteem as a function of social class: Rosenberg and pearlin revisited. American Journal of Sociology, 88(4), 763–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (2011). Self-esteem: Enduring issues and controversies. In The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences (pp. 718–746). Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  12. Emerson, E., & Hatton, C. (2007). Mental health of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities in Britain. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 191(6), 493–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Emler, N. (2001). Self esteem: The costs and causes of low self worth. York Publishing Services. [Google Scholar]
  14. Enders, C. K. (2006). Analyzing structural equation models with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling: A Second Course, 2, 493–519. [Google Scholar]
  15. Erol, R. Y., & Orth, U. (2011). Self-esteem development from age 14 to 30 years: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(3), 607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Feldman, S. S., & Elliott, G. R. (1990). At the threshold: The developing adolescent. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Garaigordobil Landazabal, M. (2005). Prosocial and creative play: Effects of a programme on the verbal and nonverbal intelligence of children aged 10–11 years. International Journal of Psychology, 40(3), 176–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Garaigordobil, M., & Pérez, J. I. (2007). Self-concept, self-esteem and psychopathological symptoms in persons with intellectual disability. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10(1), 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Greene, M. L., & Way, N. (2005). Self-esteem trajectories among ethnic minority adolescents: A growth curve analysis of the patterns and predictors of change. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15(2), 151–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Grumm, M., Nestler, S., & Von Collani, G. (2009). Changing explicit and implicit attitudes: The case of self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(2), 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hamilton, J., Gagne, P. E., & Hancock, G. R. (2003, April 21–25). The effect of sample size on latent growth models. American Educational Research Association 2003 Annual Meeting, Chicago, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
  22. Haq, M. A. U. (2016). Association between socio-demographic background and self-esteem of university students. Psychiatric Quarterly, 87, 755–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Harter, S. (2013). The development of self-esteem. In Self-esteem issues and answers (pp. 144–150). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Harter, S. (2015). The construction of the self: Developmental and sociocultural foundations. Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hirsch, B. J., & DuBois, D. L. (1991). Self-esteem in early adolescence: The identification and prediction of contrasting longitudinal trajectories. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(1), 53–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kiviruusu, O., Huurre, T., Aro, H., Marttunen, M., & Haukkala, A. (2015). Self-esteem growth trajectory from adolescence to mid-adulthood and its predictors in adolescence. Advances in Life Course Research, 23, 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Lee, J. Y., Patel, M., & Scior, K. (2023). Self-esteem and its relationship with depression and anxiety in adults with intellectual disabilities: A systematic literature review. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 67(6), 499–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Lintunen, T., Leskinen, E., Oinonen, M., Salinto, M., & Rahkila, P. (1995). Change, reliability, and stability in self-perceptions in early adolescence: A four-year follow-up study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18(2), 351–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Maïano, C., Coutu, S., Morin, A. J. S., Tracey, D., Lepage, G., & Moullec, G. (2019). Self-concept research with school-aged youth with intellectual disabilities: A systematic review. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 32(2), 238–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Mullis, A. K., Mullis, R. L., & Normandin, D. (1992). Cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons of adolescent self-esteem. Adolescence, 27(105), 51. [Google Scholar]
  33. Orth, U., Erol, R. Y., & Luciano, E. C. (2018). Development of self-esteem from age 4 to 94 years: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Orth, U., & Robins, R. W. (2014). The development of self-esteem. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(5), 381–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Widaman, K. F. (2012). Life-span development of self-esteem and its effects on important life outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Orth, U., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Robins, R. W. (2010). Self-esteem development from young adulthood to old age: A cohort-sequential longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4), 645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Park, J.-Y., & Park, E.-Y. (2019). The Rasch analysis of Rosenberg self-esteem scale in individuals with intellectual disabilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Quatman, T., & Watson, C. M. (2001). Gender differences in adolescent self-esteem: An exploration of domains. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 162(1), 93–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Robins, R. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2005). Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 158–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski, K. H., Tracy, J. L., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2002). Global self-esteem across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(3), 423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Journal of Religion and Health. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image. Wesleyan University Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Schalock, R. L., Luckasson, R., & Tassé, M. J. (2021). Twenty questions and answers regarding the 12th edition of the AAIDD manual: Intellectual disability: Definition, diagnosis, classification, and systems of supports (pp. 1–5). American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. [Google Scholar]
  44. Syropoulou, A., Vernadakis, N., Papastergiou, M., & Kourtessis, T. (2021). Psychometric evaluation of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in primary school students with mild intellectual disability: First evidence. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 114, 103964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2013). Development of self-esteem. In Self-esteem (pp. 60–79). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  46. Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2002). Self-esteem and socioeconomic status: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(1), 59–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wallace, J. R., Cunningham, T. F., & Del Monte, V. (1984). Changes in the relationship between self-esteem and locus of control. The Journal of Social Psychology, 124(2), 261–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Three hypothesized latent growth models.
Figure 1. Three hypothesized latent growth models.
Behavsci 15 00655 g001
Figure 2. The relationship between change in self-esteem and demographic characteristics.
Figure 2. The relationship between change in self-esteem and demographic characteristics.
Behavsci 15 00655 g002
Table 1. General characteristics of individuals with intellectual disabilities.
Table 1. General characteristics of individuals with intellectual disabilities.
Categoriesn%χ21st Self-Esteem2nd Self-Esteem3rd Self-Esteem
MSDt/FMSDt/FMSDt/F
Gender
  Male25363.629.307 **31.1019.5222.052 *31.4619.14−0.91033.2220.590.417
  Female14536.4 27.3912.714 33.4321.79 32.2920.84
Age
  15~2920351.0304.965 **30.6017.6380.61832.6719.680.49633.4220.370.362
  30~3910526.4 29.5218.096 31.8219.68 32.4420.40
  40~494912.3 27.4113.873 33.9223.87 34.3123.49
  50~59369.0 29.9719.706 29.6620.13 30.1220.74
  60~6451.3 21.605.128 32.6719.68 26.602.88
Severity of disabilities +
  Level 19624.1186.156 **29.8118.790.24336.7226.303.299 *39.1027.075.536 **
  Level 213734.4 28.9616.58 31.1818.95 31.2718.63
  Level 316541.5 30.3717.38 30.2316.04 30.6217.09
Education level
  Below middle school graduate16842.2153.050 **29.1317.4510.44232.5121.650.91833.0822.040.975
  High school graduate21153.0 29.9617.187 31.8219.18 32.8019.89
  Above college graduate194.8 32.9520.315 33.3917.44 32.0016.96
Comorbid disability
  Yes348.5273.618 **26.9111.753−0.99336.1224.241.17537.5624.991.386
  No36491.5 30.0217.855 31.8119.71 32.4120.15
Residential area
  Big city16842.229.553 **35.4423.17517.090 **36.9624.288.168 **32.1920.154.468 *
  Small and medium-sized city8320.9 24.373.724 27.1113.25 35.0122.45
  Rural14736.9 26.2911.749 29.7617.08 26.7911.25
Marital status
  Unmarried33884.9769.136 **30.3818.4301.25432.6520.590.56433.5121.400.487
  Married4110.3 27.5110.904 29.9517.39 28.8514.90
  Divorce133.3 22.773.940 31.6922.06 32.5420.22
  Bereavement61.5 24.502.74 23.833.87 25.402.07
Note. + Level 1: IQ below 35, severe adaptation difficulties in daily life and lifelong care required; Level 2: IQ 35~49, tasks can be performed with continuous supervision or special training; Level 3: IQ 50~70, social and vocational rehabilitation possible through education; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Model appropriateness for self-esteem.
Table 2. Model appropriateness for self-esteem.
Modelχ2DfNFICFIRMSEA (LO90~HI90)
Linear model21.4620.8360.8440.155 (1.00~1.218)
Second measurement change model0.72520.9941.0000.000 (0.00~0.074)
Third measurement change model61.40320.5240.5250.221 (0.175~0.271)
Note: NFI = normed fit index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation.
Table 3. Estimates of the second measurement model for change in self-esteem.
Table 3. Estimates of the second measurement model for change in self-esteem.
Selected ModelCategoryMeanVarianceCovariance
Second measurement change modelIntercept29.864 ***87.873 ***−62.459 ***
Slope2.777 *237.905 ***
Note: * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Park, E.-Y. Change in Self-Esteem Trajectories Among Adolescents and Adults with Intellectual Disabilities. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 655. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050655

AMA Style

Park E-Y. Change in Self-Esteem Trajectories Among Adolescents and Adults with Intellectual Disabilities. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(5):655. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050655

Chicago/Turabian Style

Park, Eun-Young. 2025. "Change in Self-Esteem Trajectories Among Adolescents and Adults with Intellectual Disabilities" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 5: 655. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050655

APA Style

Park, E.-Y. (2025). Change in Self-Esteem Trajectories Among Adolescents and Adults with Intellectual Disabilities. Behavioral Sciences, 15(5), 655. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050655

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop