Next Article in Journal
Depressive and Anxiety Symptoms, Defense Mechanisms, and Mentalized Affectivity in Individuals with Myocardial Infarction: An Empirical Investigation
Next Article in Special Issue
When Regular Education Is Not Effective and Conflicts Arise Between Systems: The Importance of Independent Educational Evaluations
Previous Article in Journal
Health Decisions Under Uncertainty: The Roles of Conspiracy Beliefs and Institutional Trust
Previous Article in Special Issue
After-School Behaviors, Self-Management, and Parental Involvement as Predictors of Academic Achievement in Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Exploring the Complexity of Children’s Math and Vocabulary Learning: The Role of Cognitive, Dispositional, and Parental Factors

1
Morgridge College of Education, University of Denver, Denver, CO 80210, USA
2
Department of Child Development, California State University, San Bernardino, CA 92407, USA
3
Shanghai Punan Kindergarten, Shanghai 200135, China
4
Department of Early Childhood Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(4), 527; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040527
Submission received: 24 February 2025 / Revised: 6 April 2025 / Accepted: 9 April 2025 / Published: 14 April 2025

Abstract

:
Early mathematical and vocabulary skills serve as critical foundations for academic success, yet the mechanisms underlying their development remain complex. This study examines the role of parents’ education, children’s attentional control, and learning approaches as predictors of kindergarteners’ mathematics and vocabulary performance. Using a sample of 149 children aged 60–72 months in Shanghai, China, we conducted a path analysis to explore direct and indirect relationships among these factors. Findings indicate that parental education indirectly predicts math ability through children’s learning approaches and attentional control, emphasizing the role of both cognitive and behavioral pathways. Conversely, vocabulary development is directly influenced by parental education and learning approaches, suggesting distinct developmental trajectories for math and language acquisition. These results highlight the interconnected nature of cognitive, behavioral, and environmental influences on early academic performance. Implications for early childhood education emphasize the need for targeted interventions that not only engage parents in fostering language-rich and cognitively stimulating environments but also support children’s motivation, persistence, and attentional capacities.

1. Introduction

The start of a child’s formal academic journey brings with it exposure to a variety of new subjects and teachings. Young children come to the academic space with a level of informal knowledge acquired outside the school setting that is enhanced through formal teaching of concepts and skills in school (Levine et al., 2010; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2002; Zippert & Rittle-Johnson, 2020). Among the skills learned within and outside the academic environment, mathematics and vocabulary skills often emerge as critical foundational abilities in the educational setting (Jordan et al., 2009; Nation & Snowling, 2004). Learning basic math concepts such as counting, shapes, patterns, and simple operations helps children develop logical reasoning, problem-solving abilities, and number sense (Jordan et al., 2009; Paliwal & Baroody, 2020; Stock et al., 2007). These skills are essential for more advanced mathematical understanding in later grades (Jordan et al., 2009) and for navigating everyday tasks that require quantitative reasoning. Similarly, a strong vocabulary supports language development, comprehension, and effective communication (Dickinson et al., 2003; Hjetland et al., 2019; Topping et al., 2013). A strong vocabulary enables children to express their thoughts, understand instructions, and engage with stories and discussions (Hoff, 2006). Vocabulary growth in kindergarten is also linked to improved reading comprehension and literacy skills (Hjetland et al., 2019), which are critical for learning across all subjects (Hjetland et al., 2019).
Given the importance of early development in math and vocabulary, many research studies have focused on the factors involved in the acquisition of these skills, working to pinpoint influential mechanisms to explain math and vocabulary achievement (e.g., Blums et al., 2017). Specifically, factors like socioeconomic status (Blums et al., 2017; Hoff, 2006), parenting interactions (Hoff, 2006), exposure to books (Combs & Higgins, 2024; Halle et al., 1997; Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2002), classroom curriculum (Cabell et al., 2025; Montague et al., 2014), cognitive abilities (Gunderson et al., 2012; Swanson, 1999; Zheng et al., 2011), and motivation for learning (Liu et al., 2022; Christodoulou et al., 2024; Tsujimoto et al., 2018) have all been examined in relation to early mathematic and vocabulary development, with findings confirming these factors as important predictors of successes and challenges in both math and vocabulary skills.
While connections between various factors and math and vocabulary achievement are worthy of note, the reality is that levels of complexity within and among these factors make understanding influences on early math and vocabulary development much more nuanced. Long-standing theoretical approaches in the field (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and advanced understanding of nature-nurture interactions (Scarr & McCartney, 1983) highlight this complexity through explanation of the intersectionality between contexts and between personal characteristics and context as an influence on a variety of educational outcomes (Alloway & Alloway, 2010; Looney et al., 2024; Odom et al., 2004). Thus, the more research can employ examinations of complex mechanisms involved in early achievement, the deeper the understanding of the phenomena present.
When it comes to educational outcomes, research has made clear that complex factors contribute to early and later achievement (Costa et al., 2024). Recently, Mindset X Context Theory has guided some of this work to illustrate that educational attainments are a function of both an individual’s personal dispositions and the contextual environments that foster those personal characteristics (Carroll et al., 2023; Walton & Yeager, 2020). That is, the psychological resources one brings to an educational setting (e.g., intrinsic interest, persistence, general motivation) can be enriched or hindered based on contextual influences (e.g., opportunities or constraints) that might be present (Carroll et al., 2023; Miller & Maricle, 2019; Walton & Yeager, 2020). A young child with an inherent interest in numbers or words, for example, might be able to foster that interest in a setting that contains opportunities for conversation, toys that develop numeracy, and exposure to books and other educational materials. Conversely, without those contextual opportunities available, interest in those areas might not be realized. Therefore, exploring both personal attributes and context becomes critical for understanding achievement.
Adding to the complexity of influences on academic success is the role of cognitive processes. Research evidence has shown that cognitive abilities enable students to process, store, and apply information effectively (Baddeley, 2007). Specifically, executive function, working memory, processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and attention facilitate a students’ ability to engage in and sustain tasks that are often correlated with positive academic outcomes (Alloway & Alloway, 2010; Spiegel et al., 2021; Zelazo & Carlson, 2020). That is, when students can plan and problem solve, hold and manipulate information, process information efficiently and accurately, adjust to different educational settings, and attend to academic tasks while filtering out distractions, the more likely they are to accomplish positive academic outcomes (Alloway et al., 2009; Gathercole et al., 2008; Simone et al., 2018). With regard to math and vocabulary skills, numerous research studies have shown a significant positive relationship between these cognitive abilities and math and vocabulary achievement (Noël, 2009; Peng et al., 2016; Peng et al., 2018).
In short, the message that results from these various research findings and theoretical foundations is that the factors that predict math and vocabulary ability are many. While there are studies that employ models and statistical techniques to capture some of this complexity, research is often segmented into various camps. One might argue, for example, that in the extant literature, contextual complexities (e.g., SES, parental involvement, teacher characteristics, environmental stimulation) are studied in tandem with either psychological complexities (e.g., motivation, personality attributes) or with cognitive complexities (e.g., attention, working memory, processing speed). What is more rarely seen (if seen at all) is a linkage of all three areas of complexity, as cognitive and more social-emotional factors (e.g., motivation and personality characteristics that guide the formation of learning approaches) are often viewed as distinct elements and are seldom examined together.
Increasing our understanding of how context, personal social-emotional attributes, and cognitive components work together to influence math and vocabulary skills is warranted. Discussions of complexity within various strands of literature center on such topics as the domain-general or context-dependent nature of cognitive abilities (Doebel & Müller, 2023; Hughes, 2023; Niebaum & Munakata, 2023), the value of person-oriented over variable-oriented motivational research (Spiegel et al., 2021; Viljaranta et al., 2016), and the high degree of connectivity of cognitive and emotional regions of the brain (Pessoa, 2008). Each of these discussions draws attention to the intersectionality of various individual and contextual components to explain how one might excel or struggle in academic settings.

1.1. The Intersection of Personal Attributes, Cognition, and Context

Individuals’ approaches to learning, including motivational qualities (Christodoulou et al., 2024; Viljaranta et al., 2016), personality or temperament traits (Christodoulou et al., 2024), and cognitive abilities (Alloway & Alloway, 2010; Gathercole et al., 2008; Simone et al., 2018), are among some of the characteristics that are related to how a child fares within the academic setting. These psychological orientations and cognitive capacities contribute to a child’s ability to engage with classroom tasks and activities, attend to relevant academic material to enhance learning, persevere through challenging tasks or setbacks, and engage in planning strategies that help to solve problems or complete academic tasks. Research has demonstrated that these resources play an important role in whether a student succeeds or struggles in the educational setting (Christodoulou et al., 2024; Gathercole et al., 2008; Viljaranta et al., 2016). However, contextual factors such as SES, access to resources, parental education levels, school environments, and family support influence both personal attributes and cognitive development (Blums et al., 2017). That is, children from low-SES backgrounds might face stressors that impair their ability to attend to learning or enhance their ability to recognize challenges and rise above them, whereas those in resource-rich environments might benefit from tools that enhance learning and parents that view education as valuable, demonstrating positive attitudes toward learning. Together, personal attributes, cognition, and context create a dynamic interplay of various patterns of development that shape academic outcomes (Carroll et al., 2023).

1.2. The Current Study

With the need to understand how context, personal learning approaches, and cognition work together to contribute to academic outcomes, the current study aimed to examine these elements in relation to children’s early math and vocabulary achievement. Specifically, with the use of path analysis, the current study assessed the direct and indirect impacts of parental education (context), children’s approaches to learning (personal psychological attributes), and attentional control (cognition) on early mathematics and vocabulary achievement.
Parental level of education. The relationship between the parental level of education and children’s academic achievements is a much-studied topic in the literature, demonstrating that how far parents go in their own schooling can significantly influence children’s math and vocabulary development through mechanisms tied to the home environment, parental involvement and beliefs, and provisions of resources afforded to children (Blums et al., 2017; Davis-Kean, 2005; Halle et al., 1997; Tan, 2015). For example, educated parents are more likely to engage in conversations with their children that include a richer and more complex vocabulary (Olaru et al., 2022; Rowe, 2008), exposing children to more advanced language from an early age. Similarly, these parents often incorporate activities like counting and number games into daily routines, thereby fostering early numeracy skills (Douglas & Rittle-Johnson, 2024). Access to resources might also be enhanced for children with educated parents, as higher education is often associated with greater financial stability, allowing for parents to provide educational toys (e.g., books, puzzles) and technology that might further enhance math and vocabulary capabilities (Davis-Kean, 2005; Halle et al., 1997).
Higher levels of parental education are also associated with parenting practices also found to factor into children’s academic achievement. For instance, studies have found that mothers with higher educational attainment are more likely to actively participate in their children’s education by reading to their children more regularly (Yarosz & Barnett, 2001), helping them with math and reading homework (Fantuzzo et al., 2000), or taking an active role in the school environment (e.g., attending school events, helping out with classroom activities, communicating with teachers), all of which is important for children’s academic achievement (Burchinal et al., 2002; Davis-Kean, 2005; El Nokali et al., 2010).
Learning Approaches. Children’s approaches to learning are varied and complex. Factors such as interest in a topic area, initiative to seek out learning opportunities, value of the task, perseverance through difficult tasks, and awareness of task goals act as drivers of engagement, effort, and motivation in academic tasks (Anthony & Ogg, 2019; Eccles & Wigfield, 2020; Viljaranta et al., 2016). Research in these areas tends to focus on a specific aspect of a learning approach (e.g., interest in a task or perseverance) rather than viewing them as a pattern of a child’s learning characteristics. For instance, studies have demonstrated that when children are interested in a topic, they are more likely to explore it on a deeper level, ask more questions related to the topic, and seek out information about the topic, all of which can contribute to increased achievement in the topic area (Christodoulou et al., 2024; Fung et al., 2018; Ma, 2022). Similarly, because the academic environment often presents various challenges (e.g., complex tasks), children who demonstrate perseverance—particularly with regard to math or vocabulary—are more likely to achieve academic success relative to counterparts who might give up easily (Fung et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2024). Therefore, on their own, various approaches to learning have a clear link to academic achievement. However, more recent research (Viljaranta et al., 2016) has highlighted the importance of examining patterns of learning, as various profiles can perhaps demonstrate different relationships to achievement (Christodoulou et al., 2024; Ma, 2022; Viljaranta et al., 2016). That is, examining how certain approaches to learning hang together in a particular profile might help to further highlight the complexity of person-oriented approaches to learning and their relationship to math and vocabulary achievement.
Attentional control. Controlling attention on a task requires one to inhibit a focus on distractors (Engle, 2002). When children can focus attention, sustain attention on a given task, manage distractions, and shift attention when needed, this level of control can play a role in the successful completion of academic tasks such as math and vocabulary (Isbell et al., 2018; Orbach & Fritz, 2022; Sánchez-Pérez et al., 2015). Specifically, a greater ability to control attention when engaged in math and vocabulary tasks allows for greater processing of information related to academic assignments (Isbell et al., 2018; Li et al., 2023), a higher likelihood of understanding mathematical patterns and new words (Carretti et al., 2009; Li et al., 2023), and an enhancement of comprehension and recall (Carretti et al., 2009). Being able to shift attention allows children to shift focus to different learning or problem-solving strategies when challenges occur (Isbell et al., 2018; Li et al., 2023), and an ability to reduce distractions helps children to attend to relevant instructional information and concentrate on relevant aspects of a learning problem (e.g., pronunciation or comprehension of a word, solving a math problem), without being disrupted with irrelevant information (Isbell et al., 2018).
Parental education, learning approaches, and attentional control. As has been previously stated, each of these variables has been established as important for academic achievement but has rarely (if at all) been studied together. Given what we know about the complexity of factors associated with children’s ability to achieve in subject areas, the current study sought to examine how context (parental education), learning approaches (personal attributes), and attentional control (cognition) work together to impact children’s math and vocabulary ability (see Figure 1). Specifically, we utilized contextual and ecological theoretical models (e.g., Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Model, Mindset X Context Theory) to examine pathways to illustrate how more distal influences (like parental education) exert their effects indirectly through more proximal processes. Parental education represents a contextual factor that can significantly shape the resources, opportunities, and stressors children encounter. These environmental influences can affect children’s learning approaches and cognitive processes which, in turn, impact their academic outcomes. Thus, our conceptual mediation model seeks to capture the dynamic, multi-level interactions described by theoretical frameworks, offering a more holistic and complex understanding of how these various factors operate together to impact vocabulary and math performance.
To that end, the following hypotheses were tested:
H1: 
Parental education has an indirect effect on children’s math ability and children’s vocabulary via children’s learning approaches and attentional control.
H2: 
Parental education has a direct effect on children’s math ability and vocabulary.
H3: 
Children’s learning approaches and attentional control mediate the effect of parental education on academic performance.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

One hundred and forty-nine children and their families were randomly selected from 60- to 72-month-old classrooms in Shanghai, China (M = 67.44 months, SD = 3.73). Among the 149 child participants, 86 were boys and 63 were girls. All participating children were monolingual (in Mandarin) and of Han ethnicity.

2.2. Procedure

The current study is a cross-sectional study. The procedure followed the protocol approved by the University Institutional Review Board. The parents of participants gave their consent for themselves and their children’s participation and filled out the survey questionnaire, which included parental education information. Teachers of participants agreed to fill out a test questionnaire about participants. Two graduate students were trained to administer the measures. Participating children were assessed in a quiet room in their kindergarten.

2.3. Measures

Three measures were used to assess participants’ attention score, math ability, and vocabulary ability. Each participant completed all three tests within one session.
The child ANT. The Child Attention Network Test (ANT) assesses attentional processes in children, which is a test developed on the E-prime platform (Ruead et al., 2004). Children are presented with visual stimuli (e.g., arrows or fish) and respond based on specific task instructions. Research indicates that children perform best in tests with story contexts and outcome feedback (Berger et al., 2000). Therefore, in the child version of ANT, colorful fish are used instead of arrows in the Flanker task. The experimenter invites the children to help feed the middle fish, instructing them to press the corresponding directional button based on the direction in which the middle fish swims. Participants were required to respond to all stimuli that were displayed on a computer screen by clicking the mouse. Each trial began with a central fixation cross. The target array was a yellow-colored line drawing of either a single yellow fish or a horizontal row of five yellow fish, presented above or below fixation, over a blue-green background. The participant was to respond based on whether the central fish was pointing to the left or right by pressing the corresponding left or right key on the mouse. On congruent trials the flanking fish were pointing in the same direction, on incongruent trials the flankers pointed in the opposite direction from the central fish, and on neutral trials the central fish appeared alone (Fan et al., 2002). All the participants were regularly exposed to technology in their kindergarten classroom and thus were familiar with computers and mouse-clicking. Participants had a practice session for about 3 min, within which they received the experimenter’s response and encouragement. Following the practice session, the formal experiment began, and the children no longer received feedback from the experimenter. The attentional control score was computed as the participant’s median reaction time for each flanker condition (across cue conditions) and subtracted the congruent from the incongruent reaction time. Thus, a small or negative attentional control score suggests the child is better at handling interference. The attentional control had the highest test-retest reliability (r = 0.77) among the three attention networks (Fan et al., 2002).
Test of Early Mathematics Ability—Third Edition (TEMA-3; Ginsburg & Baroody, 2003). The TEMA-3 is a standardized test designed to measure mathematical ability in children between 3 years 0 months and 8 years 11 months. The test was composed of 72 items measuring informal and formal knowledge of both concepts and skills in a variety of domains (Ginsburg & Baroody, 2003). Informal mathematical knowledge is acquired outside the context of schooling, and it underlies the basic mathematical knowledge that is taught in school, whereas formal mathematical knowledge represents the concepts and skills that children learn in school (Ginsburg & Baroody, 2003). Each participant’s binary answers (pass/fail) were recorded on a form. This test has internal consistency alphas equal to or above 0.94 for the different age intervals (Ginsburg & Baroody, 2003). The Chinese version of the TEMA-3 demonstrated good reliability and validity. Specifically, the internal consistency was high, with a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.932, indicating strong cross-indicator consistency. The split-half reliability reached 0.747, reflecting satisfactory internal consistency across test items. Additionally, the test-retest reliability was 0.845, suggesting strong measurement stability over time (Kang et al., 2014). For this study, the raw score on the TEMA-3 was used as the math outcome.
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised Edition (PPVT-R). PPVT-R is the Chinese version adapted from PPVT-IV, measuring receptive vocabulary skills (Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The PPVT-IV demonstrates good reliability and validity (Wing-Yin Chow & McBride-Change, 2003; Dunn & Dunn, 1997). Lu and Liu (1998) developed the PPVT-R and reported split-half reliability ranging from 0.90 to 0.97, indicating excellent internal consistency among the test items. The participant was shown a card with four pictures, and the assessor read a word and asked the child to point to the picture that corresponds to the word. A standardized score was computed that reflected the extent to which each child’s performance compared to the expected performance of same-age children in the norming population.
Parental Education. Parental education was scored as the highest education degree from parents (Santelli et al., 2000). Parental education was scored as 1 = junior school and below, 2 = high school, 3 = associate degree, and 4 = bachelor’s degree and above.
Learning approach observational assessment. Learning approach observational assessment is a checklist developed and adapted by a group of Chinese researchers aimed at assessing 10 aspects of children’s learning, including interest, initiation, concentration, persistence, resistance, information use, reflection and explanation, exploration, goals, and independence (Xu et al., 2016). Each aspect has five levels of performance, with specific behavioral descriptions corresponding to each level. Level 5 represents the highest performance, while Level 1 represents the lowest. The internal consistency of the checklist was adequate (Cronbach’s alpha α = 0.94). The internal consistency of each aspect was greater than 0.93. To further test the reliability of this measure, considering that the reporters of this measure were the teachers, a random kindergarten teacher was invited to observe children’s behavior and rate it using this measurement alongside the researcher but separately. The Kendall’s Coefficient of Concordance (W) between the two raters was 0.828, indicating a high level of reliability.
Data were analyzed first using SEM techniques and Mplus 8 software (Muthen & Muthen, 2011) with a bootstrap approach (Shrout & Bolger, 2002). To assess the adequacy of our sample size, a post hoc power analysis was conducted. With N = 149 and α = 0.05, the power to detect large effects (f2 = 0.35) was 98.9%. Thus, the study is sufficiently powered for large effects. Bootstrapping was used to provide a practical approximation of sampling distributions of indirect effects to produce confidence intervals (CI) of estimates. An indirect effect is different from zero when the confidence interval does not contain zero. In the current study, we performed a nonparametric resampling method with 1000 resamples drawn to derive the 95% CIs for the indirect effect of parental education on math and on vocabulary through learning approach and executive attention. Multiple indices, including the Comparative Fit Index and Tucker Lewis Index (CFI & TLI; Bentler, 1990), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1993), and the Standard Root Mean Residual (SRMR; Hu & Bentler, 1999), were used to assess global model fit. To determine good model–data fit, recommended cutoffs are >0.90 for the CFI and TLI and <0.10 for the RMSEA and SRMR (Kline, 2011; Mueller & Hancock, 2010). Data analyses were conducted in two phases. First, preliminary analyses were conducted, and descriptive statistics were obtained to determine whether the data met the basic assumptions of SEM. The second phase of data analysis was to test the mediation path model (see Figure 1).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The descriptive statistics and bivariate relationships among variables were analyzed by SPSS 25. The estimates are reported in Table 1. All statistical assumptions were met. There are significant correlations among variables. Attentional control (for which higher levels indicate an increased time to process incongruent situations) negatively correlated to all the other variables, indicating that the higher attentional control scores were associated with lower levels of parental education (r = −0.217, p < 0.01) and lower scores for learning approach (r = −0.196, p < 0.05), math (r = −0.291, p < 0.01), and vocabulary (r = −0.221, p < 0.01). The learning approach is highly positively correlated to math ability (r = 0.82, p < 0.01). However, the learning approach was measured by a more domain-general observational tool, while math was measured by a more domain-specific (math content) performing the task. They are considered as two distinct measurements. There were no concerns about the multicollinearity (rs < 0.70) of distinct predictors within the model.

3.2. Path Model

The path model was tested by Mplus 8 with a bootstrap approach. The “full information” method that estimates all parameters simultaneously, also known as maximum likelihood (ML) estimation, was used for this non-recursive path model as a default. Estimation of this model is a just-identified model with a perfect global fit, CFI = 1.000, TLI = 1.000, RMSEA = 0.000, and SRMR = 0.000. Unstandardized parameter estimates and 95% confidence intervals are shown in Table 2. This path model explained approximately 70% variance in math, 24.9% variance in vocabulary, 21.4% variance in learning approach, and only 4.5% variance in attentional control.
Tested model with standardized estimates are reported in Figure 2. The 95%CI [2.006, 4.317] for the indirect effect of parental education level on children’ math ability via learning approach does not contain zero. Thus, parental education level impacts children’s math ability by fostering their learning approach. Simultaneously, the 95%CI [0.005, 0.503] for the indirect effect of parental education level on children’s math ability via attentional control does not contain zero, either. However, the 95%CI [−0.240, 1.630] for the direct effect of parental education level on children’ math ability does contain a zero, which means that there is no direct impact from parental education level on children’s math ability. With regard to children’s vocabulary ability, parental education level has a direct impact on vocabulary and an indirect path through learning approaches rather than attentional control. There is no co-variance between the learning approach and attentional control, but there is a co-variance between math and vocabulary within this path model.
To assess the robustness of our findings, we conducted an additional linear regression predicting math performance while controlling for vocabulary ability. The results indicated that vocabulary (b = 0.19, p < 0.001) and learning approach (b = 3.20, p < 0.001) were significant predictors of math performance, while attentional control was not (p = 0.581). Parental education remained a significant predictor (b = 2.56, p = 0.011). The model accounted for approximately 39% of the variance in math scores (R2 = 0.389), supporting the robustness of vocabulary and learning approach in early academic development.

4. Discussion

The goal of the present study was to test the complexity of factors (contextual, personal-psychological attributes, and cognitive) that impact math and vocabulary indices. Specifically, we implemented path analysis to examine the direct and indirect effects of parental education, attentional control, and learning approaches on math and vocabulary measures among children. As hypothesized, we provide evidence that these variables impact at least one of the academic outcomes (i.e., math & vocabulary).
An important main finding in the current study is that parents’ level of education indirectly predicted math ability through a learning approach. This corroborates previous research showing that math development is best explained by a complex set of factors as opposed to being determined by one sole influencer (Costa et al., 2024). In this case, math ability was greatly impacted by one’s learning approach, which was demonstrated by numerous traits, including interests, initiation, concentration, persistence, resistance, use of reflection and explanation, exploration, goals, and independence. This finding aligns with numerous research studies that have found connections between these various personality and motivational factors and math and vocabulary achievement (e.g., Christodoulou et al., 2024; Ma, 2022; Zhang et al., 2024) and further corroborates work that examines these psychological orientations as patterns or profiles (rather than one single trait) that might show variability among learners (e.g., Anthony & Ogg, 2019; Viljaranta et al., 2016). Further, our results demonstrated that the traits that define one’s learning approach were influenced by the home environment, measured by parents’ level of education. That is, as theory and research have argued, characteristics found in various learning approaches intersect and can be enhanced or hindered by the contexts in which children find themselves (Carroll et al., 2023). In this case, parents with higher levels of education might have created environments in which these learning approaches could be enriched. Such findings align with the understanding that children’s math performance is a multidetermined construct impacted by both contextual and personal psychological factors. These findings fit well with prominent systems models of development (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979).
Another important finding from the current study is that parental education negatively predicted children’s attention control. Specifically, higher levels of parent education were associated with faster performance on the child attention network test. This suggests that the parents’ level of education may be associated with parents setting up a home environment that is conducive to fostering cognitive abilities, such as access to books, play activities, and other instructionally oriented pieces that have been shown to spark healthy cognitive functioning in children (Sakib, 2022). Further, parents’ level of education also indirectly impacted math ability through children’s attention control. This is a notable finding suggesting that children’s math ability is not only dependent on one’s learning approach but also the child’s cognitive abilities, both of which are directly impacted by parent’s level of education. This set of findings continues to add to the broader theme that child outcomes are best explained through a complex lens.
For children’s vocabulary, the findings show that parental education had a direct impact. Years of language development research can serve to explain this finding. It is well documented that children’s language starts to develop in utero months before a child is born (Mariani et al., 2023). Fetuses listen to the mother’s voice from early on, and newborns can recognize all syllables in the languages spoken to them. Following birth, children are exposed to a myriad of experiences that bolster this language development. Parental interactions, exposure to language-rich environments, and engagement with media all contribute to children’s language development, with parents serving as the primary source of these experiences. Given the strong association between parental education and the richness of linguistic input (Olaru et al., 2022; Rowe, 2008), as well as the provision of educational resources (Davis-Kean, 2005; Halle et al., 1997), it is reasonable to expect that parental education would directly influence children’s language development beyond other contributing factors.
Similar to the math-related findings, learning approaches were shown to be important dispositions to vocabulary development. Learning dispositions, encompassing traits such as persistence, exploration, and goal setting, significantly predicted vocabulary performance in the current sample. These findings align with research suggesting that positive learning behaviors facilitate cognitive engagement and exposure to language-rich experiences, which are essential for vocabulary acquisition (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2002). Dispositions such as curiosity and reflection may promote active learning opportunities, where children are more likely to seek out, process, and retain new vocabulary (Claxton & Carr, 2004). Thus, the positive relationship between learning dispositions and vocabulary highlights the importance of fostering learning-oriented behaviors that support language development during early childhood.
Taken together, the current results show that the paths to foster math and vocabulary are different. Parental education was shown to indirectly impact math skills through learning approaches and executive functions, while vocabulary was directly impacted by parental education as well as learning approaches. This offers important insight for understanding the developmental trajectories of these academic outcomes and the complexity of the factors that impact them. The differential pathways through which parental education influences math and vocabulary performance provide compelling insights into the nuanced mechanisms driving early academic outcomes. For math, the mediation through learning approaches and executive functions suggests that parental education fosters higher-order cognitive processes and behaviors, such as persistence (part of learning approaches) and attention control, which in turn support math performance (Blair & Razza, 2007; Ursache et al., 2012). This aligns with evidence highlighting the complex nature of math as requiring both behavioral regulation and domain-specific learning characteristics. In contrast, vocabulary development was directly influenced by parental education and learning approaches, underscoring the foundational role of enriched linguistic environments provided by more educated parents (Hoff, 2006; Hart & Risley, 1995). The absence of mediation in vocabulary suggests that exposure to language and rich verbal interactions in the household may be sufficient to shape children’s language outcomes without the heavy reliance on intermediary cognitive processes. These findings reveal the critical role of parents not only in cultivating language-rich interactions but also in fostering dispositions and core cognitive abilities that are instrumental for broader academic success.
There are a few shortcomings of the present study worth highlighting. First, the construct of the learning approach studied here is a conglomerate of several learning characteristics. By nature, this diminishes the construct specificity of this variable. In other words, it is difficult to pinpoint which of the components of the learning approach is most or least influential to the set of variables in this study. While recent research has highlighted the value of studying patterns of learning profiles (Christodoulou et al., 2024; Ma, 2022; Viljaranta et al., 2016), studies following up this work would also benefit from a more refined measure of learning approach, as understanding these variables individually and as a pattern is beneficial. Although parental education level is often used as a proxy for SES, we acknowledge that more granular SES indicators—such as exact years of schooling or household income—could offer improved precision. Our focus was strictly on parental education (and, therefore, these SES indicators were not collected in the current study), but robust SES data would strengthen future research in examining how these contextual elements impact the relationship between these variables. Third, a consideration of the current study is that the path model was just identified, which precludes the use of global fit indices to evaluate model adequacy. While this was intentional—given our goal to test a specific, theory-driven structure—future research could explore alternative or nested models to further assess the robustness and generalizability of the proposed relationships. Last, while path analysis offers structural insight into understanding the complex nature of children’s academic outcomes and the context in which they develop, future work should attempt to explore other analyses like structural equation modeling or network analysis that lend themselves well to examining broad complex developmental structures.
Future studies in this vein of work should attempt to address these questions longitudinally to better understand the developmental trajectory of parental education, learning approach, attention control, math ability, and vocabulary. In addition, prospective studies should explore subgroup analyses (e.g., gender and age) to examine how these effects might vary across stratified subgroups, as this would provide more nuanced information regarding the influences on math ability across demographics. Further, there are a myriad of other variables germane to math and vocabulary, for example, school influences and peer relationships, that would be worthy of study. Moreover, this set of variables would be critical to study within the school context to determine how the role of teachers may affect these outcomes.
Overall, the findings from this study illustrate the complex interplay between parental education, children’s learning approaches, and attentional control in molding early academic outcomes, putting forth a multifaceted understanding of how these factors collectively influence math and vocabulary achievement. Consistent with prior research, parental education emerged as a foundational context, directly supporting vocabulary development through enriched language environments while indirectly influencing math performance via the mediation of learning approaches and attentional control. These results affirm the importance of examining academic achievement as a multidetermined construct, where context, personal dispositions, and cognitive processes converge to influence developmental trajectories. Importantly, this study highlights the unique mechanisms underlying math and vocabulary performance, accentuating the need for targeted interventions that not only engage parents in fostering language-rich and cognitively stimulating environments but also support children’s motivation, persistence, and attentional capacities. By emphasizing these integrated pathways, this work advances the field’s understanding of early academic success and underscores the critical role of parents in cultivating both the cognitive and behavioral foundations necessary for long-term achievement. These insights provide a framework for future research and practice aimed at optimizing children’s academic outcomes in diverse contexts.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Z.L., K.C., K.P.R., L.L. and X.Z.; Methodology, Z.L., K.C., K.P.R., J.X., L.L. and X.Z.; Software, Z.L. and K.C.; Validation, K.C.; Formal analysis, K.C.; Investigation, Z.L. and J.X.; Resources, Z.L., K.C., K.P.R., J.X. and X.Z.; Data curation, Z.L. and J.X.; Writing—original draft, K.C.; Writing—review & editing, Z.L., K.C., K.P.R. and L.L.; Visualization, K.C., K.P.R. and L.L.; Supervision, X.Z.; Project administration, X.Z.; Funding acquisition, X.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China Humanities and Social Sciences Grant [11YJAZH130].

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China (11YJAZH130, approval date: 6 September 2011).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Kirkwood, H., & Elliott, J. (2009). The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory. Child Development, 80(2), 606–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Anthony, C. J., & Ogg, J. (2019). Parent involvement, approaches to learning, and student achievement: Examining longitudinal mediation. School Psychology, 34(4), 376–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action (Vol. 45). OuP Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Berger, A., Jones, L., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Computerized games to study the development of attention in childhood. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(2), 297–303. [Google Scholar]
  7. Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Blums, A., Belsky, J., Grimm, K., & Chen, Z. (2017). Building links between early socioeconomic status, cognitive ability, and math and science achievement. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(1), 16–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). Contexts of child rearing: Problems and prospects. American Psychologist, 34(10), 844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  11. Burchinal, M. R., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Pianta, R., & Howes, C. (2002). Development of academic skills from preschool through second grade: Family and classroom predictors of developmental trajectories. Journal of School Psychology, 40(5), 415–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cabell, S. Q., Kim, J. S., White, T. G., Gale, C. J., Edwards, A. A., Hwang, H., Petscher, Y., & Raines, R. M. (2025). Impact of a content-rich literacy curriculum on kindergarteners’ vocabulary, listening comprehension, and content knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 117(2), 153–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Carretti, B., Borella, E., Cornoldi, C., & De Beni, R. (2009). Role of working memory in explaining the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension difficulties: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(2), 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Carroll, J. M., Yeager, D. S., Buontempo, J., Hecht, C., Cimpian, A., Mhatre, P., Muller, C., & Crosnoe, R. (2023). Mindset × context: Schools, classrooms, and the unequal translation of expectations into math achievement. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 88(2), 7–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Christodoulou, A., Tsagkaridis, K., & Malegiannaki, A.-C. (2024). A multifactorial model of intrinsic/environmental motivators, personal traits and their combined influences on math performance in elementary school. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 39(4), 4113–4135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Claxton, G., & Carr, M. (2004). A framework for teaching learning: The dynamics of disposition. Early Years, 24(1), 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Combs, S., & Higgins, K. N. (2024). The relationship between shared picturebook reading and language development in young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 52(7), 1725–1735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Costa, A., Moreira, D., Casanova, J., Azevedo, Â., Gonçalves, A., Oliveira, Í., Azevedo, R., & Dias, P. C. (2024). Determinants of academic achievement from the middle to secondary school education: A systematic review. Social Psychology of Education, 27(6), 3533–3572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Davis-Kean, P. E. (2005). The influence of parent education and family income on child achievement: The indirect role of parental expectations and the home environment. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 294–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Doebel, S., & Müller, U. (2023). The future of research on executive function and its development: An introduction to the special issue. Journal of Cognition and Development, 24(2), 161–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Douglas, A.-A., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2024). Parental early math support: The role of parental knowledge about early math development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 66, 124–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test--Third edition (PPVT-III) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. El Nokali, N. E., Bachman, H. J., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2010). Parent involvement and children’s academic and social development in elementary school. Child Development, 81(3), 988–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Fantuzzo, J., Tighe, E., & Childs, S. (2000). Family involvement questionnaire: A multivariate assessment of family participation in early childhood education. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fung, F., Tan, C. Y., & Chen, G. (2018). Student engagement and mathematics achievement: Unraveling main and interactive effects. Psychology in the Schools, 55(7), 815–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Kirkwood, H. J., Elliott, J. G., Holmes, J., & Hilton, K. A. (2008). Attentional and executive function behaviours in children with poor working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(2), 214–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ginsburg, H. P., & Baroody, A. J. (2003). Test of early mathematics ability (3rd ed.). Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1229–1241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Halle, T. G., Kurtz-Costes, B., & Mahoney, J. L. (1997). Family influences on school achievement in low-income, African American children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Hart, B., Risley, T. R., & Kirby, J. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Canadian Journal of Education, 22(3), 323. [Google Scholar]
  35. Hjetland, H. N., Lervåg, A., Lyster, S.-A. H., Hagtvet, B. E., Hulme, C., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2019). Pathways to reading comprehension: A longitudinal study from 4 to 9 years of age. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(5), 751–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hughes, C. (2023). Executive functions: Going places at pace. Journal of Cognition and Development, 24(2), 296–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Isbell, E., Calkins, S. D., Swingler, M. M., & Leerkes, E. M. (2018). Attentional fluctuations in preschoolers: Direct and indirect relations with task accuracy, academic readiness, and school performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 388–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: Kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 850–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kang, D., Zhou, X., Tian, L., Li, Z., & Xu, J. (2014). On the applicability of test of early child mathematics ability (Chinese edition) among 5–6 year-old children in Shanghai. Early Childhood Education (Education Sciences), 2014(6), 39–45. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Levine, S. C., Suriyakham, L. W., Rowe, M. L., Huttenlocher, J., & Gunderson, E. A. (2010). What counts in the development of young children’s number knowledge? Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1309–1319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Li, T., Quintero, M., Galvan, M., Shanafelt, S., Hasty, L. M., Spangler, D. P., Lyons, I. M., Mazzocco, M. M. M., Brockmole, J. R., Hart, S. A., & Wang, Z. (2023). The mediating role of attention in the association between math anxiety and math performance: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 115(2), 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Liu, A. S., Rutherford, T., & Karamarkovich, S. M. (2022). Numeracy, cognitive, and motivational predictors of elementary mathematics achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(7), 1589–1607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Looney, L., Wong, E. H., Rosales, K. P., Bacon, J. M., & Wiest, D. J. (2024). Supporting learning differences: Effects of cognitive training on cognitive abilities in a school-based sample. Education Sciences, 14(1), 89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lu, L., & Liu, H. (1998). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  48. Ma, Y. (2022). Profiles of student science attitudes and its associations with gender and science achievement. International Journal of Science Education, 44(11), 1876–1895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mariani, B., Nicoletti, G., Barzon, G., Ortiz Barajas, M. C., Shukla, M., Guevara, R., Suweis, S. S., & Gervain, J. (2023). Prenatal experience with language shapes the brain. Science Advances, 9(47), eadj3524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Miller, D. C., & Maricle, D. E. (2019). Essentials of school neuropsychological assessment. John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  51. Montague, M., Krawec, J., Enders, C., & Dietz, S. (2014). The effects of cognitive strategy instruction on math problem solving of middle-school students of varying ability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(2), 469–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Mueller, R. O., & Hancock, G. R. (2010). Structural equation modeling. In G. R. Hancock, & R. O. Mueller (Eds.), The reviewer’s guide to quantitative methods in the social sciences (pp. 371–383). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  53. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2011). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  54. Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Niebaum, J. C., & Munakata, Y. (2023). Why doesn’t executive function training improve academic achievement? Rethinking individual differences, relevance, and engagement from a contextual framework. Journal of Cognition and Development, 24(2), 241–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Noël, M.-P. (2009). Counting on working memory when learning to count and to add: A preschool study. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1630–1643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Odom, S. L., Vitztum, J., Wolery, R., Lieber, J., Sandall, S., Hanson, M. J., Beckman, P., Schwartz, I., & Horn, E. (2004). Preschool inclusion in the United States: A review of research from an ecological systems perspective. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 4(1), 17–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Olaru, G., Robitzsch, A., Hildebrandt, A., & Schroeders, U. (2022). Examining moderators of vocabulary acquisition from kindergarten through elementary school using local structural equation modeling. Learning and Individual Differences, 95, 102136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Orbach, L., & Fritz, A. (2022). Patterns of attention and anxiety in predicting arithmetic fluency among school-aged children. Brain Sciences, 12(3), 376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Paliwal, V., & Baroody, A. J. (2020). Cardinality principle understanding: The role of focusing on the subitizing ability. ZDM, 52(4), 649–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., Dardick, W., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 48–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 455–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029–1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Sakib, N. (2022). The effect of play-based learning on the cognitive development of kindergarten students. Cultural Communication Journal, 10(1), 40–58. [Google Scholar]
  67. Santelli, J. S., Lowry, R., Brener, N. D., & Robin, L. (2000). The association of sexual behaviors with socioeconomic status, family structure, and race/ethnicity among US adolescents. American Journal of Public Health, 90(10), 1582. [Google Scholar]
  68. Sánchez-Pérez, N., Fuentes, L. J., Pina, V., López-López, J. A., & González-Salinas, C. (2015). How do different components of Effortful Control contribute to children’s mathematics achievement? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype→ environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435. [Google Scholar]
  70. Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7, 422–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Simone, A. N., Marks, D. J., Bédard, A.-C., & Halperin, J. M. (2018). Low working memory rather than ADHD symptoms predicts poor academic achievement in school-aged children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(2), 277–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Spiegel, J. A., Goodrich, J. M., Morris, B. M., Osborne, C. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2021). Relations between executive functions and academic outcomes in elementary school children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 329–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Stock, P., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2007). Early markers for arithmetic difficulties. Educational and Child Psychology, 24(2), 28–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading comprehension and working memory in learning-disabled readers: Is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(1), 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Tan, C. Y. (2015). The contribution of cultural capital to students’ mathematics achievement in medium and high socioeconomic gradient economies. British Educational Research Journal, 41(6), 1050–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Topping, K., Dekhinet, R., & Zeedyk, S. (2013). Parent–infant interaction and children’s language development. Educational Psychology, 33(4), 391–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Tsujimoto, K. C., Frijters, J. C., Boada, R., Gottwald, S., Hill, D., Jacobson, L. A., Lovett, M. W., Mark Mahone, E., Willcutt, E. G., Wolf, M., Bosson-Heenan, J., & Gruen, J. R. (2018). Achievement attributions are associated with specific rather than general learning delays. Learning and Individual Differences, 64, 8–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achievement in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Viljaranta, J., Aunola, K., & Hirvonen, R. (2016). Motivation and academic performance among first-graders: A person-oriented approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 49, 366–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Walton, G. M., & Yeager, D. S. (2020). Seed and soil: Psychological affordances in contexts help to explain where wise interventions succeed or fail. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Wing-Yin Chow, B., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Promoting language and literacy development through parent–child reading in Hong Kong preschoolers. Early Education and Development, 14(2), 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Xu, J., Li, Z., & Zhou, X. (2016). The impact of learning qualities on early mathematical abilities in 5–6-year-old children. Early Childhood Education (Educational Science), 1(2), 69–75. [Google Scholar]
  84. Yarosz, D. J., & Barnett, W. S. (2001). Who reads to young children?: Identifying predictors of family reading activities. Reading Psychology, 22(1), 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: Implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(3), 273–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Zhang, J., Yang, X., Yu, X., Xu, J., Jiang, J., & Chen, Y. (2024). Longitudinal cognitive correlates of advanced mathematical performance in primary school children. Current Psychology, 43(5), 4155–4167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Zheng, X., Swanson, H. L., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2011). Working memory components as predictors of children’s mathematical word problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(4), 481–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Zippert, E. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2020). The home math environment: More than numeracy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual mediation model.
Figure 1. Conceptual mediation model.
Behavsci 15 00527 g001
Figure 2. Tested model with standardized estimates.
Figure 2. Tested model with standardized estimates.
Behavsci 15 00527 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and pairwise correlations.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and pairwise correlations.
Mean (SD)N12345
1. Parental Education3.99 (1.24)149-
2. Attentional Control2.90 (3.49)149−0.217 **
3. Learning Approach33.77 (7.15)1280.454 **−0.196 *-
4. Math38.00 (10.69)1490.456 **−0.291 **0.821 **-
5. Vocabulary54.24 (18.79)1490.375 **−0.221 **0.442 **0.502 **-
p < 0.05 *, p < 0.01 **.
Table 2. Unstandardized parameter estimates (and SEs) and confidence intervals.
Table 2. Unstandardized parameter estimates (and SEs) and confidence intervals.
Unstandardized95%CI
EstimateSEp-ValueLower 2.5%Upper 2.5%
Direct path
Parental educationMath0.7310.4630.114−0.2401.630
Vocabulary3.1381.2270.0110.7705.438
Learning approach2.6940.4360.0001.8493.583
Attentional control−0.5990.2810.033−1.169−0.023
Learning approachMath1.1380.0910.0000.9611.319
Vocabulary0.8600.2360.0000.4071.337
Attentional controlMath−0.2570.1240.039−0.526−0.008
Vocabulary−0.5200.4080.202−1.3180.306
Indirect path
Parental education—Learning approachMath3.0660.5730.0002.0064.317
Vocabulary2.3180.7300.0021.1654.092
Parental education—Attentional controlMath0.1540.1120.1710.0050.503
Vocabulary0.3120.2980.295−0.0961.178
R-square
Math0.7000.0470.000
Vocabulary0.2490.0580.000
Learning approach0.2140.0600.000
Attentional control0.0450.0460.319
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, Z.; Chen, K.; Rosales, K.P.; Xu, J.; Looney, L.; Zhou, X. Exploring the Complexity of Children’s Math and Vocabulary Learning: The Role of Cognitive, Dispositional, and Parental Factors. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 527. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040527

AMA Style

Li Z, Chen K, Rosales KP, Xu J, Looney L, Zhou X. Exploring the Complexity of Children’s Math and Vocabulary Learning: The Role of Cognitive, Dispositional, and Parental Factors. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):527. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040527

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Zhengqing, Keting Chen, Kevin P. Rosales, Jingjing Xu, Lisa Looney, and Xin Zhou. 2025. "Exploring the Complexity of Children’s Math and Vocabulary Learning: The Role of Cognitive, Dispositional, and Parental Factors" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 527. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040527

APA Style

Li, Z., Chen, K., Rosales, K. P., Xu, J., Looney, L., & Zhou, X. (2025). Exploring the Complexity of Children’s Math and Vocabulary Learning: The Role of Cognitive, Dispositional, and Parental Factors. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 527. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040527

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop