Speech Production Development in Mandarin-Speaking Children: A Case of Lingual Stop Consonants
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Developmental Research on Children’s VOT Production in Other Languages
1.2. Developmental Research on VOT in Mandarin Chinese
1.3. Purposes and Predictions
1.4. “Robustness of Contrast” Measures of Phonological Development
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Task
2.2. Measurements and Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Age-Related Difference in VOT Means and Distributions
3.2. Sex-Related Difference in VOT Produced by Adults and Children
3.3. “Robustness of Contrast” Measures and the Issue of “Overshoot”
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Image of the Computer Screen for the Word Elicitation Task
Appendix B. The Word List for the Lingual Stops
Target Consonant | |||||
/t/ | /d/ | /k/ | /g/ | ||
Vowel context | /i/ | /thi.tsi/ | /tiŋ.tsi/ | ||
梯子 “ladder” | 钉子 “nail” | ||||
/thiŋ.tsi/ | /ti.thu/ | ||||
亭子 “pavilion” | 地图 “map” | ||||
/a/ | /thao.tsi/ | /ta. ɕaŋ/ | /khan.piŋ/ | /kao.lou/ | |
桃子 “peach” | 大象 “elephant” | 看病 “seeing a doctor” | 高楼 “high-rise building” | ||
/thaŋ. khai / | /tan. kao/ | /kha. thʂə/ | /kaŋ.pi/ | ||
糖块 “candy” | 蛋糕 “cake” | 卡车 “truck” | 钢笔 “pen” | ||
/u/ | /thu.tsi/ | /tu. tsi / | /khun.lə/ | /ku.tou/ | |
兔子 “rabbit” | 肚子 “belly” | 困了”sleepy” | 骨头 “bone” | ||
/thu.tou/ | /tun. tʂə/ | /khu. tsi/ | /kun. tsi/ | ||
土豆 “potato” | 蹲着 “squatting” | 裤子 “pants” | 棍子 “wooden stick” |
References
- Allen, G. D. (1985). How the young French child avoids the pre-voicing problem for word-initial voiced stops. Journal of Child Language, 12(1), 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bailey, P. J., & Haggard, M. P. (1980). Perception-production relations in the voicing contrast for initial stops in 3-year-olds. Phonetica, 37, 377–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckman, M. E., Benjamin, M., & Edwards, J. (2007). The influence of vocabulary growth on developmental changes in types of phonological knowledge. In J. Cole, & J. I. Hualde (Eds.), Laboratory phonology (Vol. 9, pp. 241–264). Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Bless, D. M., & Abbs, J. H. (1983). Vocal fold physiology: Contemporary research and clinical issues. College Hill Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2005). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.3.07). Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 1 May 2010).
- Bradlow, A. R., Torretta, G. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (1996). Intelligibility of normal speech I: Global and fine-grained acoustic-phonetic talker characteristics. Speech Communication, 20(3), 255–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brinca, L., Araújo, L., Nogueira, P., & Gil, C. (2016). Voice onset time characteristics of voiceless stops produced by children with European Portuguese as mother tongue. Ampersand, 3, 137–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byrd, D. (1994). Relations of sex and dialect to reduction. Speech Communication, 15(1–2), 39–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cao, Y. (1986). Gender difference in the phonetics of Beijing Mandarin (北京话语音里的性别差异). Chinese studies (汉语学习), 6, 31. [Google Scholar]
- Caramazza, A., & Yeni-Komshian, G. (1974). Voice onset time in two French dialects. Journal of Phonetics, 2, 239–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chao, K. Y., & Chen, L. (2008). A cross-linguistic study of voice onset time in stop consonant productions. Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing, 13(2), 215–232. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, L., Chao, K. Y., & Peng, J. F. (2007, September 6–7). VOT productions of word-initial stops in Mandarin and English: A cross-language study. Proceedings of the 19th Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing (pp. 303–317), Taipei, Taiwan. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, T., Jun, S.-A., & Ladefoged, P. (2002). Acoustic and aerodynamic correlates of Korean stops and fricatives. Journal of Phonetics, 30(2), 193–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, T., & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variations and universals in VOT: Evidence from 17 endangered languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 207–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cleveland, W. S., & Grosse, E. (1991). Computational methods for local regression. Statistics and Computing, 1(1), 47–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, K. (1995). Phonetic and phonological contrasts in the acquisition of voicing: Voice onset time production in Hindi and English. Journal of Child Language, 22(2), 275–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edwards, J., & Beckman, M. E. (2008). Methodological questions in studying phonological acquisition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 22(12), 939–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Benito-Garcia, C. R. (1984). The acquisition of voicing contrasts in Spanish and English learning infants and children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 11(2), 313–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51(2), 419–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gandour, J., Petty, S. H., Dardarananda, R., Dechongkit, S., & Mukngoen, S. (1986). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Thai: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 13(3), 561–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gilbert, J. H. V. (1977). A voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in 3-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 4(1), 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodell, E. W., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1993). Acoustic evidence for the development of gestural coordination in the speech of 2-year-olds: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(4), 707–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, J. R., Moore, C. A., Higashikawa, M., & Steeve, R. W. (2000). The physiologic development of speech motor control: Lip and jaw coordination. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(1), 239–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hazan, V., Romeo, R., & Pettinato, M. (2013). The impact of variation in phoneme category structure on consonant intelligibility. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics, 19, 060103. [Google Scholar]
- Hitchcock, E. R., & Koenig, L. L. (2013). The Effects of data reduction in determining the schedule of voicing acquisition in young children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(2), 441–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holliday, J. J., Reidy, P. F., Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2015). Quantifying the robustness of the English sibilant fricative contrast in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(3), 622–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, M. Y. (1991). Feminine accent in the Beijing vernacular: A sociolinguistic investigation. Journal of Chinese Language Teachers Association, 26(1), 49–54. [Google Scholar]
- Jakobson, R. (1960). Child Language, aphasia, and phonological universal. Mouton. (Original work published 1941). [Google Scholar]
- Jeannerod, M. (1988). The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements. Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Keating, P. A. (1990). Phonetic representations in a generative grammar. Journal of Phonetics, 18, 321–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keating, P. A., Linker, W., & Huffman, M. (1983). Patterns in allophone distribution for voiced and voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 277–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kent, R. D. (1976). Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic studies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19(3), 421–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kessinger, R. H., & Blumstein, S. E. (1997). Effects of speaking rate on voice onset time in Thai, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 143–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kewley-Port, D., & Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 2, 195–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kharlamov, V. (2022). Phonetic effects in the perception of VOT in a prevoicing language. Brain Sciences, 12(4), 427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenig, L. L. (2000). Laryngeal factors in voiceless consonant production in men, women, and 5-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(5), 1211–1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenig, L. L. (2001). Distributional characteristics of VOT in children’s voiceless aspirated stops and interpretation of developmental trends. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1058–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koenig, L. L., & Lucero, J. C. (2008). Stop consonant voicing and intraoral pressure contours in women and children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123, 1077–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kong, E. J., Beckman, M. E., & Edwards, J. (2012). Voice onset time is necessary but not always sufficient to describe acquisition of voiced stops: The cases of Greek and Japanese. Journal of Phonetics, 40(6), 725–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kwon, H.-B. (2010). Gender difference in speech intelligibility using speech intelligibility tests and acoustic analyses. The Journal of Advanced Prosthodontics, 2(3), 71–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Li, F. (2013). The effect of speakers’ sex on voice onset time in Mandarin stops. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133(2), EL142–EL147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, F. (2017). The development of gender-specific patterns in the production of voiceless sibilant fricatives in Mandarin Chinese. Linguistics, 55(5). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustic measurements. Word, 20, 384–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lowenstein, J. H., & Nittrouer, S. (2008). Patterns of acquisition of native voice onset time in English-learning children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(2), 1180–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, J., Chen, X., Wu, Y., & Zhang, L. (2017). Effects of age and sex on voice onset time: Evidence from Mandarin voiceless stops. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 19, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macken, M. A., & Barton, D. (1980). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7(1), 41–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacLeod, A. A. N. (2016). Phonetic and phonological perspectives on the acquisition of voice onset time by French-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 11(2), 1–15. [Google Scholar]
- MacLeod, A. A. N., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2009). The use of voice onset time by early bilinguals to distinguish homorganic stops in Canadian English and Canadian French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(1), 53–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, R. J., McCrea, C. R., & Herring, K. D. (2008). Voice onset time differences between adult males and females: Isolated syllables. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 308–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munson, B., Edwards, J., & Beckman, M. E. (2005). Phonological knowledge in typical and atypical speech-sound development. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(3), 190–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Newman, R. S., Clouse, S. A., & Burnham, J. L. (2001). The perceptual consequences of within-talker variability in fricative production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(3), 1181–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nittrouer, S. (1993). The emergence of mature gestural patterns is not uniform: Evidence from an acoustic study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(5), 959–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Okalidou, A., Petinou, K., Theodorou, E., & Karasimou, E. (2010). Development of voice onset time in standard-Greek and Cypriot-Greek-speaking preschoolers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 24(7), 503–519. [Google Scholar]
- Peng, J.-F., Chen, L.-M., & Lee, C.-C. (2014). Voice onset time of initial stops in Mandarin and Hakka: Effect of gender. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 12(1), 63–79. [Google Scholar]
- Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46(Pt 2–3), 115–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 July 2017).
- Repp, B. H. (1986). Some observations on the development of anticipatory coarticulation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 79(5), 1616–1619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, K., & Avery, P. (1995). Variability in a deterministic model of language acquisition: A theory of segmental elaboration. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Phonological acquisition and phonological theory (pp. 27–29). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Riney, T. J., Takagi, N., Ota, K., & Uchida, Y. (2007). The intermediate degree of VOT in Japanese initial voiceless stops. Journal of Phonetics, 35(3), 439–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robb, M., Gilbert, H., & Lerman, J. (2005). Influence of gender and environmental setting on voice onset time. Folia Phoniatrica Et Logopaedica, 57(3), 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rochet, B. L., & Fei, Y. (1991). Effect of consonant and vowel context on Mandarin Chinese VOT: Production and perception. Canadian Acoustics, 19(4), 105–106. [Google Scholar]
- Romeo, R., Hazan, V., & Pettinato, M. (2013). Developmental and gender-related trends of intra-talker variability in consonant production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(5), 3781–3792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ryalls, J., Zipprer, A., & Baldauff, P. A. (1997). Preliminary investigation of the effects of gender and race on voice onset time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 642–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simpson, A. P. (2009). Phonetic differences between male and female speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(2), 621–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, A. (2006). Speech motor development: Integrating muscles, movements, and linguistic units. Journal of Communication Disorders, 39(5), 331–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, A., & Goffman, L. (1998). Stability and patterning of speech movement sequences in children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(1), 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, A., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2004). Development of functional synergies for speech motor coordination in childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 45(1), 22–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31(1), 137–149. [Google Scholar]
- Stuart-Smith, J. (2007). Empirical evidence for gendered speech production: /s/ in Glaswegian. Laboratory Phonology, 9, 65–86. [Google Scholar]
- Swartz, B. L. (1992). Gender difference in voice onset time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 983–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Titze, I. R. (1989). Physiologic and acoustic differences between male and female voices. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85(4), 1699–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vennemann, T. (1971). Language acquisition and phonological theory. Lingusitics, 70, 71–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walsh, B., & Smith, A. (2002). Articulatory movements in adolescents: Evidence for protracted development of speech motor control processes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1119–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiteside, S. P., Dobbin, R., & Henry, L. (2003). Patterns of variability in voice onset time: A developmental study of motor speech skills in humans. Neuroscience letters, 347, 29–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whiteside, S. P., Henry, L., & Dobbin, R. (2004). Sex differences in voice onset time: A developmental study of phonetic context effects in British English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116(2), 1179–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whiteside, S. P., & Marshall, J. (2001). Developmental trends in voice onset time: Some evidence for sex differences. Phonetica, 58(3), 196–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, J. (2018). Development of stop consonants in three- to six-year old Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 45(5), 1091–1115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zlatin, M. A. (1974). Voicing contrast: Perceptual and productive voice onset time characteristics of adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 56(3), 981–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zlatin, M. A., & Koenigsknecht, R. A. (1976). Development of the voicing contrast: A comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 19(1), 93–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
2-Year-Olds (M = 30 Months; SD = 3 Months) | 3-Year-Olds (M = 42 Months; SD = 3 Months) | 4-Year-Olds (M = 54 Months; SD = 4 Months) | 5-Year-Olds (M = 66 Months; SD = 3 months) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Females | 14 | 13 | 12 | 12 |
Males | 10 | 15 | 11 | 13 |
Total | 24 | 28 | 23 | 25 |
Measure | Definition | Interpretation |
---|---|---|
%CP | The percentage of a child’s VOT production that can be correctly predicted by a mixed-effects logistic regression model. It is built on all children’s production available in a dataset. This measure can be calculated for each pair of stops that contrast in voicing (i.e., one measure for /t/-/d/ and one for /k/-/g/). | It indicates how much overlap exists between two contrastive voicing categories. |
Within-category dispersion | The mean of the standard deviation of the VOT distribution of voiceless unaspirated and voiceless aspirated stops. | It indicates, on average, how compact or diffuse the shape of VOT distribution of two contrastive voicing categories are. |
Between-categories distance | The difference between the means of the two VOT distributions for voiceless unaspirated and voiceless aspirated stops. | It indicates how close or distant the two categories are distributed along the VOT continuum. |
Discriminability score | The between-categories distance divided by the square root of the mean of the variances of the two VOT distributions. | It indicates how discriminable the two VOT distributions are. |
/t/ | /d/ | /k/ | /g/ | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
2-year-olds | Female | 127.2 (38.9) | 61.7 (35.3) | 112.6 (35.8) | 50.6 (32.1) |
Male | 109.3 (24.8) | 43.8 (22.0) | 108.2 (25.0) | 39.2 (23.9) | |
All | 119.8 (34.3) | 54.3 (31.3) | 110.8 (31.2) | 45.9 (28.9) | |
3-year-olds | Female | 119.7 (16.2) | 24.4 (10.1) | 116.1 (22.9) | 28.7 (10.1) |
Male | 128.8 (27.2) | 30.0 (14.0) | 122.4 (19.8) | 34.2 (16.1) | |
All | 124.6 (22.8) | 27.4 (12.4) | 119.5 (21.1) | 31.6 (13.8) | |
4-year-olds | Female | 121.0 (27.8) | 23.4 (22.3) | 123.4 (23.1) | 23.6 (11.4) |
Male | 105.4 (19.4) | 19.6 (9.9) | 115.6 (21.8) | 21.8 (10.7) | |
All | 113.5 (26.7) | 21.6 (17.3) | 119.7 (22.3) | 22.7 (10.8) | |
5-year-olds | Female | 100.3 (12.6) | 19.0 (9.3) | 107.6 (19.4) | 21.6 (8.9) |
Male | 117.4 (11.3) | 22.5 (10.4) | 106.0 (17.1) | 32.6 (21.8) | |
All | 109.2 (22.0) | 20.8 (9.8) | 106.8 (17.9) | 27.3 (17.5) | |
Adults | Female | 94.0 (12.6) | 12.4 (2.4) | 90.6 (11.3) | 22.5 (3.2) |
Male | 77.9 (11.3) | 19.1 (6.8) | 79.2 (9.7) | 31.5 (10.7) | |
All | 85.7 (14.1) | 15.8 (6.1) | 84.9 (11.8) | 27.0 (9.0) |
Robustness of Phonological Contrast Measures | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
%CP (Percentage of Tokens Correctly Predicted by Mixed Effects Models) | Within-Category Dispersion (ms) | Between-Categories Distance (ms) | Discriminability Score | ||
/t/-/d/ contrast | 2-year-olds | 0.79 (0.41) | 44.68 (21.64) | 65.47 (33.86) | 1.60 (0.98) |
3-year-olds | 0.94 (0.24) | 32.19 (13.58) | 97.14 (25.44) | 3.32 (1.71) | |
4-year-olds | 0.92 (0.23) | 26.29 (14.35) | 91.93 (22.66) | 3.78 (1.93) | |
5-year-olds | 0.97 (0.18) | 21.14 (10.44) | 88.36 (17.72) | 4.57 (2.44) | |
Adults | 0.99 (0.11) | 10.10 (4.60) | 69.93 (16.75) | 8.24 (6.66) | |
/k/-/g/ contrast | 2-year-olds | 0.80 (0.40) | 37.21 (22.36) | 64.90 (34.70) | 2.10 (1.43) |
3-year-olds | 0.92 (0.28) | 27.82 (11.53) | 87.85 (23.08) | 3.45 (1.55) | |
4-year-olds | 0.96 (0.19) | 22.54 (10.35) | 96.92 (18.68) | 4.56 (2.25) | |
5-year-olds | 0.96 (0.21) | 21.57 (8.73) | 79.42 (16.83) | 3.95 (1.79) | |
Adults | 0.98 (0.15) | 10.98 (4.35) | 57.89 (15.17) | 5.77 (2.92) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Li, F. Speech Production Development in Mandarin-Speaking Children: A Case of Lingual Stop Consonants. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 516. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040516
Li F. Speech Production Development in Mandarin-Speaking Children: A Case of Lingual Stop Consonants. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):516. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040516
Chicago/Turabian StyleLi, Fangfang. 2025. "Speech Production Development in Mandarin-Speaking Children: A Case of Lingual Stop Consonants" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 516. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040516
APA StyleLi, F. (2025). Speech Production Development in Mandarin-Speaking Children: A Case of Lingual Stop Consonants. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 516. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040516