The Role of Answer Justification in Multiple-Choice Testing: Effects on Performance and Metacognitive Accuracy
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Improving Multiple-Choice Questions with Answer Justification
1.2. The Role of Generative Learning in Answer Justification
1.3. The Role of Metacognition in Answer Justification
1.4. Present Research
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Sample
2.1.2. Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Test Performance
2.2.2. Metacognitive Accuracy
2.2.3. Familiarity Ratings and Self-Reported Answer Changing
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Sample
3.1.2. Materials
3.1.3. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Test Performance
3.2.2. Time on Test
3.2.3. Metacognitive Accuracy
3.2.4. Familiarity Ratings and Self-Reported Answer Changing
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
4.1. Metacognitive Implications
4.2. Alternative Explanations
4.3. Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
MOOC | Massive open online courses |
GPM | Gains per minute |
MC | Multiple-choice |
References
- Bassett, M. H. (2016). Teaching critical thinking without (much) writing: Multiple-choice and metacognition. Teaching Theology & Religion, 19(1), 20–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisra, K., Liu, Q., Nesbit, J. C., Salimi, F., & Winne, P. H. (2018). Inducing self-explanation: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 703–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, G. T., & Abdulnabi, H. H. (2017). Evaluating the quality of higher education instructor-constructed multiple-choice tests: Impact on student grades. Frontiers in Education, 2, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, S. K., Pan, S. C., & Butler, A. C. (2022). The science of effective learning with spacing and retrieval practice. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(9), 496–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, J. C. K., Davis, S. D., Yurtsever, A., & Myers, S. J. (2024). The magnitude of the testing effect is independent of retrieval practice performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 153(7), 1816–1837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chi, M. T., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13(2), 145–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crouch, C. H., & Mazur, E. (2001). Peer instruction: Ten years of experience and results. American Journal of Physics, 69(9), 970–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dodd, D. K., & Leal, L. (1988). Answer justification: Removing the “trick” from multiple-choice questions. Teaching of Psychology, 15(1), 37–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, J., Mueller, M. L., & Thiede, K. W. (2016). Methodology for investigating human metamemory: Problems and pitfalls. In J. Dunlosky, & S. K. Tauber (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Metamemory (pp. 23–38). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22(4), 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Earle, S. (2019). Physical geology (2nd ed.). BCcampus. Available online: https://opentextbc.ca/physicalgeology2ed (accessed on 1 March 2023).
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 717–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fukaya, T. (2013). Explanation generation, not explanation expectancy, improves metacomprehension accuracy. Metacognition and Learning, 8(1), 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gorrell, J., & Cramond, B. (1988). Students’ attitudes toward and use of written justifications for multiple-choice answers. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48(4), 935–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greving, S., & Richter, T. (2022). Practicing retrieval in university teaching: Short-answer questions are beneficial, whereas multiple-choice questions are not. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 34(5), 657–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, T. D., Mielicki, M. K., & Wiley, J. (2019a). Improving students’ metacomprehension accuracy. In J. Dunlosky, & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 619–646). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2019b). The effects of comprehension-test expectancies on metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(6), 1066–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Individual differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 36, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koretsky, M. D., Brooks, J. D., & Higgins, A. D. (2016). Written justifications to multiple-choice concept questions during active learning in class. International Journal of Science Education, 38(11), 1747–1765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kottke, J. L. (2001). Students’ reactions to written test item rebuttals. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 28(4), 256–261. [Google Scholar]
- Little, J. L., & Bjork, E. L. (2015). Optimizing multiple-choice tests as tools for learning. Memory & Cognition, 43(1), 14–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Little, J. L., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Angello, G. (2012). Multiple-choice tests exonerated, at least of some charges: Fostering test-induced learning and avoiding test-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1337–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Little, J. L., Frickey, E. A., & Fung, A. K. (2019). The role of retrieval in answering multiple-choice questions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(8), 1473–1485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, V. L., & Pressley, M. (1991). Elaborative-interrogation effects depend on the nature of the question. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(1), 113–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDaniel, M. A. (2023). Combining retrieval practice with elaborative encoding: Complementary or redundant? Educational Psychology Review, 35(3), 75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDaniel, M. A., & Donnelly, C. M. (1996). Learning with analogy and elaborative interrogation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 508–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKeachie, W. J., Pollie, D., & Speisman, J. (1955). Relieving anxiety in classroom examinations. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50(1), 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 125–173). Academic Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nield, A. F., & Wintre, M. G. (1986). Multiple choice questions with an option to comment. Teaching of Psychology, 13(4), 196–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overono, A. L. (in press). Exploring the impact of required justifications in multiple-choice elaboration questions on student experiences and performance. Journal of Scholarship of Teaching and Learning. [Google Scholar]
- Ozgungor, S., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Interactions among elaborative interrogation, knowledge, and interest in the process of constructing knowledge from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 437–443. [Google Scholar]
- Ozuru, Y., Briner, S., Kurby, C. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2013). Comparing comprehension measured by multiple-choice and open-ended questions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67(3), 215–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2018). Transfer of test-enhanced learning: Meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(7), 710–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papadopoulos, P. M., Obwegeser, N., & Weinberger, A. (2022). Let me explain! The effects of writing and reading short justifications on students’ performance. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 38(2), 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierce, B. H., & Smith, S. M. (2001). The postdiction superiority effect in metacomprehension of text. Memory & Cognition, 29, 62–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Putnam, A. L. (2015). Mnemonics in education: Current research and applications. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 1(2), 130–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Relearning attenuates the benefits and costs of spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1113–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Renkl, A., & Eitel, A. (2019). Self-explaining: Learning about principles and their application. In J. Dunlosky, & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 528–549). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivers, M. L., Dunlosky, J., & Joynes, R. (2019). The contribution of classroom exams to formative evaluation of concept-level knowledge. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 101806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roediger, H. L., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 20–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1432–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, G. (2009). A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring. Metacognition and Learning, 4, 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, M. A., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Retrieval practice with short-answer, multiple-choice, and hybrid tests. Memory, 22(7), 784–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sparck, E. M., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2016). On the learning benefits of confidence-weighted testing. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 1, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tajika, H., Nakatsu, N., Nozaki, H., Neumann, E., & Maruno, S. (2007). Effects of self-explanation as a metacognitive strategy for solving mathematical word problems. Japanese Psychological Research, 49(3), 222–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2011). Test expectancy affects metacomprehension accuracy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 264–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tullis, J. G., & Goldstone, R. L. (2020). Why does peer instruction benefit student learning? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 5, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiley, J., Griffin, T. D., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). To understand your understanding, one must understand what understanding means. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 30. Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4xb0r6jw (accessed on 10 February 2025).
- Witherby, A. E., & Carpenter, S. K. (2022). The rich-get-richer effect: Prior knowledge predicts new learning of domain-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(4), 483–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Topic-Level Judgments | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Global Judgments | Defining Geology | Minerals and Rocks | Plate Tectonics | Geologic Time | |
MC-Only Group | |||||
Predictive Judgments | 64 (3) | 77 (4) | 62 (4) | 56 (4) | 45 (4) |
Performance | 55 (16) | 68 (5) | 47 (5) | 63 (3) | 35 (5) |
Postdictive Judgments | 54 (4) | 62 (5) | 52 (5) | 50 (5) | 50 (4) |
Answer Justification Group | |||||
Predictive Judgments | 59 (4) | 72 (4) | 60 (4) | 53 (4) | 52 (4) |
Performance | 64 (18) | 77 (5) | 57 (5) | 72 (3) | 42 (5) |
Postdictive Judgments | 61 (4) | 71 (5) | 60 (5) | 55 (4) | 56 (4) |
Never | Sometimes | About Half the Time | Most of the Time | Always | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Experiment 1 | 28% | 54% | 10% | 8% | 0% |
Experiment 2 | 24% | 53% | 16% | 6% | 2% |
Topic-Level Judgments | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Global Judgments | Defining Geology | Minerals and Rocks | Plate Tectonics | Geologic Time | |
MC-Only Group | |||||
Predictive Judgments | 7.60 (0.19) | 1.79 (0.04) | 2.02 (0.06) | 2.66 (0.10) | 1.23 (0.06) |
Immediate Test Performance | 7.40 (0.18) | 1.60 (0.05) | 1.61 (0.08) | 3.25 (0.10) | 0.94 (0.07) |
Postdictive Judgments | 6.64 (0.20) | 1.70 (0.04) | 1.69 (0.07) | 2.27 (0.11) | 1.06 (0.06) |
Answer Justification Group | |||||
Predictive Judgments | 7.59 (0.20) | 1.83 (0.04) | 1.98 (0.07) | 2.66 (0.10) | 1.29 (0.06) |
Immediate Test Performance | 8.04 (0.20) | 1.70 (0.05) | 1.87 (0.08) | 3.56 (0.11) | 0.92 (0.07) |
Postdictive Judgments | 7.38 (0.23) | 1.80 (0.06) | 1.96 (0.08) | 2.59 (0.12) | 1.10 (0.06) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Clark, S.A.; Rivers, M.L.; Overono, A.L. The Role of Answer Justification in Multiple-Choice Testing: Effects on Performance and Metacognitive Accuracy. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 477. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040477
Clark SA, Rivers ML, Overono AL. The Role of Answer Justification in Multiple-Choice Testing: Effects on Performance and Metacognitive Accuracy. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):477. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040477
Chicago/Turabian StyleClark, Spenser A., Michelle L. Rivers, and Acacia L. Overono. 2025. "The Role of Answer Justification in Multiple-Choice Testing: Effects on Performance and Metacognitive Accuracy" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 477. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040477
APA StyleClark, S. A., Rivers, M. L., & Overono, A. L. (2025). The Role of Answer Justification in Multiple-Choice Testing: Effects on Performance and Metacognitive Accuracy. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 477. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040477